Delusions of Gender: How Our Minds, Society, and Neurosexism Create Difference. by CORDELIA FINE. New York: W. W. Norton Comp

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Delusions of Gender: How Our Minds, Society, and Neurosexism Create Difference. by CORDELIA FINE. New York: W. W. Norton Comp INVITED REVIEW ESSAY Delusions of Gender: How Our Minds, Society, and Neurosexism Create Difference. By CORDELIA FINE. New York: W. W. Norton & Company, 2010. Brain Storm: The Flaws in the Science of Sex Differences.ByREBECCAM. JORDAN-YOUNG. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 2010. Letitia Meynell “…we’re only trying to find the biological roots to gender inequality, so why be fussy, right?” (Fine, 108). That the search for dimorphic cognitive, affective, and behavioral sex differences con- tinues is no doubt a source of anxiety for those who have long embraced a feminist or progressive ideal of equality for all postnatal humans, regardless of their sex/gender (or other) identities. Indeed, the prevalence of media reports and best-selling accounts of scientific findings of fundamental neurological, psychological, and behavioral sex dif- ferences, in addition to the studies themselves, may give the most ardent feminists among us occasion to suspect that there might just be something to it: Putting aside the many queer, trans, and intersexed exceptions—for which some biological explana- tion must also, presumably, exist—a rational consideration of the mountain of evi- dence surely suggests that our natural history really has produced two fundamentally different types of people: men and women. Or so one might suppose. Feminists strug- gling with this haunting doubt would do well to take a look at Cordelia Fine’s Delu- sions of Gender: How Our Minds, Society, and Neurosexism Create Difference and Rebecca Jordan-Young’s Brain Storm: The Flaws in the Science of Sex Differences as effective remedies. These recent books join a now extensive literature by feminist aca- demics, both within and outside the biological and mind sciences, that meticulously and critically dissect empirical claims about human sex differences as they pertain to cognition, emotions, and behavior. Depressingly, though both address current as well as recent research, the heart of their criticism is familiar to feminists: much of the research on sex differences is, by ordinary scientific standards, bad science. Further- more, hopes for the future rest on proper controls for sexist bias and a successor science that adequately reflects the diversity of our (and, indeed, other) species. Hypatia vol. 28, no. 3 (Summer 2013) © by Hypatia, Inc. Invited Review Essay 685 Although they address much of the same material and draw some of the same conclusions (at least about the sciences), the two books have quite different approaches and virtues and will appeal to somewhat different audiences. Important to note is that neither author advocates abandoning the scientific study of human sex differences. Rather, both encourage the adoption of more nuanced, careful, and meth- odologically robust approaches to the subject. Neither denies average differences in the embodiment or behavior of various populations of human males and females— including, but not limited to, chromosomal differences, differences in hormonal pro- files, or differences in sexual behavior. Care, however, is required to parse such claims. Variability within female and male populations along any given measure, the interconnections and causal relations of the relevant physiological, psychological, and social systems, and the contingency of their development are complexities that any credible scientific study of sex differences must address. Fine and Jordan-Young target the inadequacies of some of the methods and practices found in recent research on sex differences. Moreover, they clearly reveal the looping effects among these meth- ods, the results of this research, and the present (if disavowed) sexism in our society. Feminist theorists will find that, though it revisits familiar territory, Delusions of Gender is still worth reading. Eminently readable and accessible to nonacademic audi- ences, Fine’s book pulls diverse threads together into a clear, concise, and compelling account. She divides her book into three parts—thefirstprovidingasocialcontext, the second exploring the weaknesses of various research programs, and the third look- ing forward to how this research and related ideologies can be expected to shape our children. The first part, “‘Half-Changed World,’ Half-Changed Minds,” relates the folk and popular science