Teaching Stimulus Control Via Class-Wide Multiple Schedules Of

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Teaching Stimulus Control Via Class-Wide Multiple Schedules Of PBIXXX10.1177/1098300716632878Journal of Positive Behavior InterventionsTorelli et al. 632878research-article2016 Empirical Research Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions 2017, Vol. 19(1) 14 –25 Teaching Stimulus Control © Hammill Institute on Disabilities 2016 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav via Class-Wide Multiple Schedules DOI: 10.1177/1098300716632878 of Reinforcement in Public jpbi.sagepub.com Elementary School Classrooms Jessica N. Torelli, MEd1, Blair P. Lloyd, PhD1, Claire A. Diekman, BA1, and Joseph H. Wehby, PhD1 Abstract In elementary school classrooms, students commonly recruit teacher attention at inappropriately high rates or at inappropriate times. Multiple schedule interventions have been used to teach stimulus control by signaling to students when reinforcement is and is not available contingent on an appropriate response. The purpose of the current study was to evaluate the effects of a class-wide multiple schedule on differentiated rates of student recruitment of teacher attention in two public elementary classrooms. General education teachers implemented the multiple schedule intervention in the context of a common instructional routine (i.e., small group rotations among reading centers). Results indicated that the multiple schedule intervention was effective at decreasing disruptive bids for attention when teacher attention was not available. Additional research on teacher implementation of class-wide multiple schedules is needed to evaluate whether this intervention may be identified as an effective supplement to Tier 1 classroom management strategies within multi- tiered systems of support. Keywords stimulus control, multiple schedule, class-wide, Tier 1 intervention Teaching students appropriate ways to recruit teacher atten- (SΔ) signal the unavailability of reinforcement contingent tion has been shown to increase levels of teacher praise and on a response. Multiple schedules have been used to thin instructional assistance, as well as positively impact student dense schedules of reinforcement following the acquisition performance in the classroom (Alber & Heward, 2000). of a target behavior. Such arrangements typically involve However, once students learn how to appropriately recruit preserving a dense response–reinforcer contingency in one teacher attention, these behaviors can occur at inappropriately schedule component while introducing and systematically high rates or at inappropriate times (Cammilleri, Tiger, & extending extinction in another schedule component. Hanley, 2008; Sidener, Shabani, Carr, & Roland, 2006; Relative to other methods of schedule thinning, multiple Stokes, Fowler, & Baer, 1978). Because teacher attention is schedules have been found to be more effective at facilitat- rarely continuously available to all students, it is critical not ing stimulus control while maintaining low levels of inap- only to teach students how to recruit attention but when to propriate behavior (Hanley, Iwata, & Thompson, 2001). recruit attention, particularly in classrooms with high student– To address inappropriately high rates of attention recruit- teacher ratios. ment, multiple schedule interventions have been applied in In the applied behavior analytic literature, multiple simulated classroom settings with preschool-age students schedules have been used to teach children to discriminate (e.g., Tiger & Hanley, 2004, 2005; Tiger, Hanley, & Heal, when adult attention is available contingent on appropriate 2006). Tiger and colleagues alternated between an extinc- attention-seeking behaviors. A multiple schedule is a com- tion component during one stimulus (SΔ) and a dense pound schedule of reinforcement in which two or more component schedules alternate (usually according to time), 1Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA and each component schedule is correlated with a different Corresponding Author: stimulus (Catania, 2013; Ferster & Skinner, 1957). Blair P. Lloyd, Department of Special Education, Peabody College, D Discriminative stimuli (S ) signal the availability of rein- Vanderbilt University, Box 228, Nashville, TN 37203, USA. forcement contingent on a response, whereas delta stimuli Email: [email protected] Torelli et al. 15 schedule of reinforcement (e.g., fixed ratio 1 [FR1]) during class-wide by teachers, this