Do Avian Predators Avoid Autotomous Tails?

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Do Avian Predators Avoid Autotomous Tails? Cent. Eur. J. Biol. • 6(2) • 2011 • 293-299 DOI: 10.2478/s11535-010-0119-9 Central European Journal of Biology Seeing through the lizard’s trick: do avian predators avoid autotomous tails? Research Article Bart Vervust*, Hans Van Loy, Raoul Van Damme Laboratory for Functional Morphology, Department of Biology, University of Antwerp, B-2610 Wilrijk, Belgium Received 24 June 2010; Accepted 16 November 2010 Abstract: Counter-adaptations of predators towards their prey are a far less investigated phenomenon in predator-prey interactions. Caudal autotomy is generally considered an effective last-resort mechanism for evading predators. However, in victim-exploiter relationships, the efficacy of a strategy will obviously depend on the antagonist’s ability to counter it. In the logic of the predator-prey arms race, one would expect predators to develop attack strategies that minimize the chance of autotomy of the prey and damage on the predator. We tested whether avian predators preferred grasping lizards by their head. We constructed plasticine models of the Italian wall lizard (Podarcis sicula) and placed them in natural habitat of the species. Judging from counts of beak marks on the models, birds preferentially attack the head and might also avoid the tail and limb regions. While a preference for the head might not necessarily demonstrate tail and limb avoidance, this topic needs further exploration because it suggests that even unspecialised avian predators may see through the lizard’s trick-of-the-tail. This result may have implications for our understanding of the evolution of this peculiar defensive system and the loss or decreased tendency to shed the tail on island systems with the absence of terrestrial predators. Keywords: Autotomy • Predation • Predatory • Prey arms race • Podarcis sicula © Versita Sp. z o.o. make a detour and approach spitting spiders (Scytodes 1. Introduction pallidus) from the rear, opposite the end from which There is increasing evidence that predators actually the scytodid’s spit is fired [4]. Broad headed snakes evolve counter-adaptations in response to the defensive (Hoplocephalus bungaroides) may have evolved an adaptations of their prey species. Several cases are extreme sit-and-wait foraging habit because their prey known in which predators have evolved morphological (velvet geckos, Oedura lesueurii) can detect chemicals or physiological mechanisms, which allow them to left behind by moving snakes [5]. circumvent defensive tactics of particular prey. For Caudal autotomy, the ability to shed the tail, is instance, Lake Tanganyika crabs Platytelphusa an intriguing defensive technique that has evolved armata have evolved robust chelae in response to numerous times in a variety of prey animals [6-8]. the unusually hard shells of their gastropod prey (e.g. Many aspects of lizard tail autotomy, including its Spekia, Neothauma) prey [1] and North American histological mechanisms, ecological significance, intra- garter snakes (Thamnophis sirtalis) have become and interspecific variability, phylogenetic distribution resistant to the TTX toxins of Taricha newts [2]. More and evolutionary history have received considerable commonly, animals adapt their predatory behaviour in attention (reviews in [8] and [9]).One facet that has not reaction to the defensive tactics employed by particular been considered is the question of whether predators prey species. Australian death adders (Acanthophis may develop (within a lifetime or on an evolutionary praelongus) have learned to delay the consumption of scale) behavioural mechanisms that lower the toxic toads (Bufo marinus) until the chemical defence effectiveness of tail autotomy as an escape tactic. The loses its potency [3]. Jumping spiders (Portia labiata) high number of autotomized tails – compared to entire * E-mail: [email protected] 293 Seeing through the lizard’s trick: do avian predators avoid autotomous tails? lizards – found in predator stomachs [10], indicate on lizards. In 6% of the examined pellets (N=485) we that this is a highly effective antipredator strategy. On were able to find mandibles of lizards (B. Vervust, the other hand, caudal autotomy carries a series of unpublished). This high rate of occurrence suggests important and potentially lifelong costs (for an overview a potentially important predatory role of this gull in see [11]). (insular) lizard populations. More detailed information Here, we investigate the hypothesis that avian on the study system can be found in [21]. predators will prefer grasping lizards by the head to prevent both tail autotomy (inducing the likely escape 2.2 Model construction and experiment of the prey) and reducing the chance of damage to the Because direct observation of predation on lizards is predator. That such behaviour would arise does not difficult, we used the marks left behind by predators seem improbable, as many predators typically attack on plasticine models to make inferences about the particular parts of their prey’s body. For