The Evolution of Self-Control PNAS PLUS

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Evolution of Self-Control PNAS PLUS The evolution of self-control PNAS PLUS Evan L. MacLeana,1, Brian Harea,b, Charles L. Nunna, Elsa Addessic, Federica Amicid, Rindy C. Andersone, Filippo Aurelif,g, Joseph M. Bakerh,i, Amanda E. Baniaj, Allison M. Barnardk, Neeltje J. Boogertl, Elizabeth M. Brannonb,m, Emily E. Brayn, Joel Braya, Lauren J. N. Brentb,o, Judith M. Burkartp, Josep Calld, Jessica F. Cantlonk, Lucy G. Chekeq, Nicola S. Claytonq, Mikel M. Delgador, Louis J. DiVincentis, Kazuo Fujitat, Esther Herrmannd, Chihiro Hiramatsut, Lucia F. Jacobsr,u, Kerry E. Jordanv, Jennifer R. Laudew, Kristin L. Leimgruberx, Emily J. E. Messerl, Antonio C. de A. Mouray, Ljerka Ostojicq, Alejandra Picardz, Michael L. Platta,b,o,aa, Joshua M. Plotnikq,bb, Friederike Rangecc,dd, Simon M. Readeree, Rachna B. Reddyff, Aaron A. Sandelff, Laurie R. Santosx, Katrin Schumannd, Amanda M. Seedl, Kendra B. Sewalle, Rachael C. Shawq, Katie E. Slocombez, Yanjie Sugg, Ayaka Takimotot, Jingzhi Tana, Ruoting Taol, Carel P. van Schaikp, Zsófia Virányicc, Elisabetta Visalberghic, Jordan C. Wadew, Arii Watanabeq, Jane Widnessx, Julie K. Younghh, Thomas R. Zentallw, and Yini Zhaogg Departments of aEvolutionary Anthropology, aaNeurobiology, and mPsychology and Neuroscience, and bCenter for Cognitive Neuroscience, oDuke Institute for Brain Sciences, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708; cIstituto di Scienze e Tecnologie della Cognizione Consiglio Nazionale delle Ricerche, 00197 Rome, Italy; dDepartment of Developmental and Comparative Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, D-04103 Leipzig, Germany; eDepartment of Biology, Duke University, Durham, NC 27704; fInstituto de Neuroetologia, Universidad Veracruzana, Xalapa, CP 91190, Mexico; gResearch Centre in Evolutionary Anthropology and Palaeoecology, Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool L3 3AF, United Kingdom; hCenter for Interdisciplinary Brain Sciences Research and iDepartment of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA 94305; jCenter for Animal Care Sciences, Smithsonian National Zoological Park, Washington, DC 20008; kDepartment of Brain and Cognitive Science and sDepartment of Comparative Medicine, Seneca Park Zoo, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY 14620; lDepartment of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St. Andrews, St. Andrews KY16 9JP, Scotland; nDepartment of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104; pAnthropological Institute and Museum, University of Zurich, 8057 Zurich, Switzerland; qDepartment of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EB, United Kingdom; rDepartment of Psychology and uHelen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, Berkeley, CA 94720; tGraduate School of Letters, Kyoto University, Kyoto 606-8501, Japan; Departments of vPsychology and hhWildland Resources, Utah State University, Logan, UT 84322; wDepartment of Psychology, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY 40506; xDepartment of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, CT 06520; yDepartamento Engenharia e Meio Ambiente, Universidade Federal da Paraiba, 58059-900, João Pessoa, Brazil; zDepartment of Psychology, University of York, Heslington, York YO10 5DD, United Kingdom; bbThink Elephants International, Stone Ridge, NY 12484; ccMesserli Research Institute, University of Veterinary Medicine Vienna, 1210 Vienna, Austria; ddWolf Science Center, A-2115 Ernstbrunn, Austria; eeDepartment of Biology, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada H3A 1B1; ffDepartment of Anthropology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109; and ggDepartment of Psychology, Peking University, Beijing 100871, China COGNITIVE SCIENCES PSYCHOLOGICAL AND Edited by Jon H. Kaas, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, and approved March 21, 2014 (received for review December 30, 2013) Cognition presents evolutionary research with one of its greatest how cognition varies across species, previous research has largely challenges. Cognitive evolution has been explained at the proxi- relied on proxies for cognition (e.g., brain size) or metaanalyses mate level by shifts in absolute and relative brain volume and at when testing hypotheses about cognitive evolution (76–92). The the ultimate level by differences in social and dietary complexity. lack of cognitive data collected with similar methods across large