intervention may be identified a different stimulus (SD). Each stimulus signaled whether as an effective supplement to other Tier 1 classroom man- reinforcement (i.e., teacher attention) was available contin- agement strategies. gent on appropriate bids for attention (e.g., hand-raising or To our knowledge, only two studies have trained class- calling the teacher’s name) and verbal reminders of each room teachers to implement multiple schedules on a class- contingency were provided to students prior to sessions. wide basis. Cammilleri et al. (2008) trained private school Such multiple schedule arrangements have effectively pro- teachers to implement a class-wide multiple schedule and duced discriminated responding when applied by experi- evaluated intervention effects on student social approaches menters individually and with pairs of students in small via a multiple baseline design across three private elemen- instructional rooms intended to simulate classroom settings tary school classrooms. Each classroom was comprised of (Tiger & Hanley, 2004, 2005; Tiger et al., 2006). 10 to 12 students ranging in age and skill level (kindergar- Fewer studies, however, have evaluated teacher imple- ten to sixth grade), with each student engaged in self-paced mentation of multiple schedule interventions on a class- individualized curricula. Teachers conducted 5-min ses- wide level in typical classrooms. Teaching students to sions alternating between SD (green lei signaling teacher discriminate when teacher attention is and is not available attention was available) and SΔ (red lei signaling teacher may be especially relevant in general education classrooms attention was unavailable). Teachers implemented two to because attention is seldom continuously available for any four sessions per day and described the contingency to the single student. In addition, implementation of multiple class before beginning each session. Results revealed that schedules (i.e., pairing visual stimuli with the availability of the class-wide multiple schedule produced discriminated reinforcement) requires relatively low response effort for responding such that bids for attention decreased during SΔ, teachers. As general education teachers are increasingly and maintained during SD. expected to assume greater responsibility in the implemen- Vargo, Heal, Epperley, and Kooistra (2014) evaluated tation of academic and behavioral interventions (Franklin, the effects of a class-wide multiple schedule intervention Kim, Ryan, Kelly, & Montgomery, 2012; Horner et al., (with verbal descriptions of contingencies) on hand-raising 2009; Simonsen et al., 2010), the identification of behavior via a multiple baseline design across three preschool class- support strategies that can be implemented class-wide with rooms. Each classroom included 18 to 19 students and the relative ease and efficiency is critical. Thus, multiple sched- multiple schedule intervention was implemented in the con- ules may offer an efficient strategy to address a common text of circle time routines. Teachers alternated between problem in general education classrooms. signaling the availability of reinforcement for hand raises in The identification of effective class-wide behavior sup- the presence of a blue card (SD) and the unavailability of port strategies is also critical for schools adopting multi- reinforcement in the presence of a yellow card (SΔ). Results tiered systems of support (e.g., Positive Behavioral demonstrated that hand-raising decreased in the presence of Interventions and Supports [PBIS]; Horner et al., 2009; SΔ relative to SD across all three classrooms. In addition, Sugai & Horner, 2006). Class-wide strategies are designed responding in the SΔ condition decreased overall during the to impact the greatest proportion of students and prevent multiple schedules plus rules component relative to base- more serious behavior problems from emerging (Horner & line across all classrooms. Sugai, 2015; Sugai et al., 2000). Multiple schedule inter- Because the classrooms in the study by Cammilleri and ventions may provide an avenue to strengthen critical com- colleagues (2008) had relatively low student-to-teacher ponents of a universal classroom management plan, ratios, were comprised of students who ranged in age and including maximizing structure and predictability, acknowl- skill level, and involved a unique instructional format (i.e., edging appropriate behavior, and responding consistently to self-paced individualized instruction), it is unknown inappropriate behavior (e.g., systematically withholding whether similar class-wide applications of multiple sched- attention; Simonsen, Fairbanks, Briesch, Myers, & Sugai, ules would be effective in the context of typical public ele- 2008). Specifically, the presentation of visual stimuli to sig- mentary classrooms. Results of the study by Vargo and nal the availability of teacher attention may help clarify colleagues (2014) suggest that multiple schedule interven- expectations and maximize structure and predictability dur- tions can be effectively implemented in the context of com- ing instructional routines. In addition, reinforcing student mon instructional
