Key to Activities and Reviews

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Key to Activities and Reviews Key to Activities and Reviews Chapter 3 Speech Sounds Key to Activities Activity 7 We ask people to say ‘cheese’ before taking a photograph because the vowel sound ‘ee’ requires the lips to be spread very wide thus producing a sort of ‘smile’. Activity 10 Tooth-ridge: back part Nasal Cavity front part Hard Palate Upper Teeth Velum Upper Lip Nasal Passage Lower Lip Tongue: Lower Teeth back middle Jaw front Vocal Cords tip Figure 3.7 Speech articulators – annotated © Pamela Rogerson-Revell 2011 Rogerson-Revell, P. (2011) English Phonology and Pronunciation Teaching. London: Continuum. Chapter 4 Consonants Key to Consonant Practice Activities Activity 1 Place of articulation Bilabial Labiodental dental Alveolar Palato-alveolar Palatal Velar Glottal PLOSIVE p b t d k g FRICATIVE f v θ ð s z ʃʒ h AFFRICATE ʧ ʤ NASAL m n ŋ LATERAL l Manner of articulation APPROXIMANT ω rj Activity 2 (i) a voiced bilabial plosive bit (ii) a voiced labio-dental fricative veer (iii) a voiceless palato-alveolar affricate chin (iv) a voiceless alveolar plosive tight (v) a voiced bilabial nasal might (vi) a voiced bilabial approximant which can be called a semi-vowel also weir (vii) a voiceless labio-dental fricative fare (viii) a voiced dental fricative then (ix) a voiceless palato-alveolar fricative sure (x) a voiced velar plosive got Activity 3 (i) television = voiced palato-alveolar fricative (ii) shaking = voiceless velar plosive (iii) badger = voiced palato-alveolar affricate (iv) father = voiced dental fricative (v) dung = voiced velar nasal (vi) pushing = voiceless palato-alveolar fricative (vii) solid = voiceless alveolar lateral approximant (viii) regard = voiced velar plosive (ix) patchy = voiceless palato-alveolar affricate (x) reveal = voiced labio-dental fricative © Pamela Rogerson-Revell 2011 Rogerson-Revell, P. (2011) English Phonology and Pronunciation Teaching. London: Continuum. Activity 4 white /w/ /t/ clock /k/ /l/ / /k/ edge /ʤ/ pull /p/ /l/ through /θ/ /r/ vision /v/ /ʒ/ /n/ chalk /ʧ/ /k/ shell /ʃ/ /l/ finger /f/ /ŋ/ /g/ boxes /b/ /k/ /s/ /z/ fifth /f/ /f/ /θ/ wrong /r/ /ŋ/ queue /k/ /j/ cares /k/ /z/ anger /ŋ/ /g/ Activity 5 Then, the three young boys hid in the bushes until their pursuers passed by. At one point, the youngest began to groan in fear. The other two threw themselves on top of him and smothered the noise. The danger passed. /ð/ is very common in initial position in function words such as ‘then’, ‘this’, ‘the’ and tends to occur intervocalically in content words like ‘smother’, ‘mother’, ‘father’. /θ/ often occurs in initial position in content words such as ‘throw’, ‘thin’, ‘think’, ‘thought’. Activity 6 /h/ cannot occur in final position in a word. /ŋ/ can only occur in final and medial position. /ʒ/ in ‘measure’ generally occurs in medial, intervocalic position, except for a few ‘loan’ words such as ‘garage’, ‘rouge’, ‘beige’. /r/ does not occur in final position in RP. Chapter 5 Vowels Key to Activities Activity 1 (a) /u:/ is a back vowel (b) / i:/ is