The Development and Validation of the Diagnosis Impact Scale (DIS) a Self-Report Measure of the Effect of Psychiatric Diagnoses on Recipients

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Development and Validation of the Diagnosis Impact Scale (DIS) a Self-Report Measure of the Effect of Psychiatric Diagnoses on Recipients PSYCHD The development and validation of the Diagnosis Impact Scale (DIS) a self-report measure of the effect of psychiatric diagnoses on recipients O’Connor, Niamh Award date: 2020 General rights Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. • Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research. • You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain • You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal ? Take down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Download date: 29. Sep. 2021 The Development and Validation of the Diagnosis Impact Scale (DIS): A Self-Report Measure of the Effect of Psychiatric Diagnoses on Recipients By Niamh O’ Connor BA (Hons), H.Dip. Psychology A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of PsychD Department of Psychology University of Roehampton 2019 Acknowledgements I would like to thank my Director of Studies, Professor Mick Cooper, for his unwavering enthusiasm for this research. He was incredibly responsive and containing throughout. Moreover, he was someone I could be open with. In a project like this one, a student is faced with an infinite number of gaps in their knowledge and understanding, which can be very unnerving. Mick created an atmosphere in which I was comfortable to ask questions and address the gaps in my knowledge. He was a wonderfully reliable and supportive presence and I have learnt a huge amount from him. I would also like to thank my co-supervisor, Dr Gina Pauli for being so generous with her time and incredible expertise. She was extremely patient and always willing to help. Above all she helped me to trust my judgement during the research process, which was invaluable. Her kind frankness and approachability were hugely appreciated. Dr Jac Hayes, my initial co-supervisor, was very helpful in the earlier stages of this project. She was extremely generous with her time and knowledge and most of all, brought an enlivening sense of curiosity to the project. Dr Chris Evans, whom I consulted on numerous occasions during this research project, always responded with a spirit of generosity as well as huge warmth and enthusiasm for the project. I would like to thank Dr Nicholas Longpre, for his help with statistical analysis and for being so patient and generous with his time. I would also like to thank my friends for their fantastic support and belief in me. Similarly, my close friends from the PsychD course, for their encouragement and understanding. 1 My parents, who, through their hard work and love have made it possible for me to undertake this training and through their nature have given me perspective when I needed it. Finally, this research would not have been possible without the participants and the mental health organisations who notified them about the study, I was so heartened by their willingness to contribute to the research. 2 Abstract Background: Despite the dominance of psychiatric diagnosis within mental healthcare systems, there is no validated measure of diagnosis impact on those who receive them. Consequently, there is also a paucity of statistically generalisable research on the experience of receiving and living with a diagnosis. Aims: To develop a valid and reliable measure of the effect of psychiatric diagnosis on recipients, the Diagnosis Impact Scale (DIS), for clinical and research use. Method: Measure development protocols used included the generation of an item pool, expert rating of items, and Three Step Test Interviews. Psychometric properties of the DIS were investigated, including internal consistency, convergent validity, discriminant validity, and criterion validity. A principal components analysis was carried out on a sample of 248 people with psychiatric diagnoses, followed by a two-parameter IRT analysis. Findings: The principal components analysis suggested that the 19-item DIS had two discrete subscales: Helpfulness of Diagnosis and Diagnosis-Related Self-Stigma. Each displayed excellent reliability in this sample, with Cronbach’s alphas of 0.93 (n=256) and 0.81 (n=263) for Helpfulness of Diagnosis and Diagnosis-Related Self-Stigma respectively. They were also found to have acceptable content and construct validity. Significant associations were found between both subscales and diagnosis type, perceived correct diagnosis, receipt of treatment due to diagnosis, and helpfulness of said treatment. Helpfulness of Diagnosis was significantly associated with age and time since diagnosis. Diagnosis-Related Self-Stigma was significantly associated with having multiple diagnoses, the number of these, and the type of healthcare professional who gave the diagnosis (e.g. GP or psychiatrist). The two-parameter IRT analysis showed 3 that both subscales were within acceptable ranges for discrimination and differentiation between strength of attitude. 