Recommended publications
  • A Comparison of Stimulus Control Procedures and Progressive Relaxation in the Treatment of Insomnia Mark L
    Eastern Illinois University The Keep Masters Theses Student Theses & Publications 1979 A Comparison of Stimulus Control Procedures and Progressive Relaxation in the Treatment of Insomnia Mark L. Balen Eastern Illinois University This research is a product of the graduate program in Psychology at Eastern Illinois University. Find out more about the program. Recommended Citation Balen, Mark L., "A Comparison of Stimulus Control Procedures and Progressive Relaxation in the Treatment of Insomnia" (1979). Masters Theses. 3170. https://thekeep.eiu.edu/theses/3170 This is brought to you for free and open access by the Student Theses & Publications at The Keep. It has been accepted for inclusion in Masters Theses by an authorized administrator of The Keep. For more information, please contact [email protected]. PAPJ<:R CERTIFICATE #2 TO: Graduate Degree Candidates who have written formal theses. SUBJECT: Permission to reproduce theses. The University Library is receiving a number of requests from other institutions asking permission to reproduce dissertations for inclusion in their library holdings. Although no copyright laws are involved, we feel that professional courtesy demands that permission be obtained from the author before we allow theses to be copied. Please sign one of the following statements: Bo(!th Library of Eastern Illinois University has my permission to lend my thesis to a reputable college or university for the purpose of copying it for inclusion in that institution's library or research holdings. Date Author I respectfully request Booth Library of Eastern Illinois University not allow my thesis be reproduced because Date Author pdm A Comparison of Stimu1us ControlProcedures and Progressive Relaxation in the Treatment of Insomnia (TITLE) BY Mark L.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for Insomnia
    Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for Insomnia Melanie K. Leggett, PhD, CBSM Duke University Medical Center Disclosures ▪ I have no relevant financial relationship with the manufacturers of any commercial products and/or providers of commercial services discussed in this CME activity. ▪ Neither I or any member of my immediate family has a financial relationship or interest with any proprietary entity producing health care goods or services related to the content of this CME activity. ▪ My content may include reference to commercial products; however, generic and alternative products will be discussed whenever possible. I do not intend to discuss any unapproved or investigative use of commercial products or devices. ▪ This presentation does not represent the views of the Department of Veterans Affairs or the United States Government. 2 Overview ▪ Development and model of insomnia ▪ CBTI components ▪ CBTI therapy ▪ Outcomes ▪ Dissemination 3 Objectives ▪ Participants will understand the conceptual model of insomnia. ▪ Participants will be able to define CBTI, describe the treatment components, and understand the therapeutic rationale. ▪ Participants will understand the indications and contraindications for CBTI and be able to describe treatment efficacy. 4 Development of Insomnia 5 Definition of Insomnia ▪ Insomnia characterized by: ▫ Trouble falling asleep ▫ Trouble staying asleep ▫ Early morning awakenings ▪ Distress/impairment in daytime functioning ▪ Adequate opportunity & circumstances for sleep ▪ At least 3x/week for 3 months ▪ Not better explained by another sleep disorder 6 Sleep Processes Homeostatic System Circadian System Sleep Arousal System 7 Spielman’s Conceptual Model of Insomnia 8 CBTI Components 9 What is CBTI? ▪ Multicomponent regimen 1. Sleep restriction 2. Stimulus control 3. Cognitive therapy 4. Sleep hygiene 5.