a front vowel (c) /ə/ is a central vowel © Pamela Rogerson-Revell 2011 Rogerson-Revell, P. (2011) English Phonology and Pronunciation Teaching. London: Continuum. Activity 2 Front Central Back /i:/ /ɪ/ /e/ / æ / /ə/ /ʌ/ /ɜ:/ /u:/ /ɔ:/ /ʊ/ /ɒ/ Activity 3 Ideally, you would recognize that the front and back vowels are presented in a descending degree of closeness. If you have plotted them in the following way, you can begin to see the relationships they have to parts of the mouth: iː uː ɪ ʊ front back vowels vowels e ɔː æ ɒ ɑː Activity 25 iː uː ɪ ʊ ə e ɜː ɔː æ ʌ ɒ ɑː © Pamela Rogerson-Revell 2011 Rogerson-Revell, P. (2011) English Phonology and Pronunciation Teaching. London: Continuum. Key to Vowel Practice Activities Activity 1 Examples are: got, sit, seat, sort, sat, gate, goat, part, put, pool, cup, ago, fur, sight, toy, fear, fair, get, gout, poor. Activity 3 ABCD 1 live love love love 2 bed bed bud bed 3 ran run ran ran 4 barn barn barn bun 5 hot hut hot hot 6 born bun born born 7 walk walk work walk 8 room rum room room 9 tale tale tale tell 10 rat right right right 11 john join join join 12 knee knee near knee 13 very vary very very 14 tow tow two tow 15 hum home home home 16 ground grand ground ground 17 hurt hurt hurt hut 18 pat pat pot pat 19 said said sad said 20 gate get get get Activity 4 (1) /ɒ/ /eɪ/ /ɪ/ (2) / ʌ/ /æ/ /ɒ/ (3) /ɪ/ /aɪ/ /ʊ/ (4) /ɔ:/ /ɑ:/ /e/ © Pamela Rogerson-Revell 2011 Rogerson-Revell, P. (2011) English Phonology and Pronunciation Teaching. London: Continuum. Activity 4—Cont'd (5) /ɜ:/ /ɑ:/ /e/ (6) /u:/ /e/ /eɪ/ (7) /ɪə/ /ɔɪ/ /əʊ/ (8) /aɪ/ /ɔɪ/ /i:/ (9) /aɪ/ /əʊ/ /eɪ/ (10) /ɔɪ/ /eɪ/ /aɪ/ (11) /æ/ /ɑ:/ /u:/ /ʌ/ (12) /e/ /əʊ/ /ɪ/ (13) /ɔ:/ /ɜ:/ /ɑ:/ (14) /ɪ/ /i:/ /ɔɪ/ (15) /ʊə/ /eə/ /ɪə/ Activity 5 (a) cat /æ/ seal /i:/ cool /u:/ share /eə/ hill /ɪ/ beer /ɪə/ walk /ɔ:/ feel /i:/ bin /ɪ/ sank /æ/ fur /ɜ:/ why /aɪ/ far /ɑ:/ bird /ɜ:/ shot /ɒ/ broad /ɔ:/ shore /ɔ:/ cut /ʌ/ send /e/ few /u:/ pull /ʊ/ (b) 1. /u:/ in ‘soon’ – long, close, back, rounded 2. /ɪ/ in ‘hill’ – short, close, front, neutral 3. /ɒ/ in ‘hot’ – short, open, back, rounded 4. /ɜ:/ in ‘work’ – long, mid-open, central, neutral 5. /i:/ in ‘feet’ – long, close, front, spread Activity 6 window /ɪ/ /əʊ/ roses /əʊ/ ɪ/ information /ɪ/ /ə/ /eɪ/ /ə/ woman /ʊ/ /ə/ apples /æ/ /ə/ tower /aʊə/ women /ɪ/ /ə/ usual /u:/ (/u/) /ə/ fi re /aɪə/ gentlemen /e/ /ə/ /ə/ secretary /e/ /ə/ (/ə/) /i/ idea /aɪ/ /ɪə/ © Pamela Rogerson-Revell 2011 Rogerson-Revell, P. (2011) English Phonology and Pronunciation Teaching. London: Continuum. Activity 7 a. /aɪ/ (dive) e. /eɪ/ (pain) b. /ɔɪ/ (coin) f. /ɪə/ (near) c. /eə/ (hair) g. /ʊə/ (pure) d. /aʊ/ (now) h. /əʊ/ (home) Chapter 6 Phonemes in Context Key to Activities Activity 1 chop ship not minimal pair Two phonemes (/ʃ/ v /ʧ/ and /ɒ/ v /ɪ/ are distinguished rather than one right rat minimal pair /aɪ/ v /æ/ are different phonemes muggy mucky minimal pair /g/ v /k/ are different phonemes leisure lesser minimal pair /ʒ/ v /s/ are different phonemes pool pull minimal pair /u:/ v /ʊ/ are different phonemes other author not minimal pair Two phonemes /ʌ/ v /ɔ:/ and /δ/ v /θ/ are distinguished rather than one Activity 2 In (a) the [k] is heavily aspirated, unlike the other three examples. In (c) in final position, voiceless plosives such as [k] are regularly unexploded, or only partly exploded. So the release stage may be inaudible. Activity 3 fitness fɪt ̚nəs picnic pɪk ̚nɪk tractor træk ̚tə hot dog hɒt ̚dɒg midday mɪd ̚deɪ © Pamela Rogerson-Revell 2011 Rogerson-Revell, P. (2011) English Phonology and Pronunciation Teaching. London: Continuum. Activity 4 Underline the position of potential glottal stops in the following phrases: (1) Got that? (2) Are you quite sure? (3) He’s a fitness instructor. (4) Where’s the kitten gone? Activity 5 The vowels in the stressed syllables in column 1 are shorter than in columns 2 and 3 as they precede a voiceless plosive, fricative or affricate: Short vowel long vowel long vowel neat knee need wheat we weed light lie lied piece pea peas safe say save Activity 6 (i) /p/ in ‘pig’ is heavily aspirated, unlike the other three examples, while /b/ in ‘big’ is not aspirated (ii) neither /t/ nor /d/ is aspirated in mid position but /d/ is voiced and /t/ is voiceless. The preceding vowel is lengthened before the lenis consonant /d/. (iii) neither plosive is aspirated in final position and voiceless plosives such as [p] are regularly unex- ploded, or only partly exploded. So the release stage may be inaudible. The preceding vowel is lengthened before the lenis consonant /g/. Activity 7 ‘Bill heard the loud bell just as light fell.’ © Pamela Rogerson-Revell 2011 Rogerson-Revell, P. (2011) English Phonology and Pronunciation Teaching. London: Continuum. Activity 8 The /r/ is pronounced in the underlined cases: 1 2 3 rare rarely rarer ear earplug earring tear terror terrorist fear fearful fearing Activity 9 traitor danger character tomorrow forget accurate alone organize appearance Activity 10 many, funny, actually, misery, usual, poverty meni, fʌni, æktjuəli (or æktʃuəli), mɪzəri, ju:ʒuəl, pɒvəti Key to Phoneme Practice Activities 1. (a) been (b) fill (c) cash (d) shoot mess pool thing jam full bird then hung fat harm yacht sheep much cot chuck June (e) joke (f) mouth (g) bike (h) arrived wear shake pace aloud size fierce fair private boil show voice carvings wierd choke shoulder morning © Pamela Rogerson-Revell 2011 Rogerson-Revell, P. (2011) English Phonology and Pronunciation Teaching. London: Continuum. (i) joking (j) forest willful shallow woollen mountain easy finished derby forgot 2. (i) (ii) (iii) (a) fox /fɒx/ /fɒks/ /fɔ:ks/ (b) June /ˈdu:n/ /ˈʤu:n/ /ˈʤʌn/ (c) double /ˈdʌbl/ /ˈdʊbel/ /ˈdu:bl/ (d) idea /ɪˈdɪə/ / aɪˈdɪə / /aɪˈdɪɑ/ (e) only /ˈonli:/ /ˈaunli:/ /ˈəʊnli/ (f) alarm /əˈlɑ:m/ /ɑˈ lɑ:m/ /æˈla:m/ (g) menu /ˈmenu:/ /ˈmenju:/ /ˈmenʊ/ (h) message /ˈmesæʤ/ /ˈmeseʤ/ /ˈmesɪʤ / 3. If you do not have a BBC English accent, you may have some differences in the vowels in words like ‘laugh’ (eg /læf/ rather than /lɑ:f/). (a) /kæb/ (b) /fi:l/ (c) /lɑ:f/ (d) /kæʧ/ (e) /vɔɪs/ (f) /feə/ (g) /hɑ:m/ (h) /ʧʌk/ (i) /fʊl/ (j) /ðen/ (k) /hʌŋ/ (l) /ʃu:t/ (m) /bɜ:d/ (n) /nju:z/ (o) /ˈmju:zɪk/ (p) /dɪʃ/ (q) /kju:/ (r) /ʤɔɪn/ 4.