4 Table of Content Acknowledgements p1 Abstract p3 Chapter 1: Introduction p10 1.1. Introduction to Psychiatric Diagnosis p10 1.1.1. Definition of psychiatric diagnosis. p10 1.1.2. History of psychiatric classification and diagnosis. p11 1.1.3. Diagnostic classifications. p14 1.1.4. Psychiatric diagnosis: the current context. p16 1.2. Academic Debates on Psychiatric Diagnosis p18 1.2.1. Reliability. p19 1.2.2. Validity. p20 1.2.3. Evidence for diagnostic categories. p21 1.3. Alternatives to Diagnosis p22 1.3.1. Person-centred therapy. p22 1.3.2. The Psychodynamic Diagnostic Manual. p22 1.3.3. Psychological formulation. p23 1.3.4. Power Threat Meaning Framework. p24 1.3.5. Psychosocial codes. p25 1.3.6. Phenomenological terminology. p26 1.4. Researcher Position on Psychological Distress, Psychiatric Disorders, p27 and Diagnosis Chapter 2: Literature Review p32 2.1. Introduction to Literature Review p32 2.2. Impact of Psychiatric Diagnosis on Service Users and Carers p33 2.2.1. Diagnosis and emotion. p33 2.2.1.1. Positive emotional impact of diagnosis. p33 2.2.1.2. Negative emotional impact of diagnosis. p34 5 2.2.2. Diagnosis and identity. p36 2.2.2.1. Negative impact on identity. p37 2.2.2.2. Positive impact on identity. p39 2.2.3. Diagnosis and power. p39 2.2.3.1. Diagnosis and disempowerment. p40 2.2.3.2. Diagnosis and agency. p41 2.2.3.3. Diagnosis and access to information. p42 2.2.4. Diagnosis and social life. p43 2.2.4.1. Stigma. p43 2.2.4.2. Impact of stigma. p44 2.2.4.3. Social benefits of diagnosis. p45 2.2.5. Diagnosis and treatment. p46 2.2.5.1. Positive impact of diagnosis on treatment. p46 2.2.5.2. Limited impact of diagnosis on treatment. p47 2.2.6. Diagnosis and finance. p48 2.2.7. Diagnosis and employment. p48 2.2.7. Diagnosis and education. p50 2.3. Factors Affecting the Impact of Psychiatric Diagnosis p50 2.3.1. Diagnosis and communication. p50 2.3.1.1. Service user awareness of diagnosis. p51 2.3.1.2. Service user attitude to diagnosis communication. p51 2.3.1.3. Unsatisfactory communication. p52 2.3.1.4. Clinician reluctance to diagnose. p52 2.3.1.5. Hope and support. p53 2.3.1.6. Collaboration. p53 2.3.2. Diagnosis and Time. p54 2.3.2.1. Time since diagnosis. p54 2.3.2.2. Delays in receiving a diagnosis. p54 2.3.3. Diagnosis fit. p54 2.3.4. Multiple diagnoses. p55 2.3.5. Type of diagnosis. p55 2.4. Psychiatric Diagnosis and Counselling Psychology p57 6 2.5. Rationale for the Current Study p58 2.5.1. Contribution to research. p58 2.5.2. Clinical applications. p59 Chapter 3: Methods and Results p61 3.1. Ethical approval p61 3.2. Epistemology p61 3.2.1. Critical realism. p61 3.2.2. Summary. p66 3.3. Design p66 3.4. Study 1: Item Pool Development p66 3.4.1. Creation of the item pool. p66 3.4.2. Expert online rating. p69 3.4.3. Item selection. p71 3.4.4. Three-Step Test Interviews. p72 3.4.4.1. Piloting the Three-Step Test Interviews. p72 3.4.4.2. Three-Step Test Interview participants. p72 3.4.4.3. Measure. p73 3.4.4.4. Procedure. p73 3.4.4.5. Three-Step Test Interview results. p75 3.5. Study 2: Psychometric Exploration p77 3.5.1. Psychometric exploration study participants. p78 3.5.1.1. Sample size. p78 3.5.1.2. Target population. p79 3.5.1.3. Recruitment of participants. p80 3.5.1.4. Participant demographics. p81 3.5.3. Measures p83 3.5.3.1. The Diagnosis Impact Scale (DIS). p84 3.5.3.2. Criterion validity items. p84 3.5.3.3. The Patient Feedback on Consultation Skills p85 Questionnaire (PFC). 3.5.3.4. Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS). p86 3.5.3.5. Single-item diagnosis helpfulness measure. p87 7 3.5.3.6. Social Desirability Scale- SDS-17. p87 3.5.3.7. CORE-10. p88 3.5.3.8. Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). p89 3.5.4. Procedure. p90 3.5.5. Outline of the study p91 3.5.5.1. A priori aims. p91 3.5.5.2. Hypotheses. p91 3.5.6. Results of psychometric exploration study. p92 3.5.6.1. Outline of the analysis. p92 3.5.6.2.