However, no study has integrated the experimental and phyloge- samples of species precludes meaningful species comparisons that netic approach at the scale required to rigorously test these ex- can reveal the major forces shaping cognitive evolution across planations. Instead, previous research has largely relied on various species, including humans (48, 70, 89, 93–98). measures of brain size as proxies for cognitive abilities. We ex- perimentally evaluated these major evolutionary explanations by Significance quantitatively comparing the cognitive performance of 567 indi- viduals representing 36 species on two problem-solving tasks Although scientists have identified surprising cognitive flexi- measuring self-control. Phylogenetic analysis revealed that abso- bility in animals and potentially unique features of human lute brain volume best predicted performance across species and psychology, we know less about the selective forces that favor accounted for considerably more variance than brain volume con- cognitive evolution, or the proximate biological mechanisms trolling for body mass. This result corroborates recent advances in underlying this process. We tested 36 species in two problem- evolutionary neurobiology and illustrates the cognitive consequen- solving tasks measuring self-control and evaluated the leading ces of cortical reorganization through increases in brain volume. hypotheses regarding how and why cognition evolves. Across Within primates, dietary breadth but not social group size was a species, differences in absolute (not relative) brain volume best strong predictor of species differences in self-control. Our results predicted performance on these tasks. Within primates, dietary implicate robust evolutionary relationships between dietary breadth, breadth also predicted cognitive performance, whereas social absolute brain volume, and self-control. These findings provide a sig- group size did not. These results suggest that increases in ab- nificant first step toward quantifying the primate cognitive phenome solute brain size provided the biological foundation for evo- and explaining the process of cognitive evolution. lutionary increases in self-control, and implicate species dif- ferences in feeding ecology as a potential selective pressure psychology | behavior | comparative methods | inhibitory control | favoring these skills. executive function Author contributions: E.L.M., B.H., and C.L.N. designed research; E.L.M., B.H., E.A., ince Darwin, understanding the evolution of cognition has F. Amici, R.C.A., F. Aureli, J. M. Baker, A.E.B., A.M.B., N.J.B., E.M.B., E.E.B., J.B., L.J.N.B., Sbeen widely regarded as one of the greatest challenges for J. M. Burkart, J.C., J.F.C., L.G.C., N.S.C., M.M.D., L.J.D., K.F., E.H., C.H., L.F.J., K.E.J., J.R.L., K.L.L., E.J.E.M., A.C.d.A.M., L.O., A.P., M.L.P., J.M.P., F.R., S.M.R., R.B.R., A.A.S., L.R.S., K.S., evolutionary research (1). Although researchers have identified A.M.S., K.B.S., R.C.S., K.E.S., Y.S., A.T., J.T., R.T., C.P.v.S., Z.V., E.V., J.C.W., A.W., J.W., J.K.Y., surprising cognitive flexibility in a range of species (2–40) and T.R.Z., and Y.Z. performed research; E.L.M. and C.L.N. analyzed data; and E.L.M., B.H., and potentially derived features of human psychology (41–61), we know C.L.N. wrote the paper. much less about the major forces shaping cognitive evolution (62– The authors declare no conflict of interest. 71). With the notable exception of Bitterman’s landmark studies This article is a PNAS Direct Submission. conducted several decades ago (63, 72–74), most research com- 1To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: [email protected]. paring cognition across species has been limited to small taxonomic This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10. samples (70, 75). With limited comparable experimental data on 1073/pnas.1323533111/-/DCSupplemental. www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1323533111 PNAS Early Edition | 1of9 Downloaded by guest on September 29, 2021 To address these challenges we measured cognitive skills for studies showing a positive correlation between a species’ typical self-control in 36 species of mammals and birds (Fig. 1 and group size and the neocortex ratio (80, 81, 85–87, 129, 142–145), Tables S1–S4) tested using the same experimental procedures, and cognitive differences between closely related species with different evaluated the leading hypotheses for the neuroanatomical under- group sizes (130, 137, 146, 147), and evidence