instance, sharks, predators’ target. Plasticine and clay models have lizards, birds, and mammals preferentially grasp and bite demonstrated their utility in many previous studies of their prey in the head or neck region [12-17]. While, this predation on reptiles (e.g. [17,21-23]). inclination is generally thought to aid speeding up the We produced 569 models of adult lizards by killing or swallowing process, or to serve in protection pouring non-toxic plasticine (Aquasoft, Eberhard against defensive biting, we examined which part of the Faber) into a flexible mould (pâte à modeller epoxy body of the lizard that an unspecialised avian predator superfine, Pascal Rosier) that was constructed using strikes at. a preserved museum specimen of Podarcis sicula, the same species native to the islets. Because males and females look the same from even a short 2. Experimental Procedures distance, we made use of a single morph only. The models were painted to resemble the colours of live 2.1 Study system and species animals according to the human visual system (non- This study was conducted on two small islets of the toxic paint LIVOS) and checked the reflectance of the Lastovo Nature reserve (Adriatic Sea, Croatia): Pod models using a photospectrometer. A methodological Mrčaru (42°46,7N; 16°46,7E) and Pod Kopište (42°45,7N; problem associated with the use of these models 16°43,7E). Both islands are uninhabited and consist of is a possible different perception of the models by an elevated and more vegetated centre encircled by a avian predators [24]. Therefore we test reflectance lower girdle of almost barren rocks. They contain dense of models with a spectrophotometer; reflectance populations of the Italian wall lizard (Podarcis sicula), spectra were obtained with a portable high-resolution a robust, diurnal, heliothermic lacertid lizard. There are photospectrometer (model HR 2000, Ocean Optics, no predatory mammals on the islands. Most predation Duiven, The Netherlands) to compare the colours of on Podarcis sicula is likely due to yellow-legged gulls the models (N=10) with those of real lizards (N=10). (Larus michahellis) which visit and nest on the islands This device detects reflection spectra of visible light in higher abundances than other species. Occasionally, and UV-reflectance (200-1100 nm) with a resolution other possible bird predators visit the islands, including of 1.5 nm. Visible and UV light is provided by a Ravens (Corvus corax), Falcons (Falco eleonorae, deuterium-tungsten light source (DH 2000-Bal, Ocean F. tinnunculus, F. peregrinus), common buzzards (Buteo Optics, Duiven, The Netherlands), which generates buteo), short-toed eagle (Circaetus gallicus), and illumination for wavelengths from 200 to 1100 nm. herons (Ardea cinerea, A. purpurea, Ardeola ralloides). We made use of a coincident reflectance probe It should be noted that all of these occasional species (QR400-7-UV/VIS-BX, Ø 2.6 mm, Ocean Optics, were only observed once of twice during the four-year Duiven, The Netherlands) to take spectra of the study on these islands. Additionally, gulls tend to chase surface of each individual. In order to characterize away more dangerous bird predators (e.g. crows and overall colouration, four reflectance spectra were taken falcons) and predation intensity increased near a gull across the body of the animal or model (interparietal, colony on Podarcis atrata [18]. The importance of gulls central mid dorsal spot and two lateral dorsal spots). as predators of lizards is debated (e.g. [19,20]). The spectra from these points were averaged by We conducted a dietary investigation and therefore calculating the mean reflectance intensity at the collected random faecal pellets from around the gull’s 876 increments from wavelengths 400–700 nm. The nests on the islands. The majority of regurgitates difference between real and replica lizards was contained food from only one foraging habitat. Our clearly reflected in their reflectance spectra and the dietary analyses showed that this species of gull feeds spectral parameters. Real lizards had low reflectance 294 B. Vervust et al. percentages for all wavelengths relative to the replica of the standard errors for the percentage values was lizards (B. Vervust, unpublished data). The replica performed using the equation: SE = sqrt (P(100-P)/n), lizards showed typical spectra with high reflectance of where P = the percentage and n = the sample size [26]. 550–680 nm. For determining whether our results might have been We placed the plasticine models in rows (average confounded by constant sum constraints, we repeated length: 65 m, range: 45-115 m), at approximately 2 m our analysis, leaving out the most attacked body region intervals, putting each model in approximately the (i.e., we tried to differentiate between preference and same position as the last real lizard observed prior avoidance). Statistical analyses were conducted with to positioning the model (for details see [21]). Lizard SPSS (version 15.0). densities on these islands are exceptionally high [25]. Because of the structural complexity of the islands, each model was not visible (from human eye view) from 3.