Recommended publications
  • Teaching English Fricative Sounds to the Learners Whose Mother Tongue Is Hindi
    Research Journal of English Language and Literature (RJELAL) A Peer Reviewed (Refereed) International Journal Vol.3.Issue 1. 2015 http://www.rjelal.com RESEARCH ARTICLE TEACHING ENGLISH FRICATIVE SOUNDS TO THE LEARNERS WHOSE MOTHER TONGUE IS HINDI NIZAMUDDIN Research Scholar The English and Foreign Languages University, Lucknow Campus ABSTRACT Communication is one of the most important needs of human being. For having communication, verbal, we need a language. Language is a code which has many varies. The sender of the message encodes a message into a language and receiver of it decodes it. So language is a system of communication through speech. It is speech which comes first and writing comes later in an individual’s life. Today English is spoken in almost all over the world so it is considered as international link language and it is used with different accents in different parts of the world. Because of the differences English has many varieties as British English, American English, Canadian English and Indian English etc. English spoken in India radically differs from native English in terms of Article Info: grammar, vocabulary and even in pronunciation also. Now- a- days India is considered Article Received:20/01/2015 Revised on: 04/03/2015 as a developing country. English is a second language in India so the speakers find it Accepted on: 09/03/2015 difficult in reaching the perfection which a native speaker enjoys. This is seen vividly when it comes to pronouncing certain sounds of English for example English fricatives. The present paper deals with how English fricative learning is an instructional strategy.
    [Show full text]
  • A Contrastive Study of the Ibibio and Igbo Sound Systems
    A CONTRASTIVE STUDY OF THE IBIBIO AND IGBO SOUND SYSTEMS GOD’SPOWER ETIM Department Of Languages And Communication Abia State Polytechnic P.M.B. 7166, Aba, Abia State, Nigeria. [email protected] ABSTRACT This research strives to contrast the consonant phonemes, vowel phonemes and tones of Ibibio and Igbo in order to describe their similarities and differences. The researcher adopted the descriptive method, and relevant data on the phonology of the two languages were gathered and analyzed within the framework of CA before making predictions and conclusions. Ibibio consists of ten vowels and fourteen consonant phonemes, while Igbo is made up of eight vowels and twenty-eight consonants. The results of contrastive analysis of the two languages showed that there are similarities as well as differences in the sound systems of the languages. There are some sounds in Ibibio which are not present in Igbo. Also many sounds are in Igbo which do not exist in Ibibio. Both languages share the phonemes /e, a, i, o, ɔ, u, p, b, t, d, k, kp, m, n, ɲ, j, ŋ, f, s, j, w/. All the phonemes in Ibibio are present in Igbo except /ɨ/, /ʉ/, and /ʌ/. Igbo has two vowel segments /ɪ/ and /ʊ/ and also fourteen consonant phonemes /g, gb, kw, gw, ŋw, v, z, ʃ, h, ɣ, ʧ, ʤ, l, r/ which Ibibio lacks. Both languages have high, low and downstepped tones but Ibibio further has contour or gliding tones which are not tone types in Igbo. Also, the downstepped tone in Ibibio is conventionally marked with exclamation point, while in Igbo, it is conventionally marked with a raised macron over the segments bearing it.