of sex differences to the multiple delusions and contradic- tions that arise in a society that is convinced of its own sexual equality while still being, along almost every social measure, clearly unequal. Here Fine relies heavily on social psychology. Her sources include studies revealing the sexist decisions and actions that are consistently made by individuals who are nonetheless sincerely com- mitted to sexual equality, the role of associative memory in the production of sexist assumptions, and the role of stereotype threat in women’s underperformance and play-it-safe strategies. She then brings these tools to the home front and the work- place to show how double standards and subtle, exclusionary messages keep even the most successful women working double days, while generally relegating women to less desirable social positions. Though sobering stuff, this part does highlight the social construction and profoundly contextual nature of sex differences in cognitive and affective performance, capacities, and behavior, thus providing ample reason for thinking that those group differences that are found are consistent with a largely social explanation. In the second part, “Neurosexism,” Fine turns to the various scientific programs that attempt to offer biological explanations for sex differences. She develops a his- torical theme, begun in the previous section, showing how contemporary sexism (in science and society at large) repeats the errors and injustices of the past. Clearly, Fine favors breadth over depth here, identifying multiple types of putative sex differences and various research programs, from fetal hormone testing, preference tests in 686 Hypatia neonates, intersexed children’s play behavior, and nonhuman animal studies, to neuroimaging studies on adult humans. It is upon this latter project that Fine turns her most detailed and damning critique. Fine’s case is built from the numerous ways in which neuroimaging studies of cognitive and affective states and capacities are fraught, from the much derided reverse inferences (151–52) to shockingly small sam- ple sizes and built-in tendencies to find and report differences rather than similarities (135). In this way, Fine not only introduces her readers to neurosexism, but also to the larger problem of neurononsense (154). Fine concludes this section with disturb- ing studies that link the reading of reports of essential sex differences with a tendency “to be more confident that society treats women fairly, and to feel less certain that the gender status quo is likely to change” (185). Part III, “Recycling Gender,” narrates, through child psychology and the study of parental behavior, the ways in which children are shaped from birth to constantly present their own gender and police the gender of others. With children’s gender identity and sexual expression being molded by adults, other children, clothing, chil- dren’s programming, advertising, toys, and books, no parent in the societies of the contemporary global north can pretend to offer a nongendered upbringing for their children. Fine was inspired to write the book upon discovering “[her] son’s kindergar- ten teacher reading a book that claimed that his brain was incapable of forging the connection between emotion and language” (174). The take-home message—that the science of sex differences tends to essentialize social norms underlying sexual inequality and reifies them for our children’s futures—leaves one with the urge to make Delusions of Gender required reading for kindergarten teachers everywhere. Whereas Fine has written a popular science book, Jordan-Young has written a technical academic book that is fundamentally about scientific methods and their vulnerabilities to the ideologies of sex difference and, concomitantly, sexuality and sexual orientation. Jordan-Young’s book exemplifies a certain kind of science-studies project, providing an exhaustive survey of a research program (more than 300 indi- vidual studies [xii]) to uncover its history, methods, commitments, and, in this case, a variety of devastating flaws. Rather than the familiar contemporary ethnographic approach, she takes a more traditional stance, identifying herself as a scientific insider who is offering a kind of peer review of a particular research program (10–12). Unlike Fine’s polemical and political approach, Jordan-Young limits herself to an epistemo- logical analysis of the science that is consistent with uncontroversial scientific stan- dards. Although she acknowledges the relevance of the science of sex differences in constructing larger sex/sexuality/gender norms, she does not, by and large, discuss it. Jordan-Young focuses on brain organization theory (BOT)—thetheorythatdraws together the variety of human chromosomes, hormones, brains, and genitals into a sex-dimorphic system. This theory implicitly underlies much of the research that Fine criticizes, making the two books interesting companion pieces. Proponents of BOT believe that just as humans develop sexually dimorphic genitals in utero, they also develop sexually dimorphic neurological and psychological characteristics. There are exceptions for whom genital development (intersexuals) and psychological develop- ment (intersexuals, transsexuals,