for cognitive con- pinnings and ecological drivers of variance in animal cognition. At vergence between highly social species (26, 31, 148–150). The the proximate level, both absolute (77, 99–107) and relative brain foraging hypothesis posits that dietary complexity, indexed by field size (108–112) have been proposed as mechanisms supporting reports of dietary breadth and reliance on fruit (a spatiotemporally cognitive evolution. Evolutionary increases in brain size (both ab- distributed resource), was the primary driver of primate cognitive solute and relative) and cortical reorganization are hallmarks of the evolution (151–154). This hypothesis is supported by studies human lineage and are
Recommended publications
  • The Elephant Brain in Numbers
    ORIGINAL RESEARCH ARTICLE published: 12 June 2014 NEUROANATOMY doi: 10.3389/fnana.2014.00046 The elephant brain in numbers Suzana Herculano-Houzel 1,2*, Kamilla Avelino-de-Souza 1,2, Kleber Neves 1,2, Jairo Porfírio 1,2, Débora Messeder 1,2, Larissa Mattos Feijó 1,2, José Maldonado 3 and Paul R. Manger 4 1 Instituto de Ciências Biomédicas, Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil 2 Instituto Nacional de Neurociência Translacional, São Paulo, Brazil 3 MBF Bioscience, Inc., Rio de Janeiro, Brazil 4 School of Anatomical Sciences, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of the Witwatersrand, Johannesburg, South Africa Edited by: What explains the superior cognitive abilities of the human brain compared to other, larger Patrick R. Hof, Mount Sinai School of brains? Here we investigate the possibility that the human brain has a larger number of Medicine, USA neurons than even larger brains by determining the cellular composition of the brain of Reviewed by: the African elephant. We find that the African elephant brain, which is about three times John M. Allman, Caltech, USA 9 Roger Lyons Reep, University of larger than the human brain, contains 257 billion (10 ) neurons, three times more than the Florida, USA average human brain; however, 97.5%of the neurons in the elephant brain (251 billion) are *Correspondence: found in the cerebellum. This makes the elephant an outlier in regard to the number of Suzana Herculano-Houzel, Centro de cerebellar neurons compared to other mammals, which might be related to sensorimotor Ciências da Saúde, Universidade specializations. In contrast, the elephant cerebral cortex, which has twice the mass of the Federal do Rio de Janeiro, Rua Carlos Chagas Filho, 373 – sala human cerebral cortex, holds only 5.6 billion neurons, about one third of the number of F1-009, Ilha do Fundão 21941-902, neurons found in the human cerebral cortex.
    [Show full text]
  • How Welfare Biology and Commonsense May Help to Reduce Animal Suffering
    Ng, Yew-Kwang (2016) How welfare biology and commonsense may help to reduce animal suffering. Animal Sentience 7(1) DOI: 10.51291/2377-7478.1012 This article has appeared in the journal Animal Sentience, a peer-reviewed journal on animal cognition and feeling. It has been made open access, free for all, by WellBeing International and deposited in the WBI Studies Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Ng, Yew-Kwang (2016) How welfare biology and commonsense may help to reduce animal suffering. Animal Sentience 7(1) DOI: 10.51291/2377-7478.1012 Cover Page Footnote I am grateful to Dr. Timothy D. Hau of the University of Hong Kong for assistance. This article is available in Animal Sentience: https://www.wellbeingintlstudiesrepository.org/ animsent/vol1/iss7/1 Animal Sentience 2016.007: Ng on Animal Suffering Call for Commentary: Animal Sentience publishes Open Peer Commentary on all accepted target articles. Target articles are peer-reviewed. Commentaries are editorially reviewed. There are submitted commentaries as well as invited commentaries. Commentaries appear as soon as they have been revised and accepted. Target article authors may respond to their commentaries individually or in a joint response to multiple commentaries. Instructions: http://animalstudiesrepository.org/animsent/guidelines.html How welfare biology and commonsense may help to reduce animal suffering Yew-Kwang Ng Division of Economics Nanyang Technological University Singapore Abstract: Welfare biology is the study of the welfare of living things. Welfare is net happiness (enjoyment minus suffering). Since this necessarily involves feelings, Dawkins (2014) has suggested that animal welfare science may face a paradox, because feelings are very difficult to study.