Recommended publications
  • Failure to Launch? the Influence of Limb Autotomy on the Escape Behavior of a Semiaquatic Grasshopper Paroxya Atlantica
    Behavioral Ecology doi:10.1093/beheco/arr045 Advance Access publication 4 May 2011 Original Article Failure to launch? The influence of limb autotomy on the escape behavior of a semiaquatic grasshopper Paroxya atlantica (Acrididae) Philip W. Batemana,b,c and Patricia A. Flemingb aDepartment of Zoology and Entomology, University of Pretoria, Pretoria 0002, South Africa, bSchool of Veterinary and Biomedical Sciences, Murdoch University, Murdoch, WA 6150, Australia, and cArchbold Biological Station, Lake Placid, PO Box 2057, Lake Placid, FL 33862, USA Downloaded from Autotomy is an extreme escape tactic where an animal sheds an appendage to escape predation. Many species alter their behavior postautotomy to compensate for this loss. We examined the escape behavior in the field of a semiaquatic grasshopper (Paroxya atlantica) that could escape either by flight and landing in vegetation or flight and landing in water and swimming to safety. We predicted that animals missing a hind limb would be more reactive (i.e., have longer flight initiation distances; FID) and would beheco.oxfordjournals.org prefer to escape to vegetation rather than to water as loss of a limb is likely to reduce swimming ability. However, our predictions were not supported. FID in autotomized animals was not different from that in intact animals. Furthermore, although autotom- ized grasshoppers paused more often and swam slower than intact individuals, autotomized grasshoppers more often escaped to water, reaching it via shorter flights that were lateral to the approach of the observer (intact grasshoppers more often flew directly away from the observer). We also noted differences in behavior before disturbance: Autotomised animals perched lower on emergent vegetation than did intact ones, presumably in readiness for escape via water, and also showed a greater likelihood to at Murdoch University on June 19, 2011 hide (squirreling) from the approaching observer prior to launch into flight.
    [Show full text]
  • Mantodea (Insecta), with a Review of Aspects of Functional Morphology and Biology
    aua o ew eaa Ramsay, G. W. 1990: Mantodea (Insecta), with a review of aspects of functional morphology and biology. Fauna of New Zealand 19, 96 pp. Editorial Advisory Group (aoimes mae o a oaioa asis MEMBERS AT DSIR PLANT PROTECTION Mou Ae eseac Cee iae ag Aucka ew eaa Ex officio ieco — M ogwo eae Sysemaics Gou — M S ugae Co-opted from within Systematics Group Dr B. A ooway Κ Cosy UIESIIES EESEAIE R. M. Emeso Eomoogy eame ico Uiesiy Caeuy ew eaa MUSEUMS EESEAIE M R. L. ama aua isoy Ui aioa Museum o iae ag Weigo ew eaa OESEAS REPRESENTATIVE J. F. awece CSIO iisio o Eomoogy GO o 1700, Caea Ciy AC 2601, Ausaia Series Editor M C ua Sysemaics Gou SI a oecio Mou Ae eseac Cee iae ag Aucka ew eaa aua o ew eaa Number 19 Maoea (Iseca wi a eiew o asecs o ucioa mooogy a ioogy G W Ramsay SI a oecio M Ae eseac Cee iae ag Aucka ew eaa emoa us wig mooogy eosigma cooaio siuaio acousic sesiiiy eece eaiou egeeaio eaio aasiism aoogy a ie Caaoguig-i-uicaio ciaio AMSAY GW Maoea (Iseca – Weigo SI uisig 199 (aua o ew eaa ISS 111-533 ; o 19 IS -77-51-1 I ie II Seies UC 59575(931 Date of publication: see cover of subsequent numbers Suggese om o ciaio amsay GW 199 Maoea (Iseca wi a eiew o asecs o ucioa mooogy a ioogy Fauna of New Zealand [no.] 19. —— Fauna o New Zealand is eae o uicaio y e Seies Eio usig comue- ase e ocessig ayou a ase ie ecoogy e Eioia Aisoy Gou a e Seies Eio ackowege e oowig co-oeaio SI UISIG awco – sueisio o oucio a isiuio M C Maews – assisace wi oucio a makeig Ms A Wig – assisace wi uiciy a isiuio MOU AE ESEAC CEE SI Miss M oy
