Toward an Understanding of Jonathan Edwards' Psychological Theory, and an Assessment of His Place in the History of American Psychology

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Toward an Understanding of Jonathan Edwards' Psychological Theory, and an Assessment of His Place in the History of American Psychology University of New Hampshire University of New Hampshire Scholars' Repository Doctoral Dissertations Student Scholarship Summer 1974 GRACIOUS DISCOVERIES: TOWARD AN UNDERSTANDING OF JONATHAN EDWARDS' PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORY, AND AN ASSESSMENT OF HIS PLACE IN THE HISTORY OF AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGY JAMES GEORGE BLIGHT Follow this and additional works at: https://scholars.unh.edu/dissertation Recommended Citation BLIGHT, JAMES GEORGE, "GRACIOUS DISCOVERIES: TOWARD AN UNDERSTANDING OF JONATHAN EDWARDS' PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORY, AND AN ASSESSMENT OF HIS PLACE IN THE HISTORY OF AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGY" (1974). Doctoral Dissertations. 1055. https://scholars.unh.edu/dissertation/1055 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Student Scholarship at University of New Hampshire Scholars' Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Doctoral Dissertations by an authorized administrator of University of New Hampshire Scholars' Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. INFORMATION TO USERS This material was produced from a microfilm copy of the original document. While the most advanced technological means to photograph and reproduce this document have been used, the quality is heavily dependent upon the quality of the original submitted. The following explanation of techniques is provided to help you understand markings or patterns which may appear on this reproduction. 1. The sign or "target" for pages apparently lacking from the document photographed is "Missing Page(s)". If it was possible to obtain the missing page(s) or section, they are spliced into the film along with adjacent pages. This may have necessitated cutting thru an image arid duplicating adjacent pages to insure you complete continuity. 2. When an image or. the film is obliterated with a large round black mark, it is an indication that the photographer suspected that the copy may have moved during exposure and thus cause a blurred image. You will find a good image of the page in the adjacent frame. 3. When a map, drawing or chart, etc., was part of the material being photographed die photographer followed a definite method in "sectioning" the material. It is customary to begin photoing at the upper left hand corner of a large sheet and to continue photoing from left to right in equal sections with a small overlap. If necessary, sectioning is continued again — beginning below the first row and continuing on until complete. 4. The majority of users indicate that the textual content is of greatest value, however, a somewhat higher quality reproduction could be made from "photographs" if essential to the understanding of the dissertation. Silver prints of "photographs" may be ordered at additional charge by writing the Order Department, giving the cataicgnumber, title, author and specific pages you wish reproduced. 5. PLEASE NOTE: Some pages may have indistinct print. Filmed as received. Xerox university microfilms 300 North Zeeb Road Ann Arbor, Michigan 48106 Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 74-27,976 BLIGHT, James George, 1946- GRACIOUS DISCOVERIES: TOWARD AN UNDERSTANDING OF JONATHAN EDWARDS' PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORY, AND AN ASSESSMENT OF HIS PLACE IN THE HISTORY OF AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGY. University of New Hampshire, Ph.D., 1974 Psychology, general University Microfilms. A XEROX Company, Ann Arbor, Michigan Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. GRACIOUS DISCOVERIES: TOWARD AN UNDERSTANDING OF JONATHAN EDWARD5' PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORY, AND AN ASSESSMENT OF HIS PLACE IN THE HI5T0RY OF AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGY by JAMES G. BLIGHT AoB., University of Michigan, 1970 M„A., University of New Hampshire, 1973 A THESIS Submitted to the University of New Hampshire In Partial Fulfillment of The Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy Graduate 5chool Department of Psychology August, 1974 Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. This thesis has been examined and approved. ■D {AJcU- Ls~\ Thesis director, Robert I. Watson, Prof. of Psychology . COLd. Charles E. Clark, Asso. Prof. of History Rand B. Evans, Asso. Prof. of Psychology George Mx/HI si e ru'd, Prof. Emeritus of Psychology Orlo 5trunk, Jr., Pro£^of J^ychology of Religion, Boston University 24 . 197? Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Many years ago Dorothy Parker summarized the dilemma of most writers. "I hate to write," she said, "but I love to have written." For me writing is a mostly tedious, lonely, exasperating endeavor, replete with false starts, dead-ends and illogical middles. Writing history, in essence, is mind- to-data combat with the inherent absurdity of infinite infor­ mation: a tale of more or less successful attempts to impose a coherent, compelling, and appealing scheme onto some small aspect of the record of human experience. Writing itself is hardly a labor of love; it is labor, period0 It can bring into full bloom some of an indi­ vidual's most perverse characteristics. In my case, and my closest associates and family members will fully concur with this, the writing of this dissertation has helped transform me from a part-time to near full-time grump, preoccupied star-gazer, insecure neurotic, insomniac, and cat-kicker. It can lead to a kind of perpetual trance-like state in which involvement with the outer world is viewed as a nuisance. More than a few times my wife and children interrupted my train of thought by inquiring why I was writing a note in my spaghetti sauce, or why I was squeezing my bubble gum and chewing my silly putty. My characteristic response, equally mystifying I am sure, was "Jonathan Edwards, mumble mumble." iii Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. I was immersed in a consuming intellectual passion, and I would not return from the conceptual never-land until it was done with me, or I with it. It is now done and it is indeed a joy "to have written"; to have harrassed the past with some contemporary concepts; to have tied some diverse ideas together in a tenuous but common perspective. It seems kind of ridiculous to say "thank you" in print to the many people who have helped me complete this dissertation. Some of them won't even read these words. Those who do read them need not be reminded of the enormity of the following ratio: _________ Time spent helping Jim Blight________ Time spent by Jim Blight writing "thank you" I view the following "acknowledgements," therefore, simply as a humbling reminder to myselfof my indebtedness to others. Many individuals have provided a receptive audience for my half-baked historical notions and interests. A1 Raphelson, of the University of Michigan, helped to convince a struggling young undergraduate that the history of psy­ chology might just provide a suitable outlet for his diverse interests. He was right. Alan Heimert of Harvard, Julian Jaynes of Princeton, and David Lyttle of Syracuse University read the prospectus for this project and provided me with a stimulating blend, of encouragement and raised eyebrows. Thomas Shafer of McCormick Theological Seminary not only read the prospectus but offered detailed advice concerning where to look in Edwards for pertinent information and about the iv Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. general pitfalls of doing research on Edwards. He advised me that for my broad and unorthodox purposes, it would be best to concentrate on Edwards’ published works, as he knew of nothing in Edwards' voluminous, unpublished notebooks which is inconsistent with the psychological doctrine pre­ sented in the published works. Randy Easton, Dick High, George Selement, and Ron Shor, all of the University of New Hampshire, have responded to my ear-bending tactics with nothing but bemused patience and stimulating criticism. The dictionary defines "rough" as "not refined, crude, not carefully or thoroughly worked out." This is an accurate description of the state of my rough draft which was read in its entirety by one of the hardiest groups of pioneers in all academia: Charles Clark, Rand Evans, George Haslerud, Darrett B. Rutman, ,and Robert I. Watson, all of the University of New Hampshire, and Orlo 5trunk of Boston University. For the split infinitives, illogical absurdities, and other smudges which remain in this dissertation, they are blameless. They smoothed it out as best they could. Professor Robert I. Watson has guided this project from its inception. Well, almost. Actually he has guided me as I guided it. At the beginning of our relationship of professor to graduate student, he rightfully regarded me with suspicion— as an unruly renegade who had unfortunately wandered into his ken. "What on earth," I can imagine him asking his delightful wife Hazel, "am I going to _do with that guy?" It is my good fortune that he endured me; he patiently Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. let me work out my problem on my own, mostly, while offering incisive but supportive criticism whenever I asked for it. He is a remarkable man. Without him, the history of psy­ chology would still be little more than a half-cocked hobby, and this small contribution to it simply would not exist. Stephanie Ackerman typed the final version of the dissertation. Efficiency is rare these days and so is a good sense of humor. Steph has both of these in abundance. Could it be that, as Prof. Watson's secretary she honed these traits to a fine edge as a simple survival tactic? An apocryphal story may provide a clue. For a number of years Prof. Watson has been at work producing two definitive, bibliographic volumes: one of primary and the other of secondary references on the five hundred or so most eminent deceased psychologists, from Descartes to the present.