    [Show full text]
  • THE CASE AGAINST Marine Mammals in Captivity Authors: Naomi A
    s l a m m a y t T i M S N v I i A e G t A n i p E S r a A C a C E H n T M i THE CASE AGAINST Marine Mammals in Captivity The Humane Society of the United State s/ World Society for the Protection of Animals 2009 1 1 1 2 0 A M , n o t s o g B r o . 1 a 0 s 2 u - e a t i p s u S w , t e e r t S h t u o S 9 8 THE CASE AGAINST Marine Mammals in Captivity Authors: Naomi A. Rose, E.C.M. Parsons, and Richard Farinato, 4th edition Editors: Naomi A. Rose and Debra Firmani, 4th edition ©2009 The Humane Society of the United States and the World Society for the Protection of Animals. All rights reserved. ©2008 The HSUS. All rights reserved. Printed on recycled paper, acid free and elemental chlorine free, with soy-based ink. Cover: ©iStockphoto.com/Ying Ying Wong Overview n the debate over marine mammals in captivity, the of the natural environment. The truth is that marine mammals have evolved physically and behaviorally to survive these rigors. public display industry maintains that marine mammal For example, nearly every kind of marine mammal, from sea lion Iexhibits serve a valuable conservation function, people to dolphin, travels large distances daily in a search for food. In learn important information from seeing live animals, and captivity, natural feeding and foraging patterns are completely lost.
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolution of Self-Control
    The evolution of self-control Evan L. MacLeana,1, Brian Harea,b, Charles L. Nunna, Elsa Addessic, Federica Amicid, Rindy C. Andersone, Filippo Aurelif,g, Joseph M. Bakerh,i, Amanda E. Baniaj, Allison M. Barnardk, Neeltje J. Boogertl, Elizabeth M. Brannonb,m, Emily E. Brayn, Joel Braya, Lauren J. N. Brentb,o, Judith M. Burkartp, Josep Calld, Jessica F. Cantlonk, Lucy G. Chekeq, Nicola S. Claytonq, Mikel M. Delgador, Louis J. DiVincentis, Kazuo Fujitat, Esther Herrmannd, Chihiro Hiramatsut, Lucia F. Jacobsr,u, Kerry E. Jordanv, Jennifer R. Laudew, Kristin L. Leimgruberx, Emily J. E. Messerl, Antonio C. de A. Mouray, Ljerka Ostojicq, Alejandra Picardz, Michael L. Platta,b,o,aa, Joshua M. Plotnikq,bb, Friederike Rangecc,dd, Simon M. Readeree, Rachna B. Reddyff, Aaron A. Sandelff, Laurie R. Santosx, Katrin Schumannd, Amanda M. Seedl, Kendra B. Sewalle, Rachael C. Shawq, Katie E. Slocombez, Yanjie Sugg, Ayaka Takimotot, Jingzhi Tana, Ruoting Taol, Carel P. van Schaikp, Zsófia Virányicc, Elisabetta Visalberghic, Jordan C. Wadew, Arii Watanabeq, Jane Widnessx, Julie K. Younghh, Thomas R. Zentallw, and Yini Zhaogg Departments of aEvolutionary Anthropology, aaNeurobiology, and mPsychology and Neuroscience, and bCenter for Cognitive Neuroscience, oDuke Institute for Brain Sciences, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708; cIstituto di Scienze e Tecnologie della Cognizione Consiglio Nazionale delle Ricerche, 00197 Rome, Italy; dDepartment of Developmental and Comparative Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, D-04103 Leipzig, Germany;
    [Show full text]
  • Seasonality and Brain Size Are Negatively Associated in Frogs
