Wishful Thinking in Foreign Policy: a Case Study of the Carter Administration and the Iranian Revolution

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Wishful Thinking in Foreign Policy: a Case Study of the Carter Administration and the Iranian Revolution MIAMI UNIVERSITY The Graduate School Certificate for Approving the Dissertation We hereby approve the Dissertation of Matthew H. Wahlert Candidate for the Degree: Doctor of Philosophy Patrick J. Haney Director Adeed Dawisha Reader Monica Schneider Reader Sheldon Anderson Graduate School Representative ABSTRACT WISHFUL THINKING IN FOREIGN POLICY: A CASE STUDY OF THE CARTER ADMINISTRATION AND THE IRANIAN REVOLUTION by Matthew H. Wahlert The purpose of this study is to explore the premise that the concept of wishful thinking, an element of motivational bias, can provide explanations for foreign policy decision-making. I engage the notion of wishful thinking in the form of a case study in order to explore foreign policy decisions made by the Carter Administration toward the Shah and Iran from 1977 through early 1979. International relations scholars typically view foreign policy decision-making in the context of three distinct levels of analysis – the international system, domestic politics, and individual level inputs. Compelling and useful theories attempting to explain foreign policy behavior have come from each of the three levels of analysis and even more recent literature has argued an interactive relationship among them. The individual level of analysis has led to the study of political psychology. Within the literature of political psychology, a preponderance of research concentrates on investigating notions of unbiased motivational errors, theories of cognitive dissonance, and rational actor models. However, the literature devoted to biased motivational errors – those involving hot cognition – is quite sporadic. I contend that the complexity involved in individual decision making calls for an examination of explanations beyond cold cognitive errors. Through the Carter case study - employing qualitative empirical evidence gleaned through a method of process tracing using government documents, primary sources, and memoirs of key actors – I explore whether wishful thinking, as a biased error, impacted Carter policy toward the Shah of Iran in the critical years of 1977, 1978, and the early days of 1979. WISHFUL THINKING IN FOREIGN POLICY: A CASE STUDY OF THE CARTER ADMINISTRATION AND THE IRANIAN REVOLUTION A DISSERTATION Submitted to the Faculty of Miami University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Department of Political Science by Matthew H. Wahlert Miami University Oxford, Ohio 2011 Dissertation Director: Patrick J. Haney © Matthew H. Wahlert 2011 Contents Part I – The Argument Chapter One The Psyche of Man: Motivated Bias in the Decision-Making Process, 2. The Agent: Individuals and Self-Serving Bias, 5. The Process: Motivated Processing, 8. The Product: Wishful Thinking, 12. Chapter Two Motivated Bias, the Individual Level of Analysis, and the Linking of Process, Structure, and Outcome in Foreign Policy Analysis, 20. In the Beginning, 20. Levels of Analysis to a Nested Approach and Beyond, 21. Political Science and the Dichotomy of Motivational and Cognitive Perspectives, 27. Chapter Three Motivated Biases in Foreign Policy Decision: A Review of Relevant Cases, 34. Motivated Bias and War, 47. Motivated Bias and Strategic Surprise, 51. Chapter Four Observation and Measurement: Methodological Techniques in the Inquiry into Process, Structure, and Outcome, 59. Selection of Carter and the Shah, 59. Process Tracing and Methodological Discussion, 61. Part II – The Context iii Chapter Five Toward Shahdulation: A Brief Survey of Relations between the United States and Iran, 1851 – 1977, 68. Chapter Six Perspectives on Carter as a Decision-Maker, 84. Carter as a Self-Confident Optimist, 84. The Carter Presidential Image, 87. Carter and Motivated Bias, 90. Carter and Defensive Avoidance, 92. Part III – The Cases Chapter Seven Island of Stability: The United States and the Shah, 1977, 99. Chapter Eight As Tehran Burns: The United States and the Shah, 1978, 107. Chapter Nine The Last Days of America‟s Shah: The United States and the Shah, 1979, 153. Part IV - Implications Chapter Ten Inside the White House and The Curious Case of Carter and the Shah, 168. Explanations for Carter Policy Choices, 170. The Plausibility of a Motivational Bias Explanation, 180. Part V – Appendix and References Glossary of Abbreviations, 188. National Security Meetings – Subjects, 189. Policy Review Committee Meetings – Subjects, 190. Special Coordinating Committee Meetings – Subjects, 193. Significant Dates in the Iranian Revolution, 196. Works Consulted, 200. iv Tables Table 3.1 Decision Making Models of the Bay of Pigs Invasion. 