Recommended publications
  • Non-Binary Or Genderqueer Genders
    International Review of Psychiatry ISSN: 0954-0261 (Print) 1369-1627 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/iirp20 Non-binary or genderqueer genders Christina Richards, Walter Pierre Bouman, Leighton Seal, Meg John Barker, Timo O. Nieder & Guy T’Sjoen To cite this article: Christina Richards, Walter Pierre Bouman, Leighton Seal, Meg John Barker, Timo O. Nieder & Guy T’Sjoen (2016) Non-binary or genderqueer genders, International Review of Psychiatry, 28:1, 95-102, DOI: 10.3109/09540261.2015.1106446 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/09540261.2015.1106446 Published online: 12 Jan 2016. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 174 View related articles View Crossmark data Citing articles: 1 View citing articles Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=iirp20 Download by: [212.200.65.127] Date: 18 February 2016, At: 12:21 INTERNATIONAL REVIEW OF PSYCHIATRY, 2016 VOL. 28, NO. 1, 95–102 http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/09540261.2015.1106446 REVIEW ARTICLE Non-binary or genderqueer genders Christina Richardsa,b, Walter Pierre Boumana, Leighton Sealb, Meg John Barkerc, Timo O. Niederd and Guy T’Sjoene aNottingham Centre for Gender Dysphoria, Nottingham, UK; bCharing Cross Gender Identity Clinic, London, UK; cDepartment of Psychology in Social Sciences, Open University, Milton Keynes, UK; dInterdisciplinary Transgender Health Care Centre Hamburg, Department for Sex Research and Forensic Psychiatry, University Medical Centre Hamburg-Eppendorf (UKE), Germany; eCentre for Sexology and Gender, Department of Endocrinology, Ghent University, Belgium ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY Some people have a gender which is neither male nor female and may identify as both male and Received 29 June 2015 female at one time, as different genders at different times, as no gender at all, or dispute the very Accepted 6 October 2015 idea of only two genders.
    [Show full text]
  • Neuroscience and Sex/Gender
    Neuroethics (2012) 5:211–215 DOI 10.1007/s12152-012-9165-5 EDITORIAL NOTE Neuroscience and Sex/Gender Isabelle Dussauge & Anelis Kaiser Received: 4 September 2012 /Accepted: 13 September 2012 /Published online: 2 October 2012 # Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2012 This special issue publishes interdisciplinary scholar- hosts very different epistemological approaches, a ship which aims to map and re-imagine the relations common knowledge of neuroscience and gender between neuroscience and gender studies. studies was a prerequisite for the group’stheoret- ical and methodological exchange. The participants lively debated crucial issues, from current research neuroGenderings: The Network on sex/gender difference in neuropsychology, through the implications of notions of sex/gender, The authors of the present special issue were all par- gender identity and sexuality used in neuroscien- ticipants in the workshop neuroGenderings: Critical tific experimentation, to the social workings of a Studies of the Sexed Brain (Uppsala, 2010). Then co- sexed/gendered brain. organizers, now guest editors, we work in gender More precisely, the neuroGenderings workshop studies, neuroscience, and science and technology achieved an impressive first mapping of the research studies. In 2010, we did not know for a fact that the on sex/gender in neurosciences and the methodological neuroGenderings initiative would grow and develop frames used in those sciences. We discussed, for in- into an international network and conference series. stance, the role assigned to “sexed” regions of the brain, Now we know. by analyzing the relevance of the notion of sexual di- In neuroGenderings, a transdisciplinary and inter- morphism, itself a system of significance that is always national group of researchers from the neurosciences, and solely framed by neuro-logical sexual dichotomy.