    www.nature.com/scientificreports OPEN Seasonality and brain size are negatively associated in frogs: evidence for the expensive brain Received: 7 July 2017 Accepted: 20 November 2017 framework Published: xx xx xxxx Yi Luo1,2, Mao Jun Zhong1,2, Yan Huang1,2, Feng Li1,2, Wen Bo Liao1,2 & Alexander Kotrschal3 The challenges of seasonal environments are thought to contribute to brain evolution, but in which way is debated. According to the Cognitive Bufer Hypothesis (CBH) brain size should increase with seasonality, as the cognitive benefts of a larger brain should help overcoming periods of food scarcity via, for instance, increased behavioral fexibility. However, in line with the Expensive Brain Framework (EBF) brain size should decrease with seasonality because a smaller brain confers energetic benefts in periods of food scarcity. Empirical evidence is inconclusive and mostly limited to homoeothermic animals. Here we used phylogenetic comparative analyses to test the impact of seasonality on brain evolution across 30 species of anurans (frogs) experiencing a wide range of temperature and precipitation. Our results support the EBF because relative brain size and the size of the optic tectum were negatively correlated with variability in temperature. In contrast, we found no association between the variability in precipitation and the length of the dry season with either brain size or the sizes of other major brain regions. We suggest that seasonality-induced food scarcity resulting from higher variability in temperature constrains brain size evolution in anurans. Less seasonal environments may therefore facilitate the evolution of larger brains in poikilothermic animals. Brain size varies dramatically between species1 and the recent decades have uncovered a variety of factors that drive this evolution2–10.
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolutionary History of Cetacean Brain and Body Size
    WellBeing International WBI Studies Repository 11-2013 The Evolutionary History of Cetacean Brain and Body Size Stephen H. Montgomery University of Cambridge Jonathan H. Geisler New York Institute of Technology - Old Westbury Michael R. McGowen Wayne State University Charlotte Fox University of Cambridge Lori Marino Emory University See next page for additional authors Follow this and additional works at: https://www.wellbeingintlstudiesrepository.org/acwp_vsm Part of the Animal Structures Commons, Animal Studies Commons, and the Veterinary Anatomy Commons Recommended Citation Montgomery, S. H., Geisler, J. H., McGowen, M. R., Fox, C., Marino, L., & Gatesy, J. (2013). The evolutionary history of cetacean brain and body size. Evolution, 67(11), 3339-3353. This material is brought to you for free and open access by WellBeing International. It has been accepted for inclusion by an authorized administrator of the WBI Studies Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Authors Stephen H. Montgomery, Jonathan H. Geisler, Michael R. McGowen, Charlotte Fox, Lori Marino, and John Gatesy This article is available at WBI Studies Repository: https://www.wellbeingintlstudiesrepository.org/acwp_vsm/14 The Evolutionary History of Cetacean Brain and Body Size Stephen H. Montgomery University of Cambridge Jonathan H. Geisler New York Institute of Technology Michael R. McGowen Wayne State University School of Medicine Charlotte Fox University of Cambridge Lori Marino Emory University John Gatesy University of California – Riverside KEYWORDS brain size, body size, cetaceans, dolphins, encephalization, evolution, macroevolution, whales ABSTRACT Cetaceans rival primates in brain size relative to body size and include species with the largest brains and biggest bodies to have ever evolved.