37. Table 3.2 Expanded Decision-Making Models of the Bay of Pigs Invasion, 38. v Illustrations Figure 1.1 Graphical Representation of Forms of Bias, 5. Figure 2.1 Tepid Decision Making Model, 32. vi Acknowledgements I am indebted to many individuals for the completion of this project. First and foremost, I would like to thank the members of my Ph.D. dissertation committee. Committee chair Patrick Haney offered constant advice and direction and the encouragement needed to complete the project. Likewise, I would also like to acknowledge Monica Schneider – especially for her comments on my early work – for helping me focus more clearly on a research question. Adeed Dawisha was a helpful advocate in examining my argument from many perspectives and Sheldon Anderson offered helpful criticism through the eyes of an historian. The staff at the Jimmy Carter Library in Atlanta, Georgia and the National Security Archives at George Washington University went above and beyond the call in assisting me with my project. Throughout my academic career I have accumulated a number of debts. I would like to thank the late James E. Cebula of the University of Cincinnati for his twenty years of mentorship and friendship. Without Jim, I would have never pursued advanced studies. Thomas Rivers of the University of Southern Indiana laid the groundwork for my constructionist perspective in his challenging coursework in the History of Rhetoric. Special thanks also goes out to Liam Anderson of Wright State for over ten years of mentoring and his colleagues – Laura Luehrmann and Donna Schlagheck – for helping me develop an International Relations mindset. Completing a Ph.D. while teaching high school full time is not an easy task. I would like to acknowledge the principal at St. Henry District High School in Erlanger, Kentucky – David Otte – for his patience and understanding as I completed my studies. The friendships I have built with Jay Graue, Chris Gunkle, Tony Harden, Shawn Schwarz, and Jeanne Vieth made the last few years much more bearable. I thank you for your counsel and friendship. I would also like to acknowledge all of the students and families of the St. Henry community for your unqualified support. Finally, I would like to express my unending gratitude to my mother, Donna, and my sisters – Melissa and Heather. Without the support and understanding of my family I would never have even began such a lengthy project. I also wish to acknowledge those unnamed whose support helped me complete my program of studies at Miami University, the comprehensive exams, and my dissertation. vii Part I THE ARGUMENT 1 Chapter One The Psyche of Man: Motivated Bias in the Decision Making Process Why does the decision-maker select one option over all the others? The answer to that query is certainly complex. Shafir and Tversky suggest that, given the variable of uncertainty, the decision maker assesses “potential consequences and their perceived likelihood.”1 Ultimately, the decision product, then, is the result of what the decision maker deems as “potential” and what the actor “perceives.” In other words, one must understand the process of decision making in order to fully analyze the decision product. One of the explanations for the decision making process is the concept of “wishful thinking.” The presence of uncertainty offers opportunity for the decision maker to perceive a narrative, or incoming data, in a manner that is consistent with desires and wishes. To be sure, wishful thinking appears as an explanation for decisions made in diverse fields that include finance, politics, sports, health, and romance. In literature and music, the protagonist often makes decisions based upon the notion of wishful thinking. Wishful thinking appears also throughout academia. Academic Search Complete yielded 953 and Lexis-Nexis 998 subject results under