<<

Environmental Biology of 38: 393-397,1993. 0 1993 Kluwer Academic Publishers. Printed in the Netherlands.

A new heterospecific foraging association between the puddingwife , Halichoeres radiatus, and the bar jack, ruber: evaluation of the foraging consequences

Troy A. Baird Department of Biology, University of Central Oklahoma, Edmond, OK 73034, U.S.A.

Received 24.6.1992 Accepted 12.3.1993

Key words: Feeding, Interspecific association, Social behavior, Labridae, , Reef

Synopsis

The bar jack, Caranx ruber, was commonly observed to follow individual puddingwife ,Halichoeres radiatus, that were foraging on the substrate. Individuals of both speciesactively pursued the other to main- tain these heterospecific foraging ‘teams’, were sometimes attracted to feeding actsinitiated by team partners, and the foraging rates of teamed jacks and wrasseswere positively correlated. Pilfering of food items was rare, suggestinglittle, if any, competition cost of this foraging association. The ratio of bites to search in teamed jacks was over three times that when solitary, and jacks were sometimes aggressiveto conspecifics attempting to join their team, suggestingthat the association is beneficial to the jacks. Both bite and search rates were higher in puddingwifes when teamed with a jack, indicating that the association also benefits the wrasses. Benefits to puddingwifes may be derived directly from attendants becausewrasses were sometimes attracted to jack foraging acts. However, increased foraging in wrassesmay also be a consequenceof heightened moti- vation to feed owing to heterospecific social facilitation.

Introduction swimming closely to and above or beside a single individual of the ‘nuclear’ (Moynihan 1962, Foraging in social groups is well known among ver- Strand 1988,Sikkel & Hardison 1992). tebrates (Pulliam & Millikan 1982,Pulliam & Cara- Although it is widely held that heterospecific as- co 1984,Clark & Mange11986) and feeding togeth- sociations in fishes increase feeding opportunities er with conspecifics has been shown to enhancethe for attendants (Strand 1988), apparently only three foraging effectiveness of individual birds (Krebs et studies have involved collection of the quantitative al. 1972, Gotmark et al. 1986,Benkman 1988) and data necessary to evaluate this hypothesis. Both fishes (Pitcher et al. 1982, Baird et al. 1991). Al- yellow-headed wrasses, Halichoeres garnoti, and though social foraging generally involves individu- yellowtail snapper, Ocyurus chrysurus, exhibit als of the same species, heterospecific foraging as- higher feeding rates when attending groups of goat- sociations have also been reported in mammals fishes (Aronson & Sanderson1987, Sikkel & Hardi- (Devore & Hall 1965) birds (Moynihan 1962,Cody son 1992) while , Cephalopholis spp., in- 1971)and fishes (reviewed by Strand 1988).In fish- creased feeding rates by following octopus (Dia- es, these associations are usually characterized by mant & Shpigel 1985). The influence of being at- one or more individuals of an ‘attendant’ species tended by a heterospecific on the foraging 394 performance of nuclear individuals has not been by a jack (teamed, n = 24). During this set of obser- quantified. In many cases,foraging associationsin- vations, I recorded only the foraging acts of the volve specieswith little dietary overlap, hence,com- wrasses.Five minute focal observations were re- petition from attendants is unlikely (Strand 1988). corded on jacks when attending a puddingwife Other associationsinvolve two speciesthat some- (teamed, n = 18) and when foraging independently times eat the sameprey. Attendant pilfering of food from a wrasseor conspecifics(solitary, n = 18).Dur- items uncovered by nuclear individuals