    [Show full text]
  • A Brief Description of Consonants in Modern Standard Arabic
    Linguistics and Literature Studies 2(7): 185-189, 2014 http://www.hrpub.org DOI: 10.13189/lls.2014.020702 A Brief Description of Consonants in Modern Standard Arabic Iram Sabir*, Nora Alsaeed Al-Jouf University, Sakaka, KSA *Corresponding Author: [email protected] Copyright © 2014 Horizon Research Publishing All rights reserved. Abstract The present study deals with “A brief Modern Standard Arabic. This study starts from an description of consonants in Modern Standard Arabic”. This elucidation of the phonetic bases of sounds classification. At study tries to give some information about the production of this point shows the first limit of the study that is basically Arabic sounds, the classification and description of phonetic rather than phonological description of sounds. consonants in Standard Arabic, then the definition of the This attempt of classification is followed by lists of the word consonant. In the present study we also investigate the consonant sounds in Standard Arabic with a key word for place of articulation in Arabic consonants we describe each consonant. The criteria of description are place and sounds according to: bilabial, labio-dental, alveolar, palatal, manner of articulation and voicing. The attempt of velar, uvular, and glottal. Then the manner of articulation, description has been made to lead to the drawing of some the characteristics such as phonation, nasal, curved, and trill. fundamental conclusion at the end of the paper. The aim of this study is to investigate consonant in MSA taking into consideration that all 28 consonants of Arabic alphabets. As a language Arabic is one of the most 2.
    [Show full text]
  • The Festvox Indic Frontend for Grapheme-To-Phoneme Conversion
    The Festvox Indic Frontend for Grapheme-to-Phoneme Conversion Alok Parlikar, Sunayana Sitaram, Andrew Wilkinson and Alan W Black Carnegie Mellon University Pittsburgh, USA aup, ssitaram, aewilkin, [email protected] Abstract Text-to-Speech (TTS) systems convert text into phonetic pronunciations which are then processed by Acoustic Models. TTS frontends typically include text processing, lexical lookup and Grapheme-to-Phoneme (g2p) conversion stages. This paper describes the design and implementation of the Indic frontend, which provides explicit support for many major Indian languages, along with a unified framework with easy extensibility for other Indian languages. The Indic frontend handles many phenomena common to Indian languages such as schwa deletion, contextual nasalization, and voicing. It also handles multi-script synthesis between various Indian-language scripts and English. We describe experiments comparing the quality of TTS systems built using the Indic frontend to grapheme-based systems. While this frontend was designed keeping TTS in mind, it can also be used as a general g2p system for Automatic Speech Recognition. Keywords: speech synthesis, Indian language resources, pronunciation 1. Introduction in models of the spectrum and the prosody. Another prob- lem with this approach is that since each grapheme maps Intelligible and natural-sounding Text-to-Speech to a single “phoneme” in all contexts, this technique does (TTS) systems exist for a number of languages of the world not work well in the case of languages that have pronun- today. However, for low-resource, high-population lan- ciation ambiguities. We refer to this technique as “Raw guages, such as languages of the Indian subcontinent, there Graphemes.” are very few high-quality TTS systems available.
    [Show full text]
  • The Violability of Backness in Retroflex Consonants
    The violability of backness in retroflex consonants Paul Boersma University of Amsterdam Silke Hamann ZAS Berlin February 11, 2005 Abstract This paper addresses remarks made by Flemming (2003) to the effect that his analysis of the interaction between retroflexion and vowel backness is superior to that of Hamann (2003b). While Hamann maintained that retroflex articulations are always back, Flemming adduces phonological as well as phonetic evidence to prove that retroflex consonants can be non-back and even front (i.e. palatalised). The present paper, however, shows that the phonetic evidence fails under closer scrutiny. A closer consideration of the phonological evidence shows, by making a principled distinction between articulatory and perceptual drives, that a reanalysis of Flemming’s data in terms of unviolated retroflex backness is not only possible but also simpler with respect to the number of language-specific stipulations. 1 Introduction This paper is a reply to Flemming’s article “The relationship between coronal place and vowel backness” in Phonology 20.3 (2003). In a footnote (p. 342), Flemming states that “a key difference from the present proposal is that Hamann (2003b) employs inviolable articulatory constraints, whereas it is a central thesis of this paper that the constraints relating coronal place to tongue-body backness are violable”. The only such constraint that is violable for Flemming but inviolable for Hamann is the constraint that requires retroflex coronals to be articulated with a back tongue body. Flemming expresses this as the violable constraint RETRO!BACK, or RETRO!BACKCLO if it only requires that the closing phase of a retroflex consonant be articulated with a back tongue body.