    [Show full text]
  • Creating Stimulus Control 1
    Running head: CREATING STIMULUS CONTROL 1 Creating Stimulus Control In a Classroom for Children with Autism Emily Levy California State University, Fresno Emily This is wonderful, best in the class, doesn’t need editing….very very good. CREATING STIMULUS CONTROL 2 CREATING STIMULUS CONTROL 3 Abstract Human behavior, throughout the day, is guided by certain stimuli that signal a person what response or action is required. The term “stimulus control” is used to describe the situation when a behavior occurs in the presence of some stimuli and not others (Cooper, Heron, & Heward, 2006). Stimulus control can be created through differential reinforcement. It is commonly used with children with autism during discrete trial lessons. Stimulus control can be used in a classroom environment to increase the on-task behavior of the students. Keywords: stimulus control, differential reinforcement, classroom, autism CREATING STIMULUS CONTROL 4 Creating Stimulus Control: In a Classroom for Children with Autism It is estimated that one in 110 children in the United States have autism spectrum disorder (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2011). Autism is a developmental disability that affects most children by the age of three years (American Psychiatric Association [DSM-IV-TR], 2000). There is no known cause for autism. The three main characteristics for a diagnosis of autism are deficits in language, deficits in social interactions, and restrictive, repetitive behavior. The field of Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA) implements a behavioral model that is acknowledged as an empirically validated treatment for children with autism (Schreibman, 2000). This behavioral model composes about two-thirds of the established, effective treatments for children with autism as found in the National Standards Project by the National Autism Center (2009).
    [Show full text]
  • Behavior Modification: Principles and Procedures, 4Th
    Your students can perform “classic” experiments that closely resemble those discussed in Miltenberger’s text Sniffy™ the Virtual Rat Lite, Version 2.0, Second Edition By Tom Alloway, Greg Wilson, and Jeff Graham Order the text packaged with Sniffy Lite and save your students money! Use: 0-495-42745-4 Students realize all of the benefi ts of using a real rat with none of the drawbacks as they master the concepts of classical and operant conditioning by training Sniffy Bring the theories of learning to life using Sniffy the Virtual Rat. Instructors across the country have praised Sniffy, a realistic digital rat in a Skinner Box, for his ability to give students hands- on experience in setting up and conducting experiments that demonstrate the phenomena of classical and operant conditioning. Users begin by training Sniffy to press a bar to obtain food. Then, they progress to studies of more complex learning phenomena. Throughout each study, a series of “Mind Windows” enables students to visualize how Sniffy’s experiences in the chamber produce the psychological changes that they’re learning in your course. Available on a cross-platform CD-ROM, the Lite version of Sniffy includes 16 exercises that cover the essential phenomena of learning psychology. Students grow to love Sniffy as he helps them learn to: • Simulate classical conditioning phenomena including acquisition, extinction, and spontaneous recovery. • Simulate operant conditioning phenomena including magazine training; shaping; extinction; primary and secondary reinforcement; variable-interval, variable-ratio, fi xed-interval, and fi xed-ratio schedule effects; and the partial-reinforcement effect. • Measure responses employed by research psychologists in a data form that can be printed and turned in for class assignments.
    [Show full text]
  • Introduction to Behavior Therapy Terminology
    Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU Master's Theses Graduate College 8-1973 Introduction to Behavior Therapy Terminology Alexandre Pacheo e Silva Nucci Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.wmich.edu/masters_theses Part of the Psychoanalysis and Psychotherapy Commons Recommended Citation Nucci, Alexandre Pacheo e Silva, "Introduction to Behavior Therapy Terminology" (1973). Master's Theses. 2670. https://scholarworks.wmich.edu/masters_theses/2670 This Masters Thesis-Open Access is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate College at ScholarWorks at WMU. It has been accepted for inclusion in Master's Theses by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks at WMU. For more information, please contact [email protected]. INTRODUCTION TO BEHAVIOR THERAPY TERMINOLOGY fcy Alexandre Pacheco e Silva Nucci A Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of The Graduate College in partial fulfillment of the Degree of Master of Arts Western Michigan University Kalamazoo, Michigan August 1973 Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. ACKNOWLEDGEMENT S First I wish to express my appreciation to Dr. Fred S. Keller who was the first person to consider my idea of writing this paper and who encouraged me in proceeding with it. Appreciation is also extended to Dr. Wade Hitzing, thesis advisor, and the other two members of this thesis committee, Dr. David Lyon and Dp, Paul Mount joy for their assistance to this project. Considering my staying in the United States of America as a foreign student, I want to express my full gratitude to Mr. and Mrs. B. E. Bensinger who accepted me in their home as a member of their own family.