Recommended publications
  • 'Introspectionism' and the Mythical Origins of Scientific Psychology
    Consciousness and Cognition Consciousness and Cognition 15 (2006) 634–654 www.elsevier.com/locate/concog ‘Introspectionism’ and the mythical origins of scientific psychology Alan Costall Department of Psychology, University of Portsmouth, Portsmouth, Hampshire PO1 2DY, UK Received 1 May 2006 Abstract According to the majority of the textbooks, the history of modern, scientific psychology can be tidily encapsulated in the following three stages. Scientific psychology began with a commitment to the study of mind, but based on the method of introspection. Watson rejected introspectionism as both unreliable and effete, and redefined psychology, instead, as the science of behaviour. The cognitive revolution, in turn, replaced the mind as the subject of study, and rejected both behaviourism and a reliance on introspection. This paper argues that all three stages of this history are largely mythical. Introspectionism was never a dominant movement within modern psychology, and the method of introspection never went away. Furthermore, this version of psychology’s history obscures some deep conceptual problems, not least surrounding the modern conception of ‘‘behaviour,’’ that continues to make the scientific study of consciousness seem so weird. Ó 2006 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Keywords: Introspection; Introspectionism; Behaviourism; Dualism; Watson; Wundt 1. Introduction Probably the most immediate result of the acceptance of the behaviorist’s view will be the elimination of self-observation and of the introspective reports resulting from such a method. (Watson, 1913b, p. 428). The problem of consciousness occupies an analogous position for cognitive psychology as the prob- lem of language behavior does for behaviorism, namely, an unsolved anomaly within the domain of an approach.
    [Show full text]
  • Justice of God in Damnation
    THE JUSTICE OF GOD IN THE DAMNATION OF SINNERS “That every mouth may be stopped” (Romans 3:19). By Jonathan Edwards Online Edition by: International Outreach, Inc. PO Box 1286, Ames, Iowa 50014 (515) 292-9594 THE JUSTICE OF GOD IN THE DAMNATION OF SINNERS By Jonathan Edwards "That every mouth may be stopped." (Romans 3:19) The main subject of the doctrinal part of this epistle, is the free grace of God in the salvation of men by Christ Jesus; especially as it appears in the doctrine of justification by faith alone. And the more clearly to evince this doctrine, and show the reason of it, the apostle, in the first place, establishes that point, that no flesh living can be justified by the deeds of the law. And to prove it, he is very large and particular in showing, that all mankind, not only the Gentiles, but Jews, are under sin, and so under the condemnation of the law; which is what he insists upon from the beginning of the epistle to this place. He first begins with the Gentiles; and in the first chapter shows that they are under sin, by setting forth the exceeding corruptions and horrid wickedness that overspread the Gentile world: and then through the second chapter, and the former part of this third chapter, to the text and following verse, he shows the same of the Jews, that they also are in the same circumstances with the Gentiles in this regard. They had a high thought of themselves, because they were God's covenant people, and circumcised, and the children of Abraham.