sometimes ing 5 min focal observations on teams, I recorded occurs (Dubin 1982,Strand 1988) suggestinga po- the foraging acts of both species.For both data sets, tential foraging cost to nuclear individuals. the of observations on solitary and teamed Here I report on a previously undescribed forag- subjects was randomized. The number of foraging ing association involving the , acts (defined below) per observation sessionwas di- Halichoeres radiatus, and the bar jack, Caranx rub- vided by the total minutes of observation to yield er. Puddingwife wrassesfeed exclusively on benthic per minute rates of foraging actswhich were usedin invertebrates (Randall 1967). Although bar jacks all analyses. feed largely on fish, remains of benthic-dwelling Feeding rates may vary as a function of body size mollusks and ,and sand in the guts of or time of day. To control for such variation, sub- jacks indicates that this speciessometimes eats the jects were categorized according to estimated fork same invertebrate prey as the wrasses (Randall length (small < 20 cm, medium 20-30 cm, large > 30 1967).Bar jacks are the faster, more mobile of these cm), and solitary and teamed observations were two species,and following wrassesthat are search- conducted on different individuals of the samesize ing the substrate is a striking departure from the class,within 15 min of one another. Becauseteamed usual tendency of jacks to swim higher in the water and solitary observations were conducted close to- column. Therefore, I expected that jacks might at- gether in time and on individuals of the same size tend wrassesin order to exploit benthic prey items class,I used a paired design for statistical analyses uncovered by puddingwifes, perhaps reducing the (Snedecor& Cochran 1980,p. 89). The sizeclass dis- foraging effectivenessof wrassesthrough increased tribution of 24 wrasse subject pairs was: small = 5, interspecific competition. To assessthe influence of medium = 9, large = 10,and for 18jack subject pairs this association on the foraging performance of was; small = 4, medium = 9, large = 5. both species,I recorded rates of feeding activities Although it is possible that subjects could have when individual fish were foraging solitarily, and been observedmore than once, the potential for re- when teamed together with one heterospecific. peated observationswas low becausestudy sessions were spread over a 500 m2area where both species were abundant. An inability to capture fish prohib- Methods ited correlation of food intake with the rate of fo- raging acts. Rather, I assumedthat rates of bite and Observations were conducted from 13July to 8 Au- search are correlates of food intake. Samples of gust 1991at Glover’s Reef, Belize, Central America, benthic invertebrates taken previously on this study in the channel between Long and Northeast Cays area show that prey are distributed randomly (described in Baird & Baird 1992). Fish were ob- (Baird & Liley 1989). Also, becausemany benthic served between 1300 and 1700h while snorkeling, invertebrates are mobile and secretive, it is prob- and data were recorded on slates.When not attend- able that detection of prey is correlated positively ed by a jack, puddingwife wrassesswam independ- with rate of foraging. ently from conspecificsor other species.Jacks often A foraging search was defined as the fish stop- swam with conspecifics, but solitary foraging was ping its forward swimming motion and its not unusual. I conducted separate 10 min focal ob- snout and visual field downward to scanthe bottom. servations on puddingwife wrassesforaging solita- Each of these species swam continuously except rily (n = 24 sessions)and when they were attended when foraging. Therefore, search acts were obvi- 395