    [Show full text]
  • Debating Sex Differences in Cognition: We Can Do Better What I Learnt Teaching Cordelia Fine’S “Delusions of Gender”
    Debating sex differences in cognition: we can do better What I learnt teaching Cordelia Fine’s “Delusions of Gender” Tom Stafford, @tomstafford University of Sheffield The graduate class PSY6316 ‘Current Issues in Cognitive Neuroscience’. MSc course, ~15 people. Stafford, T. (2008), A fire to be lighted: a case-study in enquiry-based learning, Practice and Evidence of Scholarship of Teaching and Learning in Higher Education, Vol. 3, No. 1, April 2008, pp.20-42. “There are sex differences in the brain” Fine (Delusions, Introduction, p xxvii) “Anti-sex difference” investigators? Cahill (2014) http://www.dana.org/Cerebrum/2014/Equal_%E2%89%A0_The_Same__Sex_Differences_in_the_Hu man_Brain/ https://whyevolutionistrue.wordpress.com/2017/01/20/are-male-and-female-brains-absolutely-identical/ Sarah Ditum, The Guardian, 18th January 2017 Not what Fine thinks. Not what Ditum thinks. Headline chosen by subeditor Original: http://web.archive.org/web/20170118081437/www.theguardian.com/books/2017/jan/18/testosterone- rex-review-cordelia-fine Current: https://www.theguardian.com/books/2017/jan/18/testosterone-rex-review-cordelia-fine We can do better We can quantify the size of differences Interpreting Cohen's d effect size an interactive visualization by Kristoffer Magnusson http://rpsychologist.com/d3/cohend/ Sex differences in cognition are small https://mindhacks.com/2017/02/14/sex-differences-in-cognition-are-small/ The Gender similarities hypothesis “The differences model, which argues that males and females are vastly different psychologically, dominates the popular media. Here, the author advances a very different view, the gender similarities hypothesis, which holds that males and females are similar on most, but not all, psychological variables” Hyde, J.
    [Show full text]
  • Investigating the Existence, Cognitive Attributes and Potential Pathological Consequences of the Extreme Female Brain
    INVESTIGATING THE EXISTENCE, COGNITIVE ATTRIBUTES AND POTENTIAL PATHOLOGICAL CONSEQUENCES OF THE EXTREME FEMALE BRAIN Sarah Louise JONES Submitted for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Division of Psychology Faculty of Social Sciences University of Bradford 2 0 1 6 Abstract INVESTIGATING THE EXISTENCE, COGNITIVE ATTRIBUTES AND POTENTIAL PATHOLOGICAL CONSEQUENCES OF THE EXTREME FEMALE BRAIN Sarah Louise Jones Key words: extreme female brain, empathising, systemising, sex differences, autism, schizophrenia, memory, cognition, gene imprinting, paranoid ideation The ‘extreme female brain’ (EFB) is derived from the empathising - systemising theory (E-S) which hypothesises that sex differences in cognition exist on a continuum, based on abilities in ‘empathising’ and ‘systemising’ (Baron-Cohen, 2003). The EFB profile; extreme empathising alongside deficient systemising, has received little attention in social cognitive neuroscience research, compared to the extreme male brain, which has advanced the knowledge of sex differences in the expression of autism. Currently, there is no solid evidence of a clinical pathology relating to the EFB nor a marker of cognition associated with a person’s ‘place’ on the E-S continuum. Here, an episodic memory paradigm with social and non-social conditions was given to participants along with measures of empathising and systemising. Scores on the social condition predicted where a person lies on the E-S continuum. The thesis then investigated the hypothesis that schizophrenia is expressed in the feminised profile (Badcock & Crepsi, 2006) i and the presumption that empathising and systemising demonstrate a trade­ off. Elements of paranoia were associated with an empathising bias. However, a bias in systemising ability was associated with schizotypy along with a significant overlap in the expression of autistic traits and schizotypy.
    [Show full text]
  • Delusions of Gender: How Our Minds, Society, and Neurosexism Create Difference
    P a g e | 145 Delusions of Gender: How Our Minds, Society, and Neurosexism Create Difference Author: Cordelia Fine, 2010, Published by W. W. Norton & Company Book review by Sorana-Alexandra CONSTANTINESCU Babes-Bolyai University, Cluj-Napoca, Romania [email protected] This book is an alternative to the vast literature that aims to show us why women are from Venus and men are from Mars. As pretentions it might sound, I consider this book to be a must read for everybody, not only for academics or researchers. Gender myth perpetuation can be in some proportion stopped by educating people about the various biases that can interfere with the outcome of studies on sex and gender, and the factors that can favor or inhibit their popularization outside academia, which could, in the long run, lead to a discrediting or at least a critical reception of the wealth of pseudo- scientific claims of biologically inherent differences in the psychological and social construction of men and women. Emphasis in writings that explain that there are hard-wired differences between men and women is placed upon the fine-tuned emotional skills of women, as opposed to their emotionally helpless male counterpart, to sugarcoat the inherently misogynistic message at the core of these studies: it is due to the very nature of our species that women are underrepresented in the hard sciences and mathematics, in leadership etc., not due to historical systemic oppression. While the history of sexism is not explicitly denied, it is provided with an implicit excuse in an appeal to nature, and the active role of excluding women from the political, economic and academic spheres is thus minimized, and passed off as being in a large part due to women being, on average, naturally deficient in the necessary skills required to be involved in these activities.