    [Show full text]
  • Consciousness and the Voices of the Mind1
    Consciousness and the Voices of the Mind1 JULIAN JAYNES Born in West Newton, Massachusetts, Julian Jaynes did his undergraduate work at Har- vard and McGill and received both his master’s and doctoral degrees in psychology from Yale. While the Psychology Department at Princeton, which he joined in 1964, is still his academic base, Dr. Jaynes has had numerous positions as Visiting Lecturer or Scholar in Residence in departments of philosophy, English, and archeology and in numerous medical schools. Starting out as a traditional comparative psychobiologist, his approach was to chart the evolution of consciousness by studying learning and brain function in various species, from the protozoa to worms, reptiles, and cats. Finding this approach unsatisfactory, he changed course and has more recently examined consciousness through historical analysis, introspec- tion, and the study of language and metaphor. Dr. Jaynes has published widely, his earlier work being on topics such as imprinting in birds and the neural mediation of mating behavior in cats. His more recent work culminated in 1976 in his book The Origin of Consciousness in the Breakdown of the Bicameral Mind. Ela- borating upon this book are numerous more recent articles published in a diversity of journals such as The History of Ideas, Art World, and The Behavioral and Brain Sciences. Few problems have had as interesting an intellectual trajectory through history as that of the mind and its place in nature. Before 1859, the year that Darwin and Wallace independently pro- posed natural selection as the basis of evolution, this issue was known as the mind/body problem with its various and sometimes ponderous solutions.
    [Show full text]
  • Julian Jaynes's Software Archeology
    Julian Jaynes’s Software Archeology DANIEL DENNETT Daniel Dennett received his B .A. from Harvard and his D. Phil. from Oxford. Following academic positions at the University of California at Irvine, at Harvard, and at Oxford, he joined the Department of Philosophy at Tufts University, where he is currently Professor of Philosophy. Dr. Dennett has also held the position of Fellow at the Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioral Sciences at Stanford University. He lectures widely in North America, Great Britain and Europe. His publications include books such as Brainstorms, The Mind’s I (with Douglas Hofstadter), and most recently Elbow Room, and journal articles on the issues of mind, consciousness, self hallucinations, and even grey matter. He has also written numerous reviews of theories of prominent philosophers. linguists, and psychologists, such as Passing the Buck to Biology on Noam Chomsky and Why do we think what we do about why we think what we do? on Nelson Goodman. What a philosopher would usually do on an occasion like this is to begin to launch into a series of devastating arguments, criticisms, and counter-examples, and I am not going to do that today, because in this instance I don’t think it would be very constructive. I think first it is very important to understand Julian Jaynes’ project to see a little bit more about what the whole shape of it is, and delay the barrage of nitpicking objections and criticisms until we have seen what the edifice as a whole is. After all, on the face of it, it is preposterous, and I have found that in talking with other philosophers my main task is to convince them to take it seriously when they are very reluctant to do this.
    [Show full text]
  • Library of Congress Collections Policy Statements: Psychology
    LIBRARY OF CONGRESS COLLECTIONS POLICY STATEMENTS ±² Collections Policy Statement Index Psychology (Classes BF and Z7201-Z7204, with some Z8000s) Contents I. Scope II. Research strengths III. Collecting Policy IV. Acquisition Sources: Current and Future V. Collecting levels I. Scope The materials on psychology covered by this statement comprise physical collections in Class BF, with corresponding subject bibliographies in Z7201-7204, and also electronic, microform, manuscript or other formats of material whose subject areas would fall into these class designations. Bibliographies on individual psychologists, in the Z8000s, are also within the scope of this statement. Materials covering psychiatry are regarded as subsets of medicine, in various subclasses of R, since psychiatrists must first acquire M.D. degrees before specializing (see the Medicine Collections Policy Statement). Various topics outside the BF areas have psychological components and are classed within their own overall subject areas, e.g.: Religious Psychology (BL53 within Religion, BL); Educational Psychology (LB1051-1091 within Theory and Practice of Education, LB); Psychohistory (D16.16 within History [General], D); Psycholinguistics (P37 within Philology and Linguistics [General], P), Industrial Psychology (HF5548 within Business Administration, HF5001-6182). Works pertaining to the psychology of individual persons, ethnic groups, or classes of persons are cataloged within those overall subjects in the LC Classification scheme, but are given the topical subdivision “–Psychology” within the Library of Congress Subject Headings (LCSH) scheme; for example: “Abusive men—Psychology” (often HV6626 within Social Pathology, HV; or RC569.5.F3 within Neurology and psychiatry, RC321-571); “Actors—Psychology” (often PN2058 within Dramatic Representation: The Theater, PN2000-3307); “African Americans—Psychology” (E185.625 within History: American: African American, E184.5-185.98); or “Women—Psychology” (often HQ1206 within Women: Feminism, HQ1101-2030.7).