swam beneath a coral formation whereupon the jack rapidly circled the coral, appearing to ‘wait’ for the wrasseto emerge. Of the nine separationsiniti- ated by wrasses,the team was maintained because jacks followed in eight instances(88.9%) and in the other casethe wrassereturned to the jack. Examination of the stomach contents of two specimensof each species,together with direct ob- servationson feeding, confirmed that both bar jacks Fig. 1. Bar jack (top) shown in typical position when attending a and puddingwife wrasseseat benthic invertebrates. puddingwife wrasse (bottom) in a heterospecific team. The wrasseswere not observed to pilfer food items from jacks. Jacksattempted to pilfer food items un- ous. A foraging bite was recorded eachtime the fish covered by wrassesduring only 3 of 70 bites taken in search posture contacted the substrate with its mouth. a I alsorecorded the frequency with which either of the two teamed fish temporarily swam more than three body lengths away from the other fish (sep- arations), and whether either fish swam back to re- I join the other within 15 sec.When teams separated for longer than 15 set prior to completion of a focal session, observations were terminated and these data were not used in analyses. Food pilfering events and interactions between the two teamed fish or with other conspecificswere also recorded.

Results P J

Bar jacks attended puddingwife wrassesby swim- ming parallel with, and above or to the side of the wrasse,usually with less than one body length sep- -I 1.6 arating the two fish (Fig. 1). Whether foraging sol- .- itarily or in a team, the wrassesswam continuously E' 1.2 along the bottom pausing only to search and take ti bites. Formation of jack-wrasseforaging teams was I: common, with many lasting longer than the dura- i! 0.8 tion of focal observations. Jacksinitiated 91.4% (96 E of 105) of the temporary separations between m 0.4 teamed fish, compared with only 8.6% that resulted from wrassesleaving teams. Of the 96 temporary separations initiated by jacks, teams were re- formed within 15 set in 50 cases (52.1%) because P J the wrassepursued the jack, whereasthe jack swam Fig. 2. a - Bites per min (X + SE) in puddingwife wrasses(P, n = to rejoin the wrasse in 46 (47.9%) of the separa- 24) and jacks (J, n = 18) when foraging solitarily (open bars) and tions. In two of the most striking examplesof team in teams (hatched bars). b-Searches per min (%+ SE) when sol- separation and re-formation, the puddingwife itary and in a team. 396 by puddingwifes, and only one of these attempts when they have detected a food item, then the high- was successful.No aggression between wrassesand er ratio of bite to search suggeststhat jacks may in- jacks was observed. However, in 17 of 42 observa- creaseprey detection efficiency by attending wrass- tions on teams, jacks initiated aggression (short es. Stomach content data indicate that bar jacks chasesand displacements) toward conspecifics that feed primarily on fish (Randall 1967). Because bar were attempting to join the team. In three observa- jacks apparently only supplement their diets with tion sessions,a second jack successfully supplanted benthic invertebrates, it may be particularly bene- and took the place of the jack participating in the ficial to attend species which are adept at bottom team at the beginning of the observation session. feeding if this increases feeding efficiency during When a jack was attending, wrasse search and the limited amount of time spent foraging on the bite rates were 1.71and 2.03 times higher (t = 3.98, benthos. This hypothesis is supported further by the p < 0.01, t = 2.65, p < 0.05 respectively) than when observation that bar jacks also occasionally fol- the wrassesforaged solitarily (Fig. 2). Wrasseswere lowed three other species that uncover benthic attracted to 22.4% (36 of 161) of the total foraging prey, Balistes vetula, Dasyatis americana, and Ky- acts (searches + bites) initiated by jacks, whereas phosus sectatrix. jacks were attracted to 31.1% (140 of 450) of the fo- Puddingwife wrasses were also attracted to fo- raging acts initiated by wrasses.Also, the total num- raging acts initiated by jacks, and displayed higher ber of foraging acts per session by jacks was corre- search and bite rates when attended. Furthermore, lated (r = 0.48, p < 0.05, n = 18) with the total forag- wrasses actively followed jacks which temporarily ing acts by wrasses. separated from them, and were not aggressive to- For jacks attending a wrasse,the average bite rate ward jacks. Together these observations suggest was 1.5 times higher than when jacks foraged sol- that, rather than being exploited by attendant jacks, itarily (Fig. 2), but this increase was not statistically the wrasses may also benefit from the association. significant (t = 0.78, p = 0.44). The search rate of Heightened rates of bite and search suggestthat the teamed jacks was only 67% of that when solitary association may increase foraging opportunities (t = 1.59, p = 0.13). As a consequence, the ratio of owing to the activities of the attendant jacks. How- bites to searchesby teamed jacks (ii bite search-’ = ever, one alternative explanation is that social sti- 0.32, SE = 0.08) was more than three times higher muli from attendants may elicit increased feeding (t = 2.52, p < 0.05) than that of solitary jacks (ii = motivation in puddingwifes. Social facilitation of 0.10, SE = 0.03). feeding by conspecifics has been demonstrated pre- viously in fishes (Olla & Samet 1974, Ryer & Olla 1991).If social facilitation explains increased forag- Discussion ing rates in teamed puddingwife wrasses, then sti- muli are from heterospecifics that exploit similar Although jacks were attracted to about one-third of food resources. the foraging acts initiated by puddingwifes and jack foraging rate was correlated positively with that of H. radiatus, I observed surprisingly little evidence Acknowledgements of jacks pilfering food items that wrasseshad locat- ed first. The absenceof pilfering suggestslittle if any This project was supported by a faculty research cost to puddingwifes owing to increased competi- grant from the Graduate College of the University tion from attendant jacks. of Central Oklahoma. The foraging activities of individuals of both spe- cies appeared to be enhanced by this association, but in different ways for each. In jacks, foraging References cited with puddingwifes increased the number of bites taken per search effort. If one assumesthat fish bite Aronson, R.B. & S.L. Sanderson. 1987. Benefits of heterospecific 397