Recommended publications
  • 6 the Pursuit of Laws
    UNIT 112 WHAT IS SOCIAL RESEARCH? 6 THE PURSUIT OF LAWS Another area of dispute in the social sciences has been between advocates of what are often termed nomothetic and idiographic approaches: between those who believe that social research is primarily concerned with discovering universal laws and those who regard its task as the production of knowledge about particu- lar situations (We came across this distinction in Section 4 when discussing the views of Max Weber.) The nomothetic approach is sometimes taken to be essential to scientific research. The goal here is often laws of the kind characteristic of physics whlch state invariant relationships among variables, for example Boyle's law claims that the volume of a gas in an enclosed space is inversely proportional to the pressure on it, given constant temperature. What are proposed are causal or functional relationships that hold universally, in all places and times where the conditions speckied in the law apply The social science discipline where the influence of the nomothetic approach has been strongest is psychology As we noted earlier, the foundations of modern experimental psychology were laid in the late nineteenth century in Germany A number of researchers, many of them trained in the natural sciences, sought to develop a scientific approach to understanding human perception and cognition. In setting up a form of psychology modelled on the natural sciences they came into direct conflict with the (at that time) much more prestigious discipline of his- tory which also regarded itself as concerned with human psychology, albeit the psychology of unique individuals - of the great figures of history.
    [Show full text]
  • Centennial Bibliography on the History of American Sociology
    University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Sociology Department, Faculty Publications Sociology, Department of 2005 Centennial Bibliography On The iH story Of American Sociology Michael R. Hill [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/sociologyfacpub Part of the Family, Life Course, and Society Commons, and the Social Psychology and Interaction Commons Hill, Michael R., "Centennial Bibliography On The iH story Of American Sociology" (2005). Sociology Department, Faculty Publications. 348. http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/sociologyfacpub/348 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Sociology, Department of at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. It has been accepted for inclusion in Sociology Department, Faculty Publications by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. Hill, Michael R., (Compiler). 2005. Centennial Bibliography of the History of American Sociology. Washington, DC: American Sociological Association. CENTENNIAL BIBLIOGRAPHY ON THE HISTORY OF AMERICAN SOCIOLOGY Compiled by MICHAEL R. HILL Editor, Sociological Origins In consultation with the Centennial Bibliography Committee of the American Sociological Association Section on the History of Sociology: Brian P. Conway, Michael R. Hill (co-chair), Susan Hoecker-Drysdale (ex-officio), Jack Nusan Porter (co-chair), Pamela A. Roby, Kathleen Slobin, and Roberta Spalter-Roth. © 2005 American Sociological Association Washington, DC TABLE OF CONTENTS Note: Each part is separately paginated, with the number of pages in each part as indicated below in square brackets. The total page count for the entire file is 224 pages. To navigate within the document, please use navigation arrows and the Bookmark feature provided by Adobe Acrobat Reader.® Users may search this document by utilizing the “Find” command (typically located under the “Edit” tab on the Adobe Acrobat toolbar).