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolution of Human Mating: Trade-Offs and Strategic Pluralism
    BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2000) 23, 573–644 Printed in the United States of America The evolution of human mating: Trade-offs and strategic pluralism Steven W. Gangestad Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87131 [email protected] Jeffry A. Simpson Department of Psychology, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX 77843 [email protected]. Abstract: During human evolutionary history, there were “trade-offs” between expending time and energy on child-rearing and mating, so both men and women evolved conditional mating strategies guided by cues signaling the circumstances. Many short-term matings might be successful for some men; others might try to find and keep a single mate, investing their effort in rearing her offspring. Recent evidence suggests that men with features signaling genetic benefits to offspring should be preferred by women as short-term mates, but there are trade-offs between a mate’s genetic fitness and his willingness to help in child-rearing. It is these circumstances and the cues that signal them that underlie the variation in short- and long-term mating strategies between and within the sexes. Keywords: conditional strategies; evolutionary psychology; fluctuating asymmetry; mating; reproductive strategies; sexual selection Research on interpersonal relationships, especially roman- attributes (e.g., physical attractiveness) tend to assume tic ones, has increased markedly in the last three decades greater importance in mating relationships than in other (see Berscheid & Reis 1998) across a variety of fields, in- types of relationships (Buss 1989; Gangestad & Buss 1993 cluding social psychology, anthropology, ethology, sociol- [see also Kenrick & Keefe: “Age Preferences in Mates Re- ogy, developmental psychology, and personology (Ber- flect Sex Differences in Human Reproductive Strategies” scheid 1994).
    [Show full text]
  • Narcissism and the Strategic Pursuit of Short-Term Mating: Universal Links Across 11 World Regions of the International Sexuality Description Project-2
    Psychological Topics, 26 (2017), 1, 89-137 Original Scientific Paper – UDC – 616.89-008.442.6 159.923.3.072 613.88 Narcissism and the Strategic Pursuit of Short-Term Mating: Universal Links across 11 World Regions of the International Sexuality Description Project-2 David P. Schmitt - Bradley University, USA, Lidia Alcalay - Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Jüri Allik - University of Tartu, Tartu, Estonia, I.C.B. Alves - Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil, Craig A. Anderson - Iowa State University, USA, A.L. Angelini - Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil, Jens B. Asendorpf - Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany, Ivars Austers - University of Latvia, Riga, Latvia, Isabel Balaguer - University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain, Américo Baptista - University of Lusofona-Lisbon, Lisbon, Portugal, Sóley S. Bender - University of Iceland, Reykjavik, Iceland, Kevin Bennett - Penn State University-Beaver, USA, Gabriel Bianchi - Slovak Academy of Sciences, Bratislava, Slovak Republic, Behrooz Birashk - Faculty of Behavioral Sciences and Mental Health, Tehran Psychiatric Institute, Tehran, Iran, April Bleske-Rechek - University of Wisconsin-Eau Claire, USA, Fredrick A. Boholst - University of San Carlos, Cebu City, Philippines, Lynda Boothroyd - University of St. Andrews, St. Andrews, Scotland, Teresa Borja - Universidad San Francisco de Quito, Quito, Ecuador, Arjan Bos - Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, Netherlands, Edwin Brainerd - Clemson University, USA, David P. Schmitt, Department of Psychology, Bradley University, Peoria, IL 62625, USA until August of 2017. Afterward send correspondence to David P. Schmitt, Psychology Division, Department of Life Sciences, Brunel University, Uxbridge, Middlesex, UB8 3PH, United Kingdom. E-mail: [email protected] Data collection representing Costa Rica was from the University of Costa Rica and was supported by the University of Costa Rica Psychological Research Institute.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Psychology
    Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1997,4 (3), 342-349 T. V. Moore's (1939) Cognitive Psychology AIMEEM. SURPRENANT and IANNEATH Purdue University, WestLafayette, Indiana Thomas Verner Moore published a book called Cognitive Psychology in 1939,almost 30 years be­ fore Neisser's (1967) more familiar and far more influential work. Although it covers most of the stan­ dard topics found in current cognitive psychology textbooks, and even anticipated several current trends, Moore's text is not cited by any ofthe major histories of the "cognitive revolution" or any cur­ rent cognitive textbook. Weprovide a briefsketch of Moore's academic life and summarize several of his papers that are still cited. After describing the psychologies prevalent in 1939,we compare Moore's text with two contemporary works, Woodworth (1938) and McGeoch (1942). We conclude by com­ paring the older textwith Neisser's and by offering several reasons why the book is virtually unknown. Cognitive psychology differs from other areas of sci­ ner, 1985; Lachman, Lachman, & Butterfield, 1979) or entific psychology in that it loudly and proudly proclaims any ofthe current undergraduate cognitive textbooks cite to have arisen out ofa (largely bloodless) revolution. The the work. Neisser himselfwas unaware ofthe earlier vol­ very phrase "cognitive revolution" implies a fundamen­ ume until a few years after his version was published; in­ tal change in theories and research into human thought deed, his title might have been different ifhe had known and behavior and a complete overthrow of the previous about the earlier work (Neisser, personal communica­ way of thinking. As with most revolutions, the circum­ tion, June 1996).
    [Show full text]
  • The Origin of Consciousness in the Breakdown of the Bicameral Mind
    JULIAN JAYNES THE ORIGIN OF CONSCIOUSNESS IN THE BREAK DOWN OF THE BICAMERAL MIND AFTERWORD T IS NOW more than a decade since this essay first appeared in Ibook form, and my publishers have encouraged me to add a post- script in which I might discuss the general reaction to this book as well as changes I might make if I were to rewrite it. A favorite practice of some professional intellectuals when at first faced with a theory as large as the one I have presented is to search for that loose thread which, when pulled, will unravel all the rest. And rightly so. It is part of the discipline of scientific thinking. In any work covering so much of the terrain of human nature and history, hustling into territories jealously guarded by myriad aggressive specialists, there are bound to be such errancies, some- times of fact but I fear more often of tone. But that the knitting of this book is such that a tug on such a bad stitch will unravel all the rest is more of a hope on the part of the orthodox than a fact in the scientific pursuit of truth. The book is not a single hypothesis. There are four main hypotheses in Books I and II. I welcome this opportunity to add some comments to each of them. 1. Consciousness is based on language. Such a statement is of course contradictory to the usual and I think superficial views of consciousness that are embedded both in popular belief and in lan- guage.
    [Show full text]
  • Evangelical Reform in Early Nineteenth Century America
    1 Published as the chapter on “Nineteenth Century America” in Don Eberly, editor, Building a Healthy Culture: Strategies for an American Renaissance, Eerdmans Publishing Co., 2001, pp. 181-199. Evangelical Reform in Early Nineteenth Century America by John G. West* To American evangelicals, the new century seemed anything but hospitable.1 Many Americans had stopped going to church. Some openly doubted Christianity, preferring to place their hopes in reason alone rather than a God who intervenes in human affairs. The nation’s cities were turning into havens of crime, promiscuity, and alcoholism. Radical social reformers dotted the landscape, attracting enthusiastic interest, if not outright support. One of the more provocative of the radicals proposed a “Declaration of Mental Independence” that denounced private property, traditional religion, and marriage as “a TRINITY of the most monstrous evils that could be combined to inflict mental and physical evil upon [man’s] whole race.”2 Even in politics, traditional religion and morality were flouted. Thomas Jefferson, one of the era’s most influential presidents, scoffed in private at the miracles of the Bible and historic Christian doctrines such as the Trinity.3 Another popular chief executive, Andrew Jackson, was the only president in American history who had killed another man in a duel.4 Yet voters didn’t seem to care. In many ways, the culture wars of America in the early 1800s seem eerily like some of the cultural conflicts in America today. Yet most historians wouldn’t describe nineteenth-century America as especially secular or amoral. If anything, the period is often held up as the epitome of a Christian America—when Christianity, or at least the Protestant variety of Christianity—was the dominant religion of the state, and when Biblical ethics supplied the basis for social relations.