the term.2 Indeed, the ubiquitous nature of wishful thinking suggests that we, as humans, at least partially allow a place for wishful thinking in the explanation of why a specific decisional option is chosen over others. Wishful thinking, however, is part of a larger study of motivated biases. The operational definition of motivated bias suggests the human process has some innate bias that causes a product of decision making to be influenced by those biases. Research into motivated bias has generally been categorized as hot cognition as opposed to questions of perceptions and beliefs which are typically seen as cold cognition. The purpose of this paper is to theoretically explore the plausibility that human decision making - at the level of agent, process, and product - is influenced by the so-called hot cognitive factors of motivated bias in the form of preferences held by the decision maker. Consequently, explanations into decisions must acknowledge the potential role of hot cognitive factors. Thus, instead of a cold model of cognition, or even strictly hot model, I suggest
Recommended publications
  • The Gainesville Iguana March 2003 the Anti-War Case Norman
    The Gainesville Iguana March 2003 The Anti-War Case Norman Balabanian The US Administration's case for war on Iraq is an utter failure; it is based not on fact but on innuendo and rhetoric. The US charges that Iraq "threw out" UN weapons inspectors in 1998 and is now concealing Weapons of Mass Destruction. The absurdity of all this is enormous, yet no well-informed voices opposing such claims can be found in the major news media. American citizens are, thus, ruled out from informed participation in making life-and-death decisions affecting them. What are the facts? The UN weapons inspectors were not "thrown out", as repeatedly claimed; rather - with just a few days notice - they were hastily withdrawn by the UN because the US and Britain warned that they planned to bomb Iraq! Which they did, and have continued ever since - an action not sanctioned by the UN and contrary to international law. Among the casualties of US mendacity in this regard are two courageous international public servants: Denis Haliday, former Asst Secy Gen of the UN and Humanitarian Coordinator in Iraq until 1998, who resigned following US bombing; and his successor, Hans von Sponeck, who resigned a year later under pressure from the US. Why would the US want a UN official who was acting in a humanitarian capacity removed? Was he perhaps taking his humanitarian concerns too seriously and complaining about the humanitarian disaster that US-supported UN sanctions were causing -- to that date more than half a million Iraqi children killed by the embargo imposed against Iraq? It is true that Saddam Hussein is a brutal dictator who has "gassed his own people" and "attacked his neighbors".
    [Show full text]
  • B-133258 Issues Related to U.S. Military Sales and Assistance to Iran
    J REPORT TO THE CONGRESS \ Issues Related To U. S. Military Sales And Assistance To Iran E -733258 Department of Defense Department of State BY THE COMPTROLLER GENERAL OF THE UNITED STATES ._ COMPTROLLER GENERAL OF THE UNITED STATES WASHINGTON. n.c. 20~413 5-133 258 4 b a, To the Speaker of the House of Representatives and the President pro tempore of the Senate This is our report on issues related to U.S. military sales and assistance to Iran. The review was made in compliance with the Budget and Accounting Act, 1921 (31 U.S.C. 53), and the Accountincj and Auditing Act of 1950 (31 U.S.C. 57). Copies of this report are being sent to the Director, Office of Management and Budget; the President and Chairman of the Export-Import Bank of the United States; the Secretary of Defense; and the Secretary of State. the Comptroller General A__.of the United States - -- .~ -- .- .- CONTENTS -Page DIGEST i CHAPTER x 1 INTRODUCTION 1 Scope of review 2 *f 2 NATURE AND MAGNITUDE OF ARMS SALES TO IRAN 3 Nature of Iran's military buildup Nature of U.S. arms sales to Iran 5 Makeup._ of U.S. sales 7 Evolution of the U.S. military role in Iran 7 U.S. economic assistance to Iran 11 3 SALE OF TECHNICAL ADVISORY SERVICES TO IRAN 12 EVOlUtiOn of TAFT 13 TAFT relationship to MAAG 15 some skills sold are in short supply in U.S. forces 17 Conclusions 17 GAO proposal . 18 Agency comments 18 4 RATIONALE FOR U.S.