    [Show full text]
  • The Rise and Domestication of Historical Sociology
    The Rise and Domestication of" Historical Sociology Craig Calhoun Historical sociology is not really new, though it has enjoyed a certain vogue in the last twenty years. In fact, historical research and scholarship (including comparative history) was central to the work of many of the founders and forerunners of sociology-most notably Max Weber but also in varying degrees Karl Marx, Emile Durkheim, and Alexis de Tocqueville among others. It was practiced with distinction more recently by sociologists as disparate as George Homans, Robert Merton, Robert Bellah, Seymour Martin Lipset, Charles Tilly, J. A. Banks, Shmuel Eisenstadt, Reinhard Bendix, Barrington Moore, and Neil Smelser. Why then, should historical sociology have seemed both new and controversial in the 1970s and early 1980s? The answer lies less in the work of historical sociologists themselves than in the orthodoxies of mainstream, especially American, sociology of the time. Historical sociologists picked one battle for themselves: they mounted an attack on modernization theory, challenging its unilinear developmental ten- dencies, its problematic histori<:al generalizations and the dominance (at least in much of sociology) of culture and psycllology over political economy. In this attack, the new generation of historical sociologists challenged the most influential of their immediate forebears (and sometimes helped to create the illusion that historical sociology was the novel invention of the younger gener- ation). The other major battle was thrust upon historical sociologists when many leaders of the dominant quantitative, scientistic branch of the discipline dismissed their work as dangerously "idiographic," excessively political, and in any case somehow not quite 'real' sociology. Historical sociology has borne the marks of both battles, and in some sense, like an army always getting ready to fight the last war, it remains unnecessarily preoccupied with them.
    [Show full text]
  • An Overview of the Anthropological Theories
    International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Vol. 4, No. 10(1); August 2014 An Overview of the Anthropological Theories Nurazzura Mohamad Diah Head Department of Sociology & Anthropology International Islamic University Malaysia Dewan Mahboob Hossain Associate Professor Department of Accounting & Information Systems University of Dhaka Bangladesh Sohela Mustari PhD Student Department of Sociology & Anthropology International Islamic University Malaysia Noor Syafika Ramli Master in Anthropology Universiti Kebangsaan Malaysia Abstract Theories are treated as the lifeblood of the disciplines like sociology and anthropology. As a newer discipline that has grown approximately over the last two hundred years, anthropology has proposed different important theories on man and culture. This article presents with an overview of the theories of anthropology developed over the last two hundred years. This can be considered as a general summarized reading of the important anthropological theories like evolutionism, diffusionism, historical particularism, functionalism, culture and personality, structuralism, neo-evolutionism, cultural ecology, cultural materialism, postmodernist and feminist explanations. This article concludes that though each of these theories was criticized by the subsequent theorists, all these theories contributed a lot in the development of anthropology as a discipline. Keywords: anthropology, theory, anthropological theory 1.0. Introduction In the academic arena, anthropology is considered as a relatively new discipline as its major development mainly happened in the nineteenth and the twentieth centuries. Though, in France and Germany, this discipline got a momentum in the seventeenth and the eighteenth centuries in different names (like ethnology, Volkskunde, Volkerkunde etc.), in English, the word ‘anthropology’ first appeared in the year of 1805 (McGee and Warms, 2012; 6).
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 1 FORMING HUMANITIES
    Chapter 1 FORMING HUMANITIES This story begins in 1636. When the first institution of higher education opened in this country, humanities played a central role. Harvard College was a Puritan-based institution with a prescribed curriculum based on the Bible, the classical trivium of language-oriented arts, and the quadrivium of mathematical or scientific arts. Students learned Greek, Aramaic, He- brew, and Syriac, along with logic and rhetoric, history and politics, as- tronomy and botany, ethics, and catechism. The philosophical complement to this common curriculum was the idea of unitary knowledge. Together, these structural and philosophical principles shaped the educational pro- grams of the other colonial colleges and, more generally, influenced think- ing about higher education in the United States for nearly two and a half centuries. The central mission of the American college was teaching in the European and English tradition. The students, who came primarily from the elite of colonial society, were trained to take their place in prestigious vocations. The faculty, who came primarily from the clergy, placed classical learning in a religious framework. Individuals offered recitations and lec- tures in specific subjects. However, they were not divided into separate schools or departments. A returning missionary might teach biology or a teacher of rhetoric do double duty in history, logic, or metaphysics (Levine and Niddifer 54, 66–67; Hutcheson 105; D. Bennett 132–33; Bogue and Aper 19–20; Kuklick, “Professionalization” 43). Even though common principles would have a long-lasting impact, dif- ferences were already evident by the founding of the second colonial col- lege in 1692.