    [Show full text]
  • Tail Autotomy Plays No Important Role in Influencing Locomotor Performance and Antipredator Behavior in a Cursorial Gecko
    ethology international journal of behavioural biology Ethology Tail Autotomy Plays No Important Role in Influencing Locomotor Performance and Anti-Predator Behavior in a Cursorial Gecko Hong-Liang Lu* , Guo-Hua Ding , Ping Ding* & Xiang Ji * Key Laboratory of Conservation Biology for Endangered Wildlife of the Ministry of Education, College of Life Sciences, Zhejiang University, Hangz- hou 310058, Zhejiang, China Jiangsu Key Laboratory for Biodiversity and Biotechnology, College of Life Sciences, Nanjing Normal University, Nanjing 210046, Jiangsu, China Correspondence Abstract Xiang Ji, Jiangsu Key Laboratory for Biodiversity and Biotechnology, College of Life We used the frog-eyed sand gecko (Teratoscincus scincus) as a model sys- Sciences, Nanjing Normal University, Nanjing tem to evaluate the locomotor costs of tail loss, and to examine whether 210046, Jiangsu, China. tailless geckos use alternative anti-predator behavior to compensate for E-mail: [email protected], xiangji150@ the costs of tail loss. Of the 16 field-captured geckos, eight were used as hotmail.com experimental animals and the remaining ones as controls. Locomotor performance, activity level and anti-predator behavior were measured Received: January 21, 2010 Initial acceptance: February 22, 2010 for experimental geckos before and after the tail-removing treatment. Final acceptance: March 15, 2010 Control geckos never undergoing the tail-removing manipulation were (J. Kotiaho) measured to serve as controls for the measurements taken at the same time for experimental geckos. Experimental geckos did not differ from doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2010.01780.x controls in activity level before they underwent the tail-removing manipulation, but became less active thereafter.
    [Show full text]
  • Characterization of Arm Autotomy in the Octopus, Abdopus Aculeatus (D’Orbigny, 1834)
    Characterization of Arm Autotomy in the Octopus, Abdopus aculeatus (d’Orbigny, 1834) By Jean Sagman Alupay A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Integrative Biology in the Graduate Division of the University of California, Berkeley Committee in charge: Professor Roy L. Caldwell, Chair Professor David Lindberg Professor Damian Elias Fall 2013 ABSTRACT Characterization of Arm Autotomy in the Octopus, Abdopus aculeatus (d’Orbigny, 1834) By Jean Sagman Alupay Doctor of Philosophy in Integrative Biology University of California, Berkeley Professor Roy L. Caldwell, Chair Autotomy is the shedding of a body part as a means of secondary defense against a predator that has already made contact with the organism. This defense mechanism has been widely studied in a few model taxa, specifically lizards, a few groups of arthropods, and some echinoderms. All of these model organisms have a hard endo- or exo-skeleton surrounding the autotomized body part. There are several animals that are capable of autotomizing a limb but do not exhibit the same biological trends that these model organisms have in common. As a result, the mechanisms that underlie autotomy in the hard-bodied animals may not apply for soft bodied organisms. A behavioral ecology approach was used to study arm autotomy in the octopus, Abdopus aculeatus. Investigations concentrated on understanding the mechanistic underpinnings and adaptive value of autotomy in this soft-bodied animal. A. aculeatus was observed in the field on Mactan Island, Philippines in the dry and wet seasons, and compared with populations previously studied in Indonesia.