    [Show full text]
  • A Different Look at the Lutsk Karaim Sound System (From the Second Half of the 19Th Century On)
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Jagiellonian Univeristy Repository Studia Linguistica Universitatis Iagellonicae Cracoviensis 128 (2011) MICHAŁ NÉMETH Jagiellonian University, Cracow A DIFFERENT LOOK AT THE LUTSK KARAIM SOUND SYSTEM (FROM THE SECOND HALF OF THE 19TH CENTURY ON) Keywords: Lutsk Karaim, Karaim phonetics and phonology, Ukrainian dialects Abstract After endeavouring to examine the grammatical descriptions published in the literature to date and to reconstruct the sound system of the south-western dialect of Karaim as it was presented in the literature, it can certainly be concluded that the matter is far from clear. This is for the simple reason that these works contradict each other at various points. The reason for such discrepancies should be sought in the historical and linguistic backgrounds of the two main centres of the south-western Karaim population, i.e. Lutsk and Halich. Even though these two centres were always in close communication with one another, and the language that was spoken in them originates beyond any doubt from one common root, they remained for centuries under slightly different linguistic influences as a result of the Slavonic languages surrounding them. The present paper aims to present and, where possible, clarify the differences which follow from the studies on the Karaim sound system we have at our disposal. An attempt is also made to identify some differences between the Lutsk and Halich subdialects of south-western Karaim, and explain their origin. Since the grammatical descriptions we are dealing with here and the written sources we are able to work with concern the end of the first half of the 19th century at the earliest, the time scale of our interest is limited to the second half of the 19th and the first four decades of the 20th century.
    [Show full text]
  • Sociophonetic Variation in Bolivian Quechua Uvular Stops
    Title Page Sociophonetic Variation in Bolivian Quechua Uvular Stops by Eva Bacas University of Pittsburgh, 2019 Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of the Dietrich School of Arts and Sciences in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Bachelor of Philosophy University of Pittsburgh 2019 Committee Page UNIVERSITY OF PITTSBURGH DIETRICH SCHOOL OF ARTS AND SCIENCES This thesis was presented by Eva Bacas It was defended on November 8, 2019 and approved by Alana DeLoge, Quechua Instructor, Department of Linguistics, University of Pittsburgh Melinda Fricke, Assistant Professor, Department of Linguistics, University of Pittsburgh Gillian Gallagher, Associate Professor, Department of Linguistics, New York University Thesis Advisor/Dissertation Director: Claude Mauk, Senior Lecturer, Department of Linguistics, University of Pittsburgh ii Copyright © by Eva Bacas 2019 iii Abstract Sociophonetic Variation in Bolivian Quechua Uvular Stops Eva Bacas, BPhil University of Pittsburgh, 2019 Quechua is an indigenous language of the Andes region of South America. In Cochabamba, Bolivia, Quechua and Spanish have been in contact for over 500 years. In this thesis, I explore sociolinguistic variation among bilingual speakers of Cochabamba Quechua (CQ) and Spanish by investigating the relationship between the production of the voiceless uvular stop /q/ and speakers’ sociolinguistic backgrounds. I conducted a speech production study and sociolinguistic interview with seven bilingual CQ-Spanish speakers. I analyzed manner of articulation and place of articulation variation. Results indicate that manner of articulation varies primarily due to phonological factors, and place of articulation varies according to sociolinguistic factors. This reveals that among bilingual CQ-Spanish speakers, production of voiceless uvular stop /q/ does vary sociolinguistically.