    [Show full text]
  • Transfer of Stimulus Control Across Instrumental Responses Is Attenuated by Extinction in Human Instru- Mental Conditioning
    International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy 2005, Vol. 5, Nº 3, pp. 207-222 Transfer of Stimulus Control across Instrumental Responses is attenuated by Extinction in Human Instru- mental Conditioning A. Matías Gámez and Juan M. Rosas* Universidad de Jaén, España ABSTRACT D Transfer of behavioral control by a discriminative stimulus (S ) between different instru- mental responses trained with the same outcome has been consistently observed in nonhuman animals, regardless of whether the discriminative stimulus and the instrumental response have undergone extinction. Based on this result, it has been proposed that extinction of D nonhuman instrumental learning does not affect S -outcome associations. Two experiments explored whether this was the case in human instrumental conditioning. Both experiments used a video-game task where participants pressed different colored keys to defend Andalusia from the attack of ships and planes. Key-pressing was reinforced with the destruction of the ship or plane. In Experiment 1 removing the response-contingent outcome extinguished the instrumental response, while responding to a non-extinguished response remained high. D As in the animal literature, Experiment 2 found positive transfer of an S between different D responses that produce the same outcome, suggesting the formation of a S -outcome association in human instrumental conditioning. Contrary to what has been found with nonhuman D animals, extinction of the response in the presence of the S eliminated the transfer effect, D suggesting that the S -outcome association is deteriorated in human instrumental extinction. Key-words: Human instrumental conditioning; extinction, contents of learning, transfer test. RESUMEN La transferencia en el control de estímulos en la respuesta instrumental es atenuada por la extinción en el condicionamiento instrumental humano.
    [Show full text]
  • Paradoxical Intention Therapy Colin A
    Chapter 6 Paradoxical Intention Therapy Colin A. Espie University of Glasgow Sleep Centre, Sackler Institute of Psychobiological Research, University of Glasgow, Scotland, UK PROTOCOL NAME Paradoxical Intention (PI) therapy. GROSS INDICATION Paradoxical intention is thought to be ideal for insomnia disorder, particu- larly where there is intense preoccupation about sleep, sleep loss, and its con- sequences. The psychophysiological insomnia phenotype, as characterized in ICSD-2, couples sleep preoccupation with the notion of “striving” to sleep, and the maladaptive relationship between effort to sleep and ability to sleep [1]. SPECIFIC INDICATION There is no evidence to suggestion that Paradoxical Intention (PI) therapy is differentially effective in the sleep onset and sleep maintenance insomnia sub- types. The rationale for PI therapy, however, seems particularly appropriate for the psychophysiological insomnia phenotype (as stated above). There are two studies that suggest that reduction of sleep-related anxiety and performance effort may be the mechanism by which PI has its effect [2,3], but there are no formal clinical trials that demonstrate greater efficacy of PI in those insomnia patients who exhibit greater sleep effort. CONTRAINDICATIONS Two related notes of caution when considering PI therapy. First, PI is not suitable for patients who adopt a very concrete approach [4], or who are cognitively impaired. PI instructions (such as “remain awake for as long as possible”) are not literal commands to be implemented in an unthinking, mechanistic way. Rather, PI presents a cognitively challenging perspective on insomnia. It is important that patients understand that PI is a vehicle for a change in outlook as well as a change in behavior.