    [Show full text]
  • Emotions in the Polish Peasant… Researcher’S Reminiscences Based on the Autobiography of Władek
    PRZEGLĄD SOCJOLOGICZNY 2019 68(4): 75–98 ISSN 0033-2356; e-ISSN 2450-9351 https://doi.org/10.26485/PS/2019/68.4/4 MaREk NOwak Uniwersytet adama Mickiewicza PIOtR LUCZYS Public Profits Sp. z o.o. EMOTIONS IN THE POLISH PEASANT… RESEARCHER’s REMINISCENCES BASED on the autobiography of Władek Abstract the article aims to answer the questions about the specificity of the presence of emotions in the classic autobiography of władek, published in: The Polish Peasant in Europe and America (1918), by william thomas and Florian Znaniecki, a work which has inspired researchers for a hundred years. the authors presented the issues from three different perspectives: (a) theoretical assumptions of sociology by Znaniecki and thomas, (b) cognitive orientation rooted in research questions and the image of the world of the early twentieth century, and (c) issues of autobiographical analysis interpreted in conjunction with contemporary assumptions of analysis formulated by Fritz Schütze. apart from questions strictly related to the work of thomas and Znaniecki, the analysis also includes elements of interpretation of the contemporary evolution of sociology of emotions, leading to the formulation of a simplified description of the evolution of biographical analysis. the conclusions aim to make an instrumental treatment of emotions both in the traditional thomas and Znaniecki texts and in the contemporary example. this suggests, among other things, a certain level of incompatibility of studies on emotions to classical and selected contemporary research studies, but also allows us to conclude PhD, Instytut Socjologii; e-mail: [email protected] (Post-doctoral degree.); https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1749-7669.
    [Show full text]
  • Nomothetic Science and Idiographic History in Twentieth-Century Americanist Anthropology
    Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, Vol. 40(1), 77–96 Winter 2004 Published online in Wiley Interscience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI 10.1002 /jhbs.10180 © 2004 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. NOMOTHETIC SCIENCE AND IDIOGRAPHIC HISTORY IN TWENTIETH-CENTURY AMERICANIST ANTHROPOLOGY R. LEE LYMAN AND MICHAEL J. O’BRIEN For over a century, Americanist anthropologists have argued about whether their disci- pline is a historical one or a scientific one. Proponents of anthropology as history have claimed that the lineages of human cultures are made up of unique events that cannot be generalized into laws. If no laws can be drawn, then anthropology cannot be a science. Proponents of anthropology as science have claimed that there indeed are laws that gov- ern humans and their behaviors and cultures, and these laws can be discovered. Interestingly, both sides have the same narrow view of what science is. The same sorts of debates over science and history were played out in evolutionary biology over a half-cen- tury ago, and what emerged was the view that that discipline and its sister discipline, pa- leontology, were both history and science—hence the term “historical sciences.” Anthropology and its sister discipline, archaeology, have only recently begun to realize that they too are historical sciences. © 2004 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. In 1989, Stephen Jay Gould (p. 51) observed, “Science has been slow to admit the dif- ferent explanatory world of history into its domain—and our interpretations have been im- poverished by this omission. Science has also tended to denigrate history ... by regarding any invocation of contingency as less elegant or less meaningful than explanations based directly on timeless ‘laws of nature.’” During the twentieth century, anthropologists trained in North America—what we term Americanist anthropologists, including archaeologists—have argued with some regularity about whether their discipline is a historical one or a scientific one (Boas, 1936; Kroeber, 1935; Trigger, 1973; Watson, 1973a).