    [Show full text]
  • The Psychometrics of Everyday Life
    COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 18, 195-224 (1986) The Psychometrics of Everyday Life ZIVA KUNDA Princeton University AND RICHARD E. NISBETT We examined people’s ability to assess everyday life correlations such as the degree of agreement that exists for various kinds of evaluations and the degree of consistency that characterizes social behavior from occasion to occasion. We found substantial accuracy for correlation estimates if two conditions were met: (1) subjects were highly familiar with the data in question and (2) the data were highly “codable,” that is, capable of being unitized and interpreted clearly. We generally found extreme inaccuracy if either of these conditions was not met. Subjects were particularly inaccurate about correlations involving social be- havior: They severely overestimated the stability of behavior across occasions. In addition, even subjects who were statistically sophisticated showed limited ap- preciation of the nggregarion principle, that is, the rule that the magnitude of a correlation increases with the number of units of evidence on which observations are based. 0 1986 Academic Press. Inc. Jane liked the movie; will you? Bill and you have served on several committees and he has always seemed very fair and very agreeable; would he make a good chairman? Our answers to such questions guide the conduct of our daily lives. Everything from the degree of pleasure to be expected from life’s minor diversions to the degree of success to be expected for lift’s major enterprises depends on the accuracy of our an- swers. Logically, answers to such questions rest on our beliefs about correla- tions, for example, correlations between different raters’ evaluations of movies or correlations between fairness and agreeableness in different The research reported here was supported by Grant NSF SES-8218846 from the National Science Foundation and 1 R01 MH38466-01 from the National Institute of Mental Health.
    [Show full text]
  • The Overconfidence Effect and IT Professionals Glen Van Der Vyver University of Southern Queensland, [email protected]
    Association for Information Systems AIS Electronic Library (AISeL) European Conference on Information Systems ECIS 2004 Proceedings (ECIS) 2004 The Overconfidence Effect and IT Professionals Glen Van Der Vyver University of Southern Queensland, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: http://aisel.aisnet.org/ecis2004 Recommended Citation Der Vyver, Glen Van, "The Overconfidence Effect and IT Professionals" (2004). ECIS 2004 Proceedings. 163. http://aisel.aisnet.org/ecis2004/163 This material is brought to you by the European Conference on Information Systems (ECIS) at AIS Electronic Library (AISeL). It has been accepted for inclusion in ECIS 2004 Proceedings by an authorized administrator of AIS Electronic Library (AISeL). For more information, please contact [email protected]. THE OVERCONFIDENCE EFFECT AND IT PROFESSIONALS Van Der Vyver, Glen, University of Southern Queensland, Department of Information Systems, West Street, 4350 Toowoomba, Australia, [email protected] Abstract Information Technology has become a core activity in most, if not all, corporations. Although IT managers continue to be under-represented in executive management, the very centrality of information technology to corporate success or failure suggests that this situation cannot endure. It has been suggested that information technology managers are too narrowly focused and technically oriented for strategic roles in the corporation. A key skill required of the executive relates to making decisions in situations imbued with uncertainty. This paper examines a cognitive process, the overconfidence effect, which has been shown to influence good decision making. An analysis is presented as to the susceptibility of IT professionals to the overconfidence effect when compared to accounting / finance and marketing professionals.
    [Show full text]
  • The Moral Psychology of Conflicts of Interest: Insights
    Journal of Applied Philosophy, Vol. 24, No. 4, 2007 TheBlackwellOxford,JJournal0264-3758©XXXOriginalPAPPaul Society MoralThagard UKofArticles Publishing forAppliedPsychology Applied Philosophy Ltd Philosophy, of ConflictsMoral 2007 of Interest Psychology of Conflicts of Interest: Insights from Affective Neuroscience PAUL THAGARD ABSTRACT This paper is an investigation of the moral psychology of decisions that involve a conflict of interest. It draws on the burgeoning field of affective neuroscience, which is the study of the neurobiology of emotional systems in the brain. I show that a recent neurocomputational model of how the brain integrates cognitive and affective information in decision-making can help to answer some important descriptive and normative questions about the moral psychology of conflicts of interest. These questions include: Why are decisions that involve conflicts of interest so common? Why are people so often unaware that they are acting immorally as the result of conflicts of interest? What is the relation of conflicts of interest to other kinds of irrationality, especially self-deception and weakness of will? What psychological, social, and logical steps can be taken to reduce the occurrence of immoral decisions resulting from conflicts of interest? I discuss five strategies for dealing with conflicts of interest: avoidance, optimal reasoning patterns, disclosure, social oversight, and understanding of neuropsychological processes. 1. Introduction Conflicts of interest arise when people make decisions biased by their personal goals, neglecting responsibilities to consider the interests of others. Such conflicts are ubiquitous, as in the following examples: •A government official who has been lavishly entertained by a lobbyist recommends that a lucrative contract go to the company represented by the lobbyist.