    [Show full text]
  • Women, Cycling, and the Public Sphere: How Discursive and Community Practices Affect Engagement
    Michigan Technological University Digital Commons @ Michigan Tech Dissertations, Master's Theses and Master's Dissertations, Master's Theses and Master's Reports - Open Reports 2015 WOMEN, CYCLING, AND THE PUBLIC SPHERE: HOW DISCURSIVE AND COMMUNITY PRACTICES AFFECT ENGAGEMENT Elsa L. Roberts Michigan Technological University Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.mtu.edu/etds Part of the Linguistics Commons, and the Women's Studies Commons Copyright 2015 Elsa L. Roberts Recommended Citation Roberts, Elsa L., "WOMEN, CYCLING, AND THE PUBLIC SPHERE: HOW DISCURSIVE AND COMMUNITY PRACTICES AFFECT ENGAGEMENT", Master's Thesis, Michigan Technological University, 2015. https://doi.org/10.37099/mtu.dc.etds/915 Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.mtu.edu/etds Part of the Linguistics Commons, and the Women's Studies Commons WOMEN, CYCLING, AND THE PUBLIC SPHERE: HOW DISCURSIVE AND COMMUNITY PRACTICES AFFECT ENGAGEMENT By Elsa L. Roberts A THESIS Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE In Rhetoric and Technical Communication MICHIGAN TECHNOLOGICAL UNIVERSITY 2015 © 2015 Elsa L. Roberts This thesis has been approved in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE in Rhetoric and Technical Communication. Department of Humanities Thesis Advisor: Victoria L. Bergvall Committee Member: Lauren M. Bowen Committee Member: M. Ann Brady Committee Member: Donald J. Lafreniere Department Chair: Ronald Strickland To my grandmother, who believed in me when no one else did. To my partner and best friend, who has been by my side since I was 18. And to my other best friend who is the Jake to my Finn, together we inhabit the struggle.
    [Show full text]
  • Gendering Animals 1. Introduction There Has Recently Been a Wave Of
    Gendering Animals 1. Introduction There has recently been a wave of feminist criticism aimed at scientific programs that investigate sex differences. Like many feminist critics before them, they draw attention to current scientific work that is replete with sexist bias (e.g., Fausto-Sterling 2012; Fine 2010, 2017; Jordan-Young 2010; Richardson 2013; Rippon 2019). Interestingly, while many of these critiques take pains to emphasize the complexity of the sex/gender distinction, sometimes seeming to elide it altogether, they inadvertently assert and reify it at the same time. Although some of these authors debunk the idea that for humans sex, gender, and sexuality can be successfully dissociated, they often do so by presupposing that humans are unique in having such complicated identities. For nonhumans sex and gender are clearly distinguished because, while many nonhuman animals and plants have sexes, few, if any, are thought to have genders. Gender is cultural and thus the stuff of human social identities, whereas sex is biological, in the realm of the natural (read nonhuman) world. This is true even for the members of species closely related to humans. As Cordelia Fine notes, “To suggest that non-human primates have socially constructed gender roles seems more or less akin to pinning a notice to one’s back that says, MOCK ME...” (2010, p. 128). In what follows, we hope to show that there are good, scientifically credible reasons for thinking that some nonhuman animals might have genders while also allowing that the troubling of the sex/gender distinction, typical in current feminist analyses, is importantly right.