    [Show full text]
  • Recognition of Nasalized and Non- Nasalized Vowels
    RECOGNITION OF NASALIZED AND NON- NASALIZED VOWELS Submitted By: BILAL A. RAJA Advised by: Dr. Carol Espy Wilson and Tarun Pruthi Speech Communication Lab, Dept of Electrical & Computer Engineering University of Maryland, College Park Maryland Engineering Research Internship Teams (MERIT) Program 2006 1 TABLE OF CONTENTS 1- Abstract 3 2- What Is Nasalization? 3 2.1 – Production of Nasal Sound 3 2.2 – Common Spectral Characteristics of Nasalization 4 3 – Expected Results 6 3.1 – Related Previous Researches 6 3.2 – Hypothesis 7 4- Method 7 4.1 – The Task 7 4.2 – The Technique Used 8 4.3 – HMM Tool Kit (HTK) 8 4.3.1 – Data Preparation 9 4.3.2 – Training 9 4.3.3 – Testing 9 4.3.4 – Analysis 9 5 – Results 10 5.1 – Experiment 1 11 5.2 – Experiment 2 11 6 – Conclusion 12 7- References 13 2 1 - ABSTRACT When vowels are adjacent to nasal consonants (/m,n,ng/), they often become nasalized for at least some part of their duration. This nasalization is known to lead to changes in perceived vowel quality. The goal of this project is to verify if it is beneficial to first recognize nasalization in vowels and treat the three groups of vowels (those occurring before nasal consonants, those occurring after nasal consonants, and the oral vowels which are vowels that are not adjacent to nasal consonants) separately rather than collectively for recognizing the vowel identities. The standard Mel-Frequency Cepstral Coefficients (MFCCs) and the Hidden Markov Model (HMM) paradigm have been used for this purpose. The results show that when the system is trained on vowels in general, the recognition of nasalized vowels is 17% below that of oral vowels.
    [Show full text]
  • Phonetics and Phonology in Russian Unstressed Vowel Reduction: a Study in Hyperarticulation
    Phonetics and Phonology in Russian Unstressed Vowel Reduction: A Study in Hyperarticulation Jonathan Barnes Boston University (Short title: Hyperarticulating Russian Unstressed Vowels) Jonathan Barnes Department of Modern Foreign Languages and Literatures Boston University 621 Commonwealth Avenue, Room 119 Boston, MA 02215 Tel: (617) 353-6222 Fax: (617) 353-4641 [email protected] Abstract: Unstressed vowel reduction figures centrally in recent literature on the phonetics-phonology interface, in part owing to the possibility of a causal relationship between a phonetic process, duration-dependent undershoot, and the phonological neutralizations observed in systems of unstressed vocalism. Of particular interest in this light has been Russian, traditionally described as exhibiting two distinct phonological reduction patterns, differing both in degree and distribution. This study uses hyperarticulation to investigate the relationship between phonetic duration and reduction in Russian, concluding that these two reduction patterns differ not in degree, but in the level of representation at which they apply. These results are shown to have important consequences not just for theories of vowel reduction, but for other problems in the phonetics-phonology interface as well, incomplete neutralization in particular. Introduction Unstressed vowel reduction has been a subject of intense interest in recent debate concerning the nature of the phonetics-phonology interface. This is the case at least in part due to the existence of two seemingly analogous processes bearing this name, one typically called phonetic, and the other phonological. Phonological unstressed vowel reduction is a phenomenon whereby a given language's full vowel inventory can be realized only in lexically stressed syllables, while in unstressed syllables some number of neutralizations of contrast take place, with the result that only a subset of the inventory is realized on the surface.
    [Show full text]
  • Lecture 5 Sound Change
    An articulatory theory of sound change An articulatory theory of sound change Hypothesis: Most common initial motivation for sound change is the automation of production. Tokens reduced online, are perceived as reduced and represented in the exemplar cluster as reduced. Therefore we expect sound changes to reflect a decrease in gestural magnitude and an increase in gestural overlap. What are some ways to test the articulatory model? The theory makes predictions about what is a possible sound change. These predictions could be tested on a cross-linguistic database. Sound changes that take place in the languages of the world are very similar (Blevins 2004, Bateman 2000, Hajek 1997, Greenberg et al. 1978). We should consider both common and rare changes and try to explain both. Common and rare changes might have different characteristics. Among the properties we could look for are types of phonetic motivation, types of lexical diffusion, gradualness, conditioning environment and resulting segments. Common vs. rare sound change? We need a database that allows us to test hypotheses concerning what types of changes are common and what types are not. A database of sound changes? Most sound changes have occurred in undocumented periods so that we have no record of them. Even in cases with written records, the phonetic interpretation may be unclear. Only a small number of languages have historic records. So any sample of known sound changes would be biased towards those languages. A database of sound changes? Sound changes are known only for some languages of the world: Languages with written histories. Sound changes can be reconstructed by comparing related languages.