    [Show full text]
  • D2.1 Redefining What Data Is and the Terms We Use to Speak of It
    D2.1 Redefining what data is and the terms we useto speak of it. Jennifer Edmond, Georgina Nugent Folan To cite this version: Jennifer Edmond, Georgina Nugent Folan. D2.1 Redefining what data is and the terms we useto speak of it.. [Research Report] Trinity College Dublin. 2018. hal-01842385 HAL Id: hal-01842385 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01842385 Submitted on 18 Jul 2018 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. H2020-ICT-2016-1-732340 Deliverable Title: D2.1 Redefining what data is and the terms we use to speak of it. Deliverable Date: 31st March 2018 Version: V2.0 Project Acronym : KPLEX Project Title: Knowledge Complexity Funding Scheme: H2020-ICT-2016-1 Grant Agreement number: 732340 Project Coordinator: Dr. Jennifer Edmond ([email protected]) Project Management Michelle Doran ([email protected]) Contact: Project Start Date: 01 January 2017 Project End Date: 31 March 2018 WP No.: 2 WP Leader: Dr Jennifer Edmond, TCD Authors and Contributors Dr Jennifer Edmond ([email protected]) (Name and email address): Dr Georgina Nugent-Folan ([email protected]) Dissemination Level: PU Nature of Deliverable: R = report 1 H2020-ICT-2016-1-732340 Abstract: How do we define data? Through a series of interviews with computer scientists and a text mining exercise, this report presents the investigations into the how data is defined and conceived of by computer scientists.
    [Show full text]
  • Individualised Or Standardised Outcome Measures: a Co-Habitation?
    CORE Metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk Provided by University of Lincoln Institutional Repository Administration and Policy in Mental Health and Mental Health Services Research (2019) 46:425–428 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10488-019-00928-z POINT OF VIEW Individualised or Standardised Outcome Measures: A Co-habitation? Mark Ashworth1 · Daniel Guerra2 · Maria Kordowicz1 Published online: 5 March 2019 © The Author(s) 2019 Abstract Mental health outcome measurement is conflicted between two different schools of thought which underlie the division between standardised (nomothetic) and individualised or patient-generated (idiographic) measures. The underpinning philoso- phies of both approaches have very different starting points in terms of how we understand the world. And yet the strengths of both may contribute something useful for patients and mental health services. We suggest a convergence of approaches with new thinking on options for co-habitation. Keywords Outcome measurement · Patient-generated measure · Standardized measure · Epistomology Introduction knowledge. Similarly, idiographic knowledge also required access to nomothetic methods. His argument was for philo- There is a divide at the heart of mental health outcome meas- sophical and scientific paradigms to move towards a more urement. The divide is driven by epistemology, the branch holistic understanding of human and social phenomena. of philosophy concerned with the theory of knowledge, how The American psychologist Allport is generally credited we know things, the nature of knowledge and its limitations. with introducing the terms, ‘nomothetic’ and ‘idiographic’, The German philosopher Immanuel Kant, in his Cri- into the discipline of psychology. The ideas were developed tique of Pure Reason (1781), described two distinct forms in his theories of personality: ‘Personality: A psychologi- of knowledge.