    [Show full text]
  • The Art of Thinking Clearly
    For Sabine The Art of Thinking Clearly Rolf Dobelli www.sceptrebooks.co.uk First published in Great Britain in 2013 by Sceptre An imprint of Hodder & Stoughton An Hachette UK company 1 Copyright © Rolf Dobelli 2013 The right of Rolf Dobelli to be identified as the Author of the Work has been asserted by him in accordance with the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means without the prior written permission of the publisher, nor be otherwise circulated in any form of binding or cover other than that in which it is published and without a similar condition being imposed on the subsequent purchaser. A CIP catalogue record for this title is available from the British Library. eBook ISBN 978 1 444 75955 6 Hardback ISBN 978 1 444 75954 9 Hodder & Stoughton Ltd 338 Euston Road London NW1 3BH www.sceptrebooks.co.uk CONTENTS Introduction 1 WHY YOU SHOULD VISIT CEMETERIES: Survivorship Bias 2 DOES HARVARD MAKE YOU SMARTER?: Swimmer’s Body Illusion 3 WHY YOU SEE SHAPES IN THE CLOUDS: Clustering Illusion 4 IF 50 MILLION PEOPLE SAY SOMETHING FOOLISH, IT IS STILL FOOLISH: Social Proof 5 WHY YOU SHOULD FORGET THE PAST: Sunk Cost Fallacy 6 DON’T ACCEPT FREE DRINKS: Reciprocity 7 BEWARE THE ‘SPECIAL CASE’: Confirmation Bias (Part 1) 8 MURDER YOUR DARLINGS: Confirmation Bias (Part 2) 9 DON’T BOW TO AUTHORITY: Authority Bias 10 LEAVE YOUR SUPERMODEL FRIENDS AT HOME: Contrast Effect 11 WHY WE PREFER A WRONG MAP TO NO
    [Show full text]
  • After the Accords Anwar Sadat
    WMHSMUN XXXIV After the Accords: Anwar Sadat’s Cabinet Background Guide “Unprecedented committees. Unparalleled debate. Unmatched fun.” Letters From the Directors Dear Delegates, Welcome to WMHSMUN XXXIV! My name is Hank Hermens and I am excited to be the in-room Director for Anwar Sadat’s Cabinet. I’m a junior at the College double majoring in International Relations and History. I have done model UN since my sophomore year of high school, and since then I have become increasingly involved. I compete as part of W&M’s travel team, staff our conferences, and have served as the Director of Media for our college level conference, &MUN. Right now, I’m a member of our Conference Team, planning travel and training delegates. Outside of MUN, I play trumpet in the Wind Ensemble, do research with AidData and for a professor, looking at the influence of Islamic institutions on electoral outcomes in Tunisia. In my admittedly limited free time, I enjoy reading, running, and hanging out with my friends around campus. As members of Anwar Sadat’s cabinet, you’ll have to deal with the fallout of Egypt’s recent peace with Israel, in Egypt, the greater Middle East and North Africa, and the world. You’ll also meet economic challenges, rising national political tensions, and more. Some of the problems you come up against will be easily solved, with only short-term solutions necessary. Others will require complex, long term solutions, or risk the possibility of further crises arising. No matter what, we will favor creative, outside-the-box ideas as well as collaboration and diplomacy.