    [Show full text]
  • Hypnotic Relaxation and Insomnia: a Simple Solution? Harry E
    Hypnotic Relaxation and Insomnia: A Simple Solution? Harry E. Stanton, Ph.D. Objective: After a brief review of the use of behavioral strategies in the treatment of sleep onset insomnia, attention is centered upon one such strategy, hypnotic relaxation. Methods: A specific technique embracing visualization of a garden scene; letting go of problems; and a special place visualization, is described and its application to the problem of insomnia illustrated by means of three case studies. Results: Each of these studies deals with a different type of insomnia: 1) slow sleep onset, 2) waking during the night, and 3) difficulty in sleeping during the day. Conclusions: The success of the technique with all three cases suggests it might well be used more widely in the treatment of insomnia. (Sleep and Hypnosis 1999;1:64-67) Key words: hypnosis, relaxation, insomnia, therapy INTRODUCTION relaxation, stimulus control, paradoxical intention, and a placebo treatment on mild, moderate, and severe ue to uneasiness over the serious negative side effects insomniacs. Their findings demonstrated that behavioral D of sleep medications, considerable research effort has treatment approaches, particularly stimulus control, were been directed into the search for alternative, nondrug effective in reducing sleep onset insomnia, irrespective of interventions capable of alleviating insomnia. Behavioral severity level. With non-drug interventions proving to be strategies such as progressive relaxation, cognitive successful in treating insomnia, it is not suprising that the relaxation, biofeedback, stimulus control, and paradoxical effect of hypnotic relaxation methods has come under intention are one type of intervention that has produced investigation (9,10). In the latter study, progressive promising results (1-3).
    [Show full text]
  • Behavioral Humanism. TNSTTTUTION Stanford Univ., Calif
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 064 253 95 SP 005 797 AUTHOR Thoresen, Carl E. TITLE Behavioral Humanism. TNSTTTUTION stanford Univ., Calif. Stanford Center for Research and Development in Teaching. SPONS AGENCY Office of Education (DHEW), Washington, D.C. REPORT NO ROM-88 BUREAU NO BR-5-0252 PUB DATE Apr 72 CONTRACT OW-6-10-078 NOTE 35p. EDRS PRICE MF-$0.6F HC-$3.29 DESCRIPTORS *Behavioral Sciences; A4Behavior Theories; *Individual Development; *Psychology; *Self Control ABSTRACT Behavioral humanism is defined as the synthesis of behav4.oral techniques with humanistic goals. Contemporary humanism, especially humanistic psychology, offers directions for the kind of behavior that individuals should be able to engage in; contemporary behaviorism offers principles and procedures to help individuals increase their humanistic actions. The intensive experimental study of the individual (N-1)is discussed as an intimate research strategy appropriate to humanistic concerns. Freedom is viewed as the power to control the variables that infillence one's behavior. Behavioral self-control by means of self-observation, individual programming, and environmental planning is offered as a means of developing humanistic behavior. An initial translation of humanistic concerns into action (response) is nffered. The purpose of the research reported here is to create and empirically validate techniques to help individuals develop self-control skills. An extensive bibliography is i.ncluded. (Author) U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH, STANFORD CENTER EDUCATION & WELFARE FOR RESEARCH AND DEVELOPMENT OFFICE OF EDUCATION THIS DOCUMENT HAS BEEN REPRO- IN TEACHING DUCED EXACTLY AS RECEIVED FROM THE PERSON OR ORGANIZATION ORIG- INATING IT POINTS OF VIEW OR OPIN- IONS STATED DO NOT NECESSARILY REPRESENT OFFICIAL OFFICE OF EDU- CATION POSITION OR POLICY.