    [Show full text]
  • Science, Sentience, and Animal Welfare
    WellBeing International WBI Studies Repository 1-2013 Science, Sentience, and Animal Welfare Robert C. Jones California State University, Chico, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://www.wellbeingintlstudiesrepository.org/ethawel Part of the Animal Studies Commons, Ethics and Political Philosophy Commons, and the Nature and Society Relations Commons Recommended Citation Jones, R. C. (2013). Science, sentience, and animal welfare. Biology and Philosophy, 1-30. This material is brought to you for free and open access by WellBeing International. It has been accepted for inclusion by an authorized administrator of the WBI Studies Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Science, Sentience, and Animal Welfare Robert C. Jones California State University, Chico KEYWORDS animal, welfare, ethics, pain, sentience, cognition, agriculture, speciesism, biomedical research ABSTRACT I sketch briefly some of the more influential theories concerned with the moral status of nonhuman animals, highlighting their biological/physiological aspects. I then survey the most prominent empirical research on the physiological and cognitive capacities of nonhuman animals, focusing primarily on sentience, but looking also at a few other morally relevant capacities such as self-awareness, memory, and mindreading. Lastly, I discuss two examples of current animal welfare policy, namely, animals used in industrialized food production and in scientific research. I argue that even the most progressive current welfare policies lag behind, are ignorant of, or arbitrarily disregard the science on sentience and cognition. Introduction The contemporary connection between research on animal1 cognition and the moral status of animals goes back almost 40 years to the publication of two influential books: Donald Griffin’s The Question of Animal Awareness: Evolutionary Continuity of Mental Experience (1976) and Peter Singer’s groundbreaking Animal Liberation (1975).
    [Show full text]
  • Animal Models of Pain: Progress and Challenges
    REVIEWS Animal models of pain: progress and challenges Jeffrey S. Mogil Abstract | Many are frustrated with the lack of translational progress in the pain field, in which huge gains in basic science knowledge obtained using animal models have not led to the development of many new clinically effective compounds. A careful re-examination of animal models of pain is therefore warranted. Pain researchers now have at their disposal a much wider range of mutant animals to study, assays that more closely resemble clinical pain states, and dependent measures beyond simple reflexive withdrawal. However, the complexity of the phenomenon of pain has made it difficult to assess the true value of these advances. In addition, pain studies are importantly affected by a wide range of modulatory factors, including sex, genotype and social communication, all of which must be taken into account when using an animal model. Therapeutic index Pain is both a highly important health problem and an The debate is complicated by the lack of published The ratio of the minimum dose increasingly mature topic of study. Experiments on pain negative data, both from animal studies and from clini- of a drug that causes toxic using human subjects are practically challenging, funda- cal trials. However, in general animal models are thought effects to the therapeutic dose, mentally (and perhaps inescapably) subjective, and ethi- to be fairly effective in ‘backward’ validation (detecting used as a relative measure of cally self-limiting, and thus laboratory animal models analgesic activity of drugs already known to be clini- drug safety. of pain are widely used (BOX 1).
    [Show full text]
  • Comparative Morphology of the Stinger in Social Wasps (Hymenoptera: Vespidae)
    insects Article Comparative Morphology of the Stinger in Social Wasps (Hymenoptera: Vespidae) Mario Bissessarsingh 1,2 and Christopher K. Starr 1,* 1 Department of Life Sciences, University of the West Indies, St Augustine, Trinidad and Tobago; [email protected] 2 San Fernando East Secondary School, Pleasantville, Trinidad and Tobago * Correspondence: [email protected] Simple Summary: Both solitary and social wasps have a fully functional venom apparatus and can deliver painful stings, which they do in self-defense. However, solitary wasps sting in subduing prey, while social wasps do so in defense of the colony. The structure of the stinger is remarkably uniform across the large family that comprises both solitary and social species. The most notable source of variation is in the number and strength of barbs at the tips of the slender sting lancets that penetrate the wound in stinging. These are more numerous and robust in New World social species with very large colonies, so that in stinging human skin they often cannot be withdrawn, leading to sting autotomy, which is fatal to the wasp. This phenomenon is well-known from honey bees. Abstract: The physical features of the stinger are compared in 51 species of vespid wasps: 4 eumenines and zethines, 2 stenogastrines, 16 independent-founding polistines, 13 swarm-founding New World polistines, and 16 vespines. The overall structure of the stinger is remarkably uniform within the family. Although the wasps show a broad range in body size and social habits, the central part of Citation: Bissessarsingh, M.; Starr, the venom-delivery apparatus—the sting shaft—varies only to a modest extent in length relative to C.K.