    [Show full text]
  • Biases in Research: Risk Factors for Non-Replicability in Psychotherapy and Pharmacotherapy Research
    Psychological Medicine, Page 1 of 12. © Cambridge University Press 2016 REVIEW ARTICLE doi:10.1017/S003329171600324X Biases in research: risk factors for non-replicability in psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy research F. Leichsenring1*†, A. Abbass2, M. J. Hilsenroth3, F. Leweke1, P. Luyten4,5, J. R. Keefe6, N. Midgley7,8, S. Rabung9,10, S. Salzer11,12 and C. Steinert1 1 Department of Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, Justus-Liebig-University Giessen, Giessen, Germany; 2 Department of Psychiatry, Dalhousie University, Centre for Emotions and Health, Halifax, NS, Canada; 3 The Derner Institute of Advanced Psychological Studies, Adelphi University, NY, USA; 4 Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Leuven, Klinische Psychologie (OE), Leuven, Belgium; 5 Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, London, UK; 6 Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA; 7 The Anna Freud Centre, London, UK; 8 Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, UCL, London, UK; 9 Department of Psychology, Alpen-Adria-Universität Klagenfurt, Universitätsstr, Klagenfurt, Austria; 10 Department of Medical Psychology, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany; 11 Clinic of Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, Georg-August-Universität Goettingen, Göttingen, Germany; 12 International Psychoanalytic University (IPU), Berlin, Germany Replicability of findings is an essential prerequisite of research. For both basic and clinical research, however, low rep- licability of findings has recently been reported. Replicability may be affected by research biases not sufficiently con- trolled for by the existing research standards. Several biases such as researcher allegiance or selective reporting are well-known for affecting results. For psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy research, specific additional biases may affect outcome (e.g.
    [Show full text]
  • Human Rights in Iran Under the Shah
    Case Western Reserve Journal of International Law Volume 12 Issue 1 Article 7 1980 Human Rights in Iran under the Shah Richard W. Cottam Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarlycommons.law.case.edu/jil Part of the International Law Commons Recommended Citation Richard W. Cottam, Human Rights in Iran under the Shah, 12 Case W. Res. J. Int'l L. 121 (1980) Available at: https://scholarlycommons.law.case.edu/jil/vol12/iss1/7 This Comment is brought to you for free and open access by the Student Journals at Case Western Reserve University School of Law Scholarly Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Case Western Reserve Journal of International Law by an authorized administrator of Case Western Reserve University School of Law Scholarly Commons. Volume 12, Number 1, Winter 1980 COMMENT Human Rights in Iran Under the Shah by Professor Richard W. Cottam* I. INTRODUCTION FOR ANY ADVOCATE of human rights, the events surrounding the Iranian revolution must be a source of continuing agony. But for any- one interested in gaining a sharper understanding of some of the basic issues concerning human rights, the dramatic developments in Iran should be highly instructive. The early summary executions in Iran and the later public trials conducted by revolutionary Islamic courts were properly condemned by western human rights advocates as failing to ap- proach the requirements of due process. Yet the great majority of those who were tried and executed were charged with terrible violations of the most elemental human rights; and the testimony of the accused, so rich in detail and so internally consistent as to be credible,1 tends to confirm the worst charges against the Shah's regime.
    [Show full text]
  • Khomeinism, the Islamic Revolution and Anti Americanism
    Khomeinism, the Islamic Revolution and Anti Americanism Mohammad Rezaie Yazdi A thesis submitted to the University of Birmingham For the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY School of Political Science and International Studies University of Birmingham March 2016 University of Birmingham Research Archive e-theses repository This unpublished thesis/dissertation is copyright of the author and/or third parties. The intellectual property rights of the author or third parties in respect of this work are as defined by The Copyright Designs and Patents Act 1988 or as modified by any successor legislation. Any use made of information contained in this thesis/dissertation must be in accordance with that legislation and must be properly acknowledged. Further distribution or reproduction in any format is prohibited without the permission of the copyright holder. Abstract The 1979 Islamic Revolution of Iran was based and formed upon the concept of Khomeinism, the religious, political, and social ideas of Ayatullah Ruhollah Khomeini. While the Iranian revolution was carried out with the slogans of independence, freedom, and Islamic Republic, Khomeini's framework gave it a specific impetus for the unity of people, religious culture, and leadership. Khomeinism was not just an effort, on a religious basis, to alter a national system. It included and was dependent upon the projection of a clash beyond a “national” struggle, including was a clash of ideology with that associated with the United States. Analysing the Iran-US relationship over the past century and Khomeini’s interpretation of it, this thesis attempts to show how the Ayatullah projected "America" versus Iranian national freedom and religious pride.