    [Show full text]
  • NATURE and NURTURE • Coolnotcute.Com NATURE and NURTURE Boys Will Be Boys
    NATURE and NURTURE • coolnotcute.com NATURE and NURTURE Boys will be Boys This is the second of three essays relating to a female bias in picture book content. The first essay, COOL not CUTE, examines the origins of this bias and how it manifests itself. The third essay, FIGHTERS and FASHIONISTAS, addresses gender-stereotyping. This essay is intended to be read between the other two. All three essays can be found at coolnotcute.com In this essay I’m going to examine some of the scientific research that relates to the argument I’ve made about the female bias in picture book content in COOL not CUTE. I’ll be focussing on the work of psychologists Melissa Hines1, Simon Baron-Cohen2 and Richard E. Tremblay3 in particular. Although I have a keen interest in psychology, I’m obviously not an expert, so I’ve asked psychologist Claire Lawrence4, a friend of mine, to proof the science of this essay. While I’ve included some of Claire’s comments, it should be assumed that the opinions expressed are my own, unless otherwise stated. As I mentioned in COOL not CUTE, when I refer to boy-typical or girl-typical preferences, I’m making a generalised argument. Girls with boy-typical preferences and boys with girl-typical preferences need books that appeal to them just as much as any other child. I’m aware that even labelling preferences as “boy-typical” and “girl-typical” will raise concerns with some readers about conditioning children to conform to sexual stereotypes. These are important concerns and I’ve addressed them in a separate essay, FIGHTERS and FASHIONISTAS.
    [Show full text]
  • Public Engagement with Neuroscience
    The Brain in Society: Public Engagement with Neuroscience Cliodhna O’Connor Thesis submitted for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy University College London September 2013 DECLARATION I, Cliodhna O’Connor, confirm that the work presented in this thesis is my own. Where information has been derived from other sources, I confirm that this has been indicated in the thesis. ____________________________________________ Cliodhna O’Connor 1 DEDICATION To Mom and Dad, with love and thanks 2 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS My first thanks go to my supervisor, Hélène Joffe, who has guided and encouraged me tirelessly over the last three years. I will always be grateful for the time and energy that she has devoted to my work. The research would not have been possible without the financial support that I received from several sources: the EPSRC; the Faraday Institute for Science & Religion at St Edmund’s College, Cambridge; the Easter Week 1916 commemoration scholarship scheme; the UCL Graduate School Research Projects Fund; and the UCL Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology. I very much appreciate all of these contributions. The work presented in this thesis owes much to countless conversations I have had with colleagues, both within and outside UCL. The comments of the editors and anonymous reviewers of the journals to which I submitted articles over the course of my PhD were extremely helpful in refining my ideas, as were the audiences at the various conferences and workshops at which I presented my research. I would also like to thank Caroline Bradley for her help in the analysis stages. Finally, I wish to express my sincere gratitude to my family, friends and boyfriend for their constant support throughout the last three years.
    [Show full text]
  • SBC Publication List July 13Th 2019
    PUBLICATIONS BY SIMON BARON-COHEN (updated 13 July 2019) * Top 20 most important SECTION 1: PEER REVIEWED JOURNAL ARTICLES (in chronological order) 1985 1. *Baron-Cohen, S, Leslie, A.M., & Frith, U, (1985) Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind?” Cognition, 21, 37-46. 1986 2. Baron-Cohen, S, Leslie, A.M., & Frith, U, (1986) Mechanical, behavioural and Intentional understanding of picture stories in autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 113-125. 1987 3. Baron-Cohen, S, (1987) Autism and symbolic play. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 139-148. 4. Baron-Cohen, S, Wyke, M, & Binnie, C, (1987) Hearing words and seeing colours: an experimental investigation of a case of synaesthesia. Perception, 16, 761-767. 1988 5. Baron-Cohen, S, (1988) Social and pragmatic deficits in autism: cognitive or affective? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18, 379-402. 6. Baron-Cohen, S, (1988) An assessment of violence in a young man with Asperger's Syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 29, 351-360. 7. Baron-Cohen, S, (1988) “Without a theory of mind one cannot participate in a conversation”. Cognition, 29, 83-84. 1989 8. Baron-Cohen, S, (1989) The autistic child's theory of mind: a case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 285-297. 9. Baron-Cohen, S, (1989) Perceptual role taking and protodeclarative pointing in autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 113-127. 10. Baron-Cohen, S, (1989) Do autistic children have obsessions and compulsions? British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 28, 193-200. 11.