    [Show full text]
  • Tail Autotomy Effects on the Escape Behavior of the Lizard Gonatodes Albogularis (Squamata: Sphaerodactylidae), from C Rdoba
    Domínguez-López et al. Revista Chilena de Historia Natural (2015) 88:1 DOI 10.1186/s40693-014-0010-6 RESEARCH Open Access Tail autotomy effects on the escape behavior of the lizard Gonatodes albogularis (Squamata: Sphaerodactylidae), from Córdoba, Colombia Moisés E Domínguez-López1*, Ángela M Ortega-león2 and Gastón J Zamora-abrego3 Abstract Background: Caudal autotomy appears to be an adaptation strategy to reduce the risk of being preyed upon. In an encounter with a predator, the prey must reduce the risk of being preyed upon, and one of the strategies that has exerted a strong pressure on selection has been tail loss. In lizards, it has been demonstrated that tail loss reduces the probability of survival in the event of a second attack; therefore, they must resort to new escape strategies to reduce the risk of falling prey. In order to evaluate the effect of tail loss on the escape behavior of Gonatodes albogularis in natural conditions, we took samples from a forest interior population. We expected that individuals that had not lost their tails would allow the predator to get closer than those that had lost it. For each sample, we recorded the following: (1) escape behavior, measured through three distances (e.g., approach distance, escape distance, and final distance); (2) distance to shelter; and (3) length of tail. We included only males in the study since we did not record any females without a tail and far fewer with a regenerated tail. Results: We found that tail loss does have an effect on the escape behavior of G.
    [Show full text]
  • Tail Autotomy, Tail Size, and Locomotor Performance in Lizards*
    669 Tail Autotomy, Tail Size, and Locomotor Performance in Lizards* Eric J. McElroy1,† Introduction Philip J. Bergmann2 Autotomy is a widespread phenomenon in which an animal 1Department of Biology, College of Charleston, Charleston, voluntarily sheds an appendage, as defined by Fredericq (1892) South Carolina 29401; 2Department of Biology, Clark and reviewed by Maginnis (2006). Perhaps the most conspic- University, Worcester, Massachusetts 01610 uous form of autotomy involves the loss of the tail, as exhibited by many species of lizards and salamanders (Wake and Dresner Accepted 3/2/2013; Electronically Published 11/5/2013 1967; Arnold 1984, 1988). Tail autotomy is most often asso- ciated with attempted predation, with the animal sacrificing its tail to a predator in order to escape. The most obvious benefit to this behavior is that the animal survives the predation at- ABSTRACT tempt (Daniels et al. 1986), with the potential for future re- The effect of tail autotomy on locomotor performance has been productive output. studied in a number of lizard species. Most of these studies Whereas the benefits of tail autotomy are simple and obvious, (65%) show that tail autotomy has a negative effect on sprint the costs associated with this behavior are more diverse and speed, some studies (26%) show no effect of autotomy on sprint obscure (recently reviewed in Clause and Capaldi 2006; Bate- speed, and a few (9%) show a positive effect of autotomy on man and Fleming 2009). Several decades of research have sprint speed. A variety of hypotheses have been proposed to shown that autotomy can result in the loss of fat reserves (Dial explain the variation across these studies, but none has been and Fitzpatrick 1981; Wilson and Booth 1998); reduced time tested.
    [Show full text]
  • Nothing in Cognitive Neuroscience Makes Sense Except in the Light of Evolution
    Perspective Nothing in Cognitive Neuroscience Makes Sense Except in the Light of Evolution Oscar Vilarroya 1,2 1 Department of Psychiatry and Legal Medicine, Autonomous University of Barcelona, Cerdanyola del Valles, 08193 Barcelona, Spain; [email protected] 2 Hospital del Mar Medical Research Institute, 08003 Barcelona, Spain Abstract: Evolutionary theory should be a fundamental guide for neuroscientists. This would seem a trivial statement, but I believe that taking it seriously is more complicated than it appears to be, as I argue in this article. Elsewhere, I proposed the notion of “bounded functionality” As a way to describe the constraints that should be considered when trying to understand the evolution of the brain. There are two bounded-functionality constraints that are essential to any evolution-minded approach to cognitive neuroscience. The first constraint, the bricoleur constraint, describes the evolutionary pressure for any adaptive solution to re-use any relevant resources available to the system before the selection situation appeared. The second constraint, the satisficing constraint, describes the fact that a trait only needs to behave more advantageously than its competitors in order to be selected. In this paper I describe how bounded-functionality can inform an evolutionary-minded approach to cognitive neuroscience. In order to do so, I resort to Nikolaas Tinbergen’s four questions about how to understand behavior, namely: function, causation, development and evolution. The bottom line of assuming Tinbergen’s questions is that any approach to cognitive neuroscience is intrinsically tentative, slow, and messy. Citation: Vilarroya, O. Nothing in Keywords: evolutionary constraints; neuroscientific theories; evolution of the brain Cognitive Neuroscience Makes Sense Except in the Light of Evolution.