    [Show full text]
  • The Case of Sequential Testing James WB Elsey, Anna I. Filmer, Lott
    1 Don’t Forget the Psychology in Psychological Analyses: The Case of Sequential Testing James W.B. Elsey, Anna I. Filmer, Lotte E. Stemerding Author Note Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands This manuscript was supported by salaries and departmental funding from the University of Amsterdam, and by Prof. Dr Merel Kindt's ERC Advanced Grant 743263. 2 Abstract Sequential testing enables researchers to monitor and analyze data as it arrives, and decide whether or not to continue data collection depending on the results. Although there are approaches that can mitigate many statistical issues with sequential testing, we suggest that current discussions of the topic are limited by focusing almost entirely on the mathematical underpinnings of analytic approaches. An important but largely neglected assumption of sequential testing is that the data generating process under investigation remains constant across the experimental cycle. Without care, psychological factors may result in violations of this assumption when sequential testing is used: researchers’ behavior may be changed by the observation of incoming data, in turn influencing the process under investigation. We argue for the consideration of an ‘insulated’ sequential testing approach, in which research personnel remain blind to the results of interim analyses. We discuss different ways of achieving this, from automation to collaborative inter-lab approaches. As a practical supplement to the issues we raise, we introduce an evolving resource aimed at helping researchers navigate both the statistical and psychological pitfalls of sequential testing: the Sequential Testing Hub (www.sequentialtesting.com). The site includes a guide for involving an independent analyst in a sequential testing pipeline, an annotated bibliography of relevant articles covering statistical aspects of sequential testing, links to tools and tutorials centered around how to actually implement a sequential analysis in practice, and space for suggestions to help develop this resource further.
    [Show full text]
  • Walker Percy, Looking for the Right Happened in the Trevon Martin Hate Crime
    2013 Presented By The Pirate’s Alley Faulkner Society Photograph by Joséphine Sacabo Faith & The Search for Meaning As Inspiration for The Arts Published December 1, 2013, New Orleans, LA Guarantors Bertie Deming Smith & The Deming Foundation, Cathy Pierson & Charles Heiner Theodosia Nolan, Tia & James Roddy & Peter Tattersall Judith “Jude” Swenson In Memory of James Swenson Randy Fertel and the Ruth U. Fertel Foundation Joseph DeSalvo, Jr., Rosemary James & Faulkner House, Inc. Frank G. DeSalvo, Attorney The J.J. and Dr. Donald Dooley Fund: Samuel L. Steel, III, Administrator Pam Friedler Joséphine Sacabo & Dalt Wonk Louisiana Division of the Arts, Department of Culture, Recreation & Tourism The State Library of Louisiana & The Louisiana Festival of the Book The Louisiana State Museum Hotel Monteleone & The Monteleone Family: Anne Burr, Greer & David Monteleone, Denise Monteleone, Ruthie Monteleone Anne & Ron Pincus Diane Manning, Floyd McLamb, Courtenay McDowell & Richard Gregory Hartwig & Nancy Moss In Memory of Betty Moss, New Orleans Hispanic Heritage Foundation David Speights in Memory of Marti Speights Mary Freeman Wisdom Foundation, Joyce & Steve Wood Zemurray Foundation Good Friends Jennifer E. Adams; Barbara Arras; Barbara & Edwin Beckman; Deena Bedigian; John & Marcia Biguenet; C.J. Blanda; Roy Blount, Jr. & Joan Griswold; Angie Bowlin; Birchey Butler; Charles Butt; Hortensia Calvo; Batou & Patricia Chandler Cherie Chooljian; Jackie Clarkson; Ned Condini; Mary Len Costa; Moira Crone & Rodger Kamenetz; Jerri Cullinan & Juli Miller Hart; W. Brent Day; Susan de la Houssaye; Stephanie, Robin, & Joan Durant; Louis Edwards; James Farwell & Gay Lebreton;Madeline Fischer; Christopher Franzen, Patty Friedmann; Jon Geggenheimer; David & Sandra Groome; Douglas & Elaine Grundmeyer; Christine Guillory; Janet & Steve Haedicke; Michael Harold & Quinn Peeper; Ken Harper & David Evard; W.
    [Show full text]