    [Show full text]
  • Relapse Prevention
    Relapse Prevention An Overview of Marlatt’s Cognitive-Behavioral Model Mary E. Larimer, Ph.D., Rebekka S. Palmer, and G. Alan Marlatt, Ph.D. Relapse prevention (RP) is an important component of alcoholism treatment. The RP model proposed by Marlatt and Gordon suggests that both immediate determinants (e.g., high-risk situations, coping skills, outcome expectancies, and the abstinence violation effect) and covert antecedents (e.g., lifestyle factors and urges and cravings) can contribute to relapse. The RP model also incorporates numerous specific and global intervention strategies that allow therapist and client to address each step of the relapse process. Specific interventions include identifying specific high-risk situations for each client and enhancing the client’s skills for coping with those situations, increasing the client’s self-efficacy, eliminating myths regarding alcohol’s effects, managing lapses, and restructuring the client’s perceptions of the relapse process. Global strategies comprise balancing the client’s lifestyle and helping him or her develop positive addictions, employing stimulus control techniques and urge- management techniques, and developing relapse road maps. Several studies have provided theoretical and practical support for the RP model. KEY WORDS:AODD (alcohol and other drug dependence) relapse; relapse prevention; treatment model; cognitive therapy; behavior therapy; risk factors; coping skills; self efficacy; expectancy; AOD (alcohol and other drug) abstinence; lifestyle; AOD craving; intervention; alcohol cue; reliability (research methods); validity (research methods); literature review elapse, or the return to heavy cess, a series of events that unfold over alcohol use following a period of time (Annis 1986; Litman et al. 1979; MARY E.
    [Show full text]
  • Acceptance and Mindfulness in Cognitive Behavior Therapy
    Acceptance and Mindfulness in Cognitive Behavior Therapy Understanding and Applying the New Therapies Edited by James D. Herbert Evan M. Forman John Wiley & Sons, Inc. JWBT357-FM_i-x.indd i 10/7/10 2:41:53 PM This book is printed on acid-free paper. ϱ Copyright © 2011 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc. All rights reserved. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Inc., Hoboken, New Jersey. Published simultaneously in Canada. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, scanning, or otherwise, except as permitted under Section 107 or 108 of the 1976 United States Copyright Act, without either the prior written permission of the publisher, or authorization through payment of the appropriate per-copy fee to the Copyright Clearance Center, Inc., 222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers, MA 01923, (978) 750-8400, fax (978) 646-8600, or on the web at www. copyright.com. Requests to the publisher for permission should be addressed to the Permissions Department, John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 111 River Street, Hoboken, NJ 07030, (201) 748-6011, fax (201) 748-6008. Limit of Liability/Disclaimer of Warranty: While the publisher and author have used their best efforts in preparing this book, they make no representations or warranties with respect to the accuracy or completeness of the contents of this book and specifically disclaim any implied warranties of merchantability or fitness for a particular purpose. No warranty may be created or extended by sales representatives or written sales materials. The advice and strategies contained herein may not be suitable for your situation.
    [Show full text]
  • An Exploratory Study of Seven Procedures to Encourage College Student Studying Behavior
    Loyola University Chicago Loyola eCommons Master's Theses Theses and Dissertations 1977 An Exploratory Study of Seven Procedures to Encourage College Student Studying Behavior Calvin Edward Hainzinger Loyola University Chicago Follow this and additional works at: https://ecommons.luc.edu/luc_theses Part of the Education Commons Recommended Citation Hainzinger, Calvin Edward, "An Exploratory Study of Seven Procedures to Encourage College Student Studying Behavior" (1977). Master's Theses. 2972. https://ecommons.luc.edu/luc_theses/2972 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Theses and Dissertations at Loyola eCommons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Master's Theses by an authorized administrator of Loyola eCommons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-Noncommercial-No Derivative Works 3.0 License. Copyright © 1977 Calvin Edward Hainzinger AN EXPLORATORY STUDY OF SEVEN PROCEDURES TO ENCOURAGE COLLEGE STUDENT STUDYING BEHAVIOR by Calvin Hainzinger A Thesis Submitted to the-Faculty of the Graduate School of Loyola University of Chicago in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for. the Degree of Master of Arts July 1977 .. VITA Calvin Edward Hainzinger was born in Chicago, Illinois on January 22, 1953. His elementary and secondary education was ob­ tained in the public schools of Niles, Illinois and Park Ridge, Illinois. He was granted a Bachelor of Arts Degree from the Uni­ versity of Illinois at Chicago Circle with a major in psychology in December, 1974. In February, 1975, he entered the Master of Arts program at Loyola University of Chicago where he pursued the study of educational psychology.
    [Show full text]