    [Show full text]
  • Simplified Three-Dimensional Model Provides Anatomical Insights in Lizards’ Caudal Autotomy As Printed Illustration
    Anais da Academia Brasileira de Ciências (2015) 87(1): 63-70 (Annals of the Brazilian Academy of Sciences) Printed version ISSN 0001-3765 / Online version ISSN 1678-2690 http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/0001-3765201520130298 www.scielo.br/aabc Simplified three-dimensional model provides anatomical insights in lizards’ caudal autotomy as printed illustration JOANA D.C.G. DE AMORIM1, ISADORA TRAVNIK2 and BERNADETE M. DE SOUSA1,2 1Programa de Pós-Graduação em Comportamento e Biologia Animal, Universidade Federal de Juiz de Fora, Martelos, 36036-900 Juiz de Fora, MG, Brasil 2Universidade Federal de Juiz de Fora, Departamento de Zoologia, Laboratório de Herpetologia, Martelos, 36036-900 Juiz de Fora, MG, Brasil Manuscript received on August 29, 2013; accepted for publication on January 8, 2014 ABSTRACT Lizards’ caudal autotomy is a complex and vastly employed antipredator mechanism, with thorough anatomic adaptations involved. Due to its diminished size and intricate structures, vertebral anatomy is hard to be clearly conveyed to students and researchers of other areas. Three-dimensional models are prodigious tools in unveiling anatomical nuances. Some of the techniques used to create them can produce irregular and complicated forms, which despite being very accurate, lack didactical uniformity and simplicity. Since both are considered fundamental characteristics for comprehension, a simplified model could be the key to improve learning. The model here presented depicts the caudal osteology of Tropidurus itambere, and was designed to be concise, in order to be easily assimilated, yet complete, not to compromise the informative aspect. The creation process requires only basic skills in manipulating polygons in 3D modeling softwares, in addition to the appropriate knowledge of the structure to be modeled.
    [Show full text]
  • The Effect of Cheliped Loss on Blue Crab Callinectes Sapidus Rathbun Foraging Rate on Soft-Shell Clams Mya Arenaria L
    J. Exp. Mar. Bioi. Eeol., 151 (1991) 245-256 245 © 1991 Elsevier Science Publishers B.V. All rights reserved 0022-0981/91/$03.50 JEMBE 01644 The effect of cheliped loss on blue crab Callinectes sapidus Rathbun foraging rate on soft-shell clams Mya arenaria L. L. David Smith and Anson H. Hines Smithsonian Environmental Research Center, Edgewater, Maryland, USA (Received 21 September 1990; revision received 4 February 1991; accepted 9 May 1991) Abstract: Loss of single chelipeds was common (4-17%) in populations of blue crab Callinectes sapidus Rathbun surveyed in Chesapeake Bay and along the southeastern Atlantic coast and Gulf of Mexico. In contrast, blue crabs missing both chelipeds were relatively rare (0-5%). Laboratory experiments were conducted to determine the effects ofcheliped autotomy on blue crab foraging rate on soft-shell clams Mya arenaria L. In replicated aquarium experiments, clams (44-72 mm shell length) were allowed 48 h to burrow in sandy substratum before an intact male crab or one missing one or both chelipeds was introduced. After 48 h, clams were checked for evidence of siphon injury or mortality. Foraging rate (clams consumed/48 h) of crabs missing a single crusher cheliped did not differ significantly from that of intact crabs. Blue crabs compensated for the loss of a crusher by using the cutter cheliped and opposite first walking leg to forage. In contrast, crabs missing both chelipeds experienced a greater feeding handicap; their consumption of clams was significantly lower than that of intact crabs. The low incidence of individuals missing both chelipeds suggests that such injury does little to diminish blue crab predation on M.
    [Show full text]