    [Show full text]
  • Capturing the Unique Nature of the Human Sciences: the Second Demarcation Problem
    Behavior and Philosophy, 47, 15-33 (2019). © 2019 Cambridge Center for Behavioral Studies 15 CAPTURING THE UNIQUE NATURE OF THE HUMAN SCIENCES: THE SECOND DEMARCATION PROBLEM William T. O’Donohue Kenneth Burleigh Alexander D. Kinzler University of Nevada, Reno ABSTRACT: As Meehl (1978) noted four decades ago, psychology has progressed at a significantly slower rate than the natural sciences. This paper argues that one factor that may account for this discrepancy in problem solving progress is that the scientific methods used by psychologists have not properly taken into account unique characteristics of humans that the natural sciences need not consider. This issue has been called “the second demarcation problem” i.e., that a distinction between the methods of the natural sciences (the Naturwissenshaften) and the human sciences (the Geisteswissenshaften) -- needs to be made in order to properly understand the research methods that are necessary for scientific progress in each. This paper describes four such unique characteristics of humans as justification for this second demarcation: 1) radical idiography, 2) phenomenological experience, 3) the possibility of free will, and 4) radically new knowledge. Key words: Idiography, Phenomenology, Free Will, Research, Methodology, Psychological Science1 Paul Meehl’s 1978 seminal article, “Theoretical Risks and Tabular Asterisks: Sir Karl, Sir Ronald, and the Slow Progress of Soft Psychology” argued that the science of psychology has progressed at a much slower rate than the physical sciences. For instance, what scientific problem in clinical psychology is settled to the degree that say, motion or electricity is in the natural sciences? In a word, none -- even though the laws of motion and electricity in physics were uncovered centuries ago.
    [Show full text]
  • Merton, Robert K. (1968) Social Theory and Social Structure. New York: the Free Press Table of Contents in Word
    Merton, Robert K. (1968) Social Theory and Social Structure. New York: The Free Press Table of contents in word Merton, Robert K. (1968) Social Theory and Social Structure. New York: The Free Press1 Table of contents in word....................................................................................................1 Note on layout .....................................................................................................................2 SOCIAL THEORY AND SOCIAL STRUCTURE............................................................2 PREFACE TO THE 1968 ENLARGED EDITION ...........................................................3 PREFACE TO THE 1957 REVISED EDITION ................................................................5 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS...................................................................................................6 CONTENTS -AN OVERVIEW..........................................................................................7 Part I ON THEORETIC SOCIAL THEORY AND SOCIAL STRUCTURE..................23 I ON THE HISTORY AND SYSTEMATICS OF SOCIOLOGICAL THEORY........23 II ON SOCIOLOGICAL THEORIES OF THE MIDDLE RANGE ............................59 III MANIFEST AND LATENT FUNCTIONSTOWARD THE CODIFICATION OF FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS IN SOCIOLOGY FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS............92 iv THE BEARING OF SOCIOLOGICAL THEORY ON EMPIRICAL RESEARCH .158 V THE BEARING OF EMPIRICAL RESEARCH ON SOCIOLOGICAL THEORY174 Part II STUDIES IN SOCIAL AND CULTURAL STUCTURE INTRODUCTION....189 VI SOCIAL STRUCTURE AND ANOMIE ..............................................................198
    [Show full text]
  • A Diversity of Divisions: Tracing the History of the Demarcation Between
    A Diversity of Divisions: Tracing the FOCUS History of the Demarcation between the Sciences and the Humanities Jeroen Bouterse, Leiden University Bart Karstens, University of Amsterdam Abstract: Throughout history, divides between the sciences and the humanities have been drawn in many different ways. This essay shows that the notion of a divide became more urgent and pronounced in the second half of the nineteenth century. While this shift has several causes, the essay focuses on the rise of the social sciences, which is interpreted as posing a profound challenge to the established disciplines of the study of humankind. This is demonstrated by zooming in on linguistics, one of the key traditional disciplines of the humanities. Through the assumption of a correspon- dence between mental and linguistic categories, psychology became of central im- portance in the various conceptions of linguistics that emerged in the nineteenth century. Both linguistics and psychology were very much engaged in a process of discipline formation, and opinions about the proper directions of the fields varied considerably. Debates on these issues catalyzed the construction of more radical divisions between the sciences and the humanities. Both Wilhelm Dilthey’s dichot- omy between understanding and explanation and Wilhelm Windelband’s dichotomy between nomothetic and idiographic sciences respond to these debates. While their constructions are often lumped together, the essay shows that they actually meant very different things and have to be treated accordingly. n the past few years, awareness has grown that the history of the humanities is an integral I part of the history of science, which, for our purposes here, can also be thought of as the history of knowledge.
    [Show full text]