foraging by the wrasse,Halichoeresgarnoti (Pisces: Moynihan, M. 1962.The organization and probable evolution of Labridae). Em. Biol. Fish. 18: 303-308. some mixed species of neotropical birds. Smithsonian Misc. Baird, T.A. & T.D. Baird. 1992.Colony formation and some pos- Coil. 143: l-40. sible benefits and costs of gregarious living in the territorial Olla, B.L. & C. Samet. 1974.Fish-to-fish attraction and facilita- sand tilefish, Malacanthus plumieri. Bull. Mar. Sci. 50: 56-65. tion of feeding behavior as mediated by visual stimuli in Baird, T.A. & N.R. Liley. 1989.The evolutionary significance of striped mullet, Mugil cephalus. J. Fish. Res. Board Can. 31: harem polygyny in the sand tilefish, Malacanthusplumieri: re- 1621-1630. source or female defence? Anim. Behav. 38: 817-829. Pitcher, T.J., A.E. Magurran & I.J. Winfield. 1982.Fish in larger Baird, T.A., C.H. Ryer & B.L. Olla. 1991.Social enhancement of shoals find food faster. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 10: 149-151. foraging on an ephemeral food source in juvenile walleye pol- Pulliam, R.H. & G.C. Millikan. 1982.Social organization in the lock, Theragra chalcogramma. Env. Biol. Fish. 31: 307-311. nonreproductive season. pp. 169-197. In: D.S. Farner, J.R. Benkman, C.W. 1988.Flock size,fooddispersion, and the feeding King & K.C. Parkes (ed.) Avian Biology, Vol. 6, Academic behavior of crossbills. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 23: 167-175. Press,London. Clark, C.W. & M. Mangel. 1986.The evolutionary advantagesof Pulliam, R.H. & T. Caraco. 1984. Living in groups: is there an group foraging. Theor. Pop. Biol. 30: 45-75. optimal group size?pp. 122-147.In: J.R. Krebs & N.B. Davies Cody, M.L. 1971.Finch flocks of the Mojave Desert. Theor. Pop. (ed.) Behavioural Ecology an Evolutionary Approach, Sec- Biol. 2: 142-158. ond Edition, Blackwell Scientific Publications, oxford. Devore, I. & R.R.L. Hall. 1965.Baboon ecology. pp. 2@52. In: I. Randall, J.E. 1967.Food habits of reef fishes of the . Devore (ed.) Primate Behavior, Holt, Rinehart & Winston, Stud. Trop. Oceanog. 5: 665-847. New York. Ryer, C.H. & B.L. Olla. 1991.Information transfer and the facil- Diamant, A. & M. Shpigel. 1985. Interspecific feeding associ- itation and inhibition of feeding in a schooling fish. Env. Biol. ations of (Teleosti: Serranidae) with octopuses and Fish. 30: 317-323. moray eels in the Gulf of Eilat (Aqaba). Env. Biol. Fish. 13: Sikkel, PC. & PD. Hardison. 1992. Interspecific feeding associ- 153-159. ations between the goatfish Muloides mavtinicus (Mullidae) Dubin, R.E. 1982. Behavioral interactions between Caribbean and a possible aggressivemimic, the snapper Ocyurus chrysu- reef fish and eels (Muraenidae and Ophichthidae). Copeia rus (Lutjanidae). Copeia 1992:914-917. 1982:229-232. Snedecor, G.W. & W.G. Cochran. 1980.Statistical methods. 7th Gotmark, F., D.W. Winkler & M. Andersson. 1986.Flock-feeding Edition, Iowa State University Press,Ames. 507 pp. on fish schools increases individual successin gulls. Nature Strand, S.1988. Following behavior: interspecific foraging associ- 319: 589-591. ations among Gulf of California reef fishes. Copeia 1988:351- Krebs, J.R., M.H. MacRoberts & J.M. Cullen. 1972.Flocking and 357. feeding in the great tit, Parus major, an experimental study. Ibis 114:507-530.