    [Show full text]
  • Ramifications of Julian Jaynes's Theory of Consciousness for Traditional General Semantics
    Ramifications of Julian Jaynes's Theory of Consciousness for Traditional General Semantics Philip Ardery Good morning. Thank you for coming. In my talk, first I will explore why General Semantics theorists over 70 years generally have overlooked the human behaviors that give rise to the sharpest and most violent of today's conflicts of world views—namely, religious behaviors. Second, I will introduce some theories formulated by cultural anthropologist Julian Jaynes which "explain" religion and which have helped me extensionalize my own definition of the so-called "verbal levels", a General Semantics term I previously had defined intensionally. I will close by haranguing you briefly with an argument that the perspectives opened up by Jaynes can help us strengthen General Semantics doctrines. I trace the general neglect of religion by General Semantics theorists back to Korzybski's own proud rejection of religious influences. He said this to students at the 1948-49 Institute of General Semantics intensive seminar: I was born a Pole in Warsaw in a very peculiar family. My father was an engineer and for generations they were either mathematicians or senators, lawyers, economists, engineers, what not – all scientific, scientific, scientific. … [T]he milieu of the country was Catholic, but we didn't have any need of any religion. We were not agnostic, we were not atheists, we were just scientific for generations, so although the milieu was Catholic, I was not contaminated…. This was very fundamental in my whole work. I was completely free.1 Korzysbski's freedom came at a price. Thanks to his freedom, he hardly understood religion, even failing to recognize the religious content in a major event in his own life.
    [Show full text]
  • The Earliest Prints and Paintings of New England Indians
    Rhode Island History Pubhvhed by The Rhude leland Hrst oncal Volume 4 1, Number 3 August 1982 'iO(;lcty, \1 Powe r Streel, Providence. Rhode J~I.1n d, 029CX>, and pruned bv a ~u n l trcm t he SU IC 01 Rhode bland .1nd Provide nce Plamauons r ItI'ter h Garrahv, ('.cwernnl Contents lcsued QU.1 l1crl)' ill Providence. Rhode 1..land , Pebruar v ,\b ~', AURU H and Novem ber Second da!>~ po"URe pard The Earliest Prints and Paintings 73 at Providence, Rhode lvland of New England Indians Eduorral ott1l;C~.11 Ald uch House, WI L LIAM S . S IM MONS 110 Benevolent Street. Providence. Rhode l~l.and Oly«. Rechard F Suplc!> "re<ldtnt The Threat of Radicalism : Seward's Alden.\1 Anderso n, vice pre<ldenl Candidacy and th e Rhode Island :'\.1r.. Ed" In G, Fr",hCI, VICt presldtnt Richa rd A Sherman, vectctarv Gubernatorial Election of 1860 .\, Rachel Cunh., o.ul<tanl secretarv 'AM ES L. H USTON \itc phe n C W llh. m ~ , treasurer Alben Car lom, aH"rOnl treasurer f H l O\ll' \ {H TH l "~I ETY Book Reviews 100 C.rl Bnden baugh Svdne v V lam es Anrome ue F D{lwnln~ Corrections !OJ PUIlLICA TIOS, COM MITTE E Dr s.cct>c n I Goldtlw,ky, chonman Gordon Allen Henry L P geckwuh lr Dr Francis H, Cbafee Prot. Howard r. Cbudacutt Wenddl D Garrcn Robert Allen G reene Ted Holmberg Pamela A. Kennedy Leonard 1 Levin Alan Simp"," wm McKenz ie Wood ward W.
    [Show full text]