    [Show full text]
  • LT3212 Phonetics Assignment 4 Mavis, Wong Chak Yin
    LT3212 Phonetics Assignment 4 Mavis, Wong Chak Yin Essay Title: The sound system of Japanese This essay aims to introduce the sound system of Japanese, including the inventories of consonants, vowels, and diphthongs. The phonological variations of the sound segments in different phonetic environments are also included. For the illustration, word examples are given and they are presented in the following format: [IPA] (Romaji: “meaning”). Consonants In Japanese, there are 14 core consonants, and some of them have a lot of allophonic variations. The various types of consonants classified with respect to their manner of articulation are presented as follows. Stop Japanese has six oral stops or plosives, /p b t d k g/, which are classified into three place categories, bilabial, alveolar, and velar, as listed below. In each place category, there is a pair of plosives with the contrast in voicing. /p/ = a voiceless bilabial plosive [p]: [ippai] (ippai: “A cup of”) /b/ = a voiced bilabial plosive [b]: [baɴ] (ban: “Night”) /t/ = a voiceless alveolar plosive [t]: [oto̞ ːto̞ ] (ototo: “Brother”) /d/ = a voiced alveolar plosive [d]: [to̞ mo̞ datɕi] (tomodachi: “Friend”) /k/ = a voiceless velar plosive [k]: [kaiɰa] (kaiwa: “Conversation”) /g/ = a voiced velar plosive [g]: [ɡakɯβsai] (gakusai: “Student”) Phonetically, Japanese also has a glottal stop [ʔ] which is commonly produced to separate the neighboring vowels occurring in different syllables. This phonological phenomenon is known as ‘glottal stop insertion’. The glottal stop may be realized as a pause, which is used to indicate the beginning or the end of an utterance. For instance, the word “Japanese money” is actually pronounced as [ʔe̞ ɴ], instead of [je̞ ɴ], and the pronunciation of “¥15” is [dʑɯβːɡo̞ ʔe̞ ɴ].
    [Show full text]
  • UC Berkeley Dissertations, Department of Linguistics
    UC Berkeley Dissertations, Department of Linguistics Title The Aeroacoustics of Nasalized Fricatives Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/00h9g9gg Author Shosted, Ryan K Publication Date 2006 eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California The Aeroacoustics of Nasalized Fricatives by Ryan Keith Shosted B.A. (Brigham Young University) 2000 M.A. (University of California, Berkeley) 2003 A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics in the GRADUATE DIVISION of the UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA, BERKELEY Committee in charge: John J. Ohala, Chair Keith Johnson Milton M. Azevedo Fall 2006 The dissertation of Ryan Keith Shosted is approved: Chair Date Date Date University of California, Berkeley Fall 2006 The Aeroacoustics of Nasalized Fricatives Copyright 2006 by Ryan Keith Shosted 1 Abstract The Aeroacoustics of Nasalized Fricatives by Ryan Keith Shosted Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics University of California, Berkeley John J. Ohala, Chair Understanding the relationship of aerodynamic laws to the unique geometry of the hu- man vocal tract allows us to make phonological and typological predictions about speech sounds typified by particular aerodynamic regimes. For example, some have argued that the realization of nasalized fricatives is improbable because fricatives and nasals have an- tagonistic aerodynamic specifications. Fricatives require high pressure behind the suprala- ryngeal constriction as a precondition for high particle velocity. Nasalization, on the other hand, vents back pressure by allowing air to escape through the velopharyngeal orifice. This implies that an open velopharyngeal port will reduce oral particle velocity, thereby potentially extinguishing frication. By using a mechanical model of the vocal tract and spoken fricatives that have undergone coarticulatory nasalization, it is shown that nasal- ization must alter the spectral characteristics of fricatives, e.g.
    [Show full text]