    [Show full text]
  • The Linguistic Defanging of Women's Issues and the Legal Danger Of
    17 NEV. L.J. 667 ORWOLL -FINAL.DOCX 5/10/17 1:12 PM PREGNANT “PERSONS”: THE LINGUISTIC DEFANGING OF WOMEN’S ISSUES AND THE LEGAL DANGER OF “BRAIN-SEX” LANGUAGE Andrea Orwoll* “Women have had the power of naming stolen from us.”1 Mary Daly “Linguistic imbalances are worthy of study because they bring into sharper fo- cus real-world imbalances and inequities.”2 Professor Robin Tolmach Lakoff' TABLE OF CONTENTS INTRODUCTION ................................................................................................ 668 I. THE HISTORY OF THE WORD “WOMAN” IN THE LAW: CONSTITUTION TO COVERTURE TO CREDIT ......................................... 673 A. The Founding’s Missing Women ................................................. 673 B. Sexist Legal Language ................................................................. 674 C. Legal Euphemisms for “Woman” ................................................ 675 D. Uses of “Woman” to Close the Legal Gap .................................. 676 II. THE ROOTS OF THE LEGAL SYSTEM’S MISOGYNY AND NEUROSEXISM ..................................................................................... 678 A. Affirming Physical Reality and Debunking Brain Sex ................. 679 B. Legalized Neurosexism ................................................................ 685 III. MISOGYNY BY ANY OTHER NAME ...................................................... 688 * J.D. Candidate, Spring 2017, William S. Boyd School of Law, University of Nevada, Las Vegas. I would like to thank my many pillars of writerly support—the Nevada Law Journal’s staff for their careful, conscientious edits, and Professor Linda Berger for her guidance and generous assistance with feminist history and phraseology. I also honor the many brave fem- inists who came before me, without whose words I could not write today. 1 MARY DALY, BEYOND GOD THE FATHER: TOWARD A PHILOSOPHY OF WOMEN’S LIBERATION 8 (New paperback ed. 1985). 2 ROBIN TOLMACH LAKOFF, LANGUAGE AND WOMAN'S PLACE: TEXT AND COMMENTARIES 69 (Mary Bucholtz ed., rev. and expanded ed.
    [Show full text]
  • Neurocultures – Neurogenderings II at the University of (Rectorate, University of Vienna) Vienna 13-15 September, 2012
    SATURDAY, 15 SEPTEMBER 2012 09.00-11.00: Panel III: NeuroCultures and 15.00-17.30: Panel V: Empirical NeuroGenderings II Brain Plasticity Kristina Gupta: “Transsexual Brains”: More of the Same and Victoria Pitts-Taylor: Embodied Simulation and Situated Something New Neurons: Lessons from Feminist Epistemologies Christel Gumy: The Gendered Tools of the Construction of Rachel Weitzenkorn: Disability and the Cerebral Subject the Unisex Adolescent Brain Heidi Maibom/Robyn Bluhm: It's All in the Brain, but not Lise Eliot: Neuroplasticity and the Development of Sex All of the Time: The Influence of Situation on Gender Differences Differences in Neuronal Activity Emily Ngubia Kuria: Experimenting with Gender. How Science Catherine Vidal: Neuro-Pedagogy against Neuro-Sexism Constructs Difference Deboleena Roy: Estrogen Receptors in the Brain. A Case for Chair: Emily Ngubia Kuria Situational Neuroendocrinology 11.00-11.30: Tea/Coffee Break Chair: Cordelia Fine 17.45-18.15: Round-up and Farewell 11.30-13.30: Panel IV: Theory and Epistemology Rebecca Jordan-Young/Sigrid Schmitz of NeuroGenderings Hannah Fitsch: What Goes around, Comes around: Visual Conference Board Knowledge in fMRI and its Implications for Research Isabelle Dussauge (Uppsala University) Practices Grit Höppner (University of Vienna) Katrin Nikoleyczik: Imaging Matters: An Agential Realist Rebecca Jordan-Young (Columbia University) Account of Neuroscientific Knowledge Production Anelis Kaiser (University of Freiburg) Alexander Stingl: Semantic Gaps, Epistemic Deficiencies, Cynthia
    [Show full text]