    [Show full text]
  • A Review of Effects of Environment on Brain Size in Insects
    insects Review A Review of Effects of Environment on Brain Size in Insects Thomas Carle Faculty of Biology, Kyushu University, Fukuoka 819-0395, Japan; [email protected] Simple Summary: What makes a big brain is fascinating since it is considered as a measure of intelligence. Above all, brain size is associated with body size. If species that have evolved with complex social behaviours possess relatively bigger brains than those deprived of such behaviours, this does not constitute the only factor affecting brain size. Other factors such as individual experience or surrounding environment also play roles in the size of the brain. In this review, I summarize the recent findings about the effects of environment on brain size in insects. I also discuss evidence about how the environment has an impact on sensory systems and influences brain size. Abstract: Brain size fascinates society as well as researchers since it is a measure often associated with intelligence and was used to define species with high “intellectual capabilities”. In general, brain size is correlated with body size. However, there are disparities in terms of relative brain size between species that may be explained by several factors such as the complexity of social behaviour, the ‘social brain hypothesis’, or learning and memory capabilities. These disparities are used to classify species according to an ‘encephalization quotient’. However, environment also has an important role on the development and evolution of brain size. In this review, I summarise the recent studies looking at the effects of environment on brain size in insects, and introduce the idea that the role of environment might be mediated through the relationship between olfaction and vision.
    [Show full text]
  • Modi Ed Formulas for Calculation of Encephalization Quotient in Dogs
    Modied Formulas for Calculation of Encephalization Quotient in Dogs Saganuwan Alhaji Saganuwan ( [email protected] ) Federal University of Agriculture Research note Keywords: German Shepherd, Allometry, Encephalization Quotient, Intelligence Posted Date: March 18th, 2021 DOI: https://doi.org/10.21203/rs.3.rs-322274/v1 License: This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. Read Full License Page 1/8 Abstract Objective Dogs are a breed of animals that play important roles, ranging from security passing through companionship to models of research for application in humans. Intelligence is the key factor to success in life, most especially for dogs that are used for security purposes at the airports, seaports, public places, houses, schools and farms. However, it has been reported that there is correlation between intelligence, body weight, height and craniometry in human. In view of this, literatures on body weight, height and body surface areas of ten dogs were assessed with a view to determining their comparative level of intelligence. Results Findings revealed that dogs share brain common allometric relationships with human as shown by Encephalization Quotient (EQ)= Brain Mass/0.14 x Body weight0.528 as compared with Brain Mass /0.12 x Body Weight0.66 and Brain Mass (E)=kpβ, where p is the body weight,k=0.14 and β=0.528 which yielded better results as compared with the other formulas. Dogs with BSA, weight and height similar to that of human are the most intelligent. Doberman Pinscher is the most intelligent followed by German Shepherd, Labrador Retriever, Golden Retriever, respectively.
    [Show full text]
  • Primate Encephalization
    Provided for non-commercial research and educational use only. Not for reproduction, distribution or commercial use. This chapter was originally published in the book Progress in Brain Research, Vol. 195, published by Elsevier, and the attached copy is provided by Elsevier for the author's benefit and for the benefit of the author's institution, for non-commercial research and educational use including without limitation use in instruction at your institution, sending it to specific colleagues who know you, and providing a copy to your institution’s administrator. All other uses, reproduction and distribution, including without limitation commercial reprints, selling or licensing copies or access, or posting on open internet sites, your personal or institution’s website or repository, are prohibited. For exceptions, permission may be sought for such use through Elsevier's permissions site at: http://www.elsevier.com/locate/permissionusematerial From: Louis Lefebvre, Primate encephalization. In Michel A. Hofman, Dean Falk, editors: Progress in Brain Research, Vol. 195, Amsterdam: The Netherlands, 2012, pp. 393-412. ISBN: 978-0-444-53860-4 © Copyright 2012 Elsevier B.V. Elsevier Author's personal copy M. A. Hofman and D. Falk (Eds.) Progress in Brain Research, Vol. 195 ISSN: 0079-6123 Copyright Ó 2012 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. CHAPTER 19 Primate encephalization Louis Lefebvre* Department of Biology, McGill University, Montréal, QC, Canada Abstract: Encephalization is a concept that implies an increase in brain or neocortex size
    [Show full text]
  • Does Diving Limit Brain Size in Cetaceans?
    WellBeing International WBI Studies Repository 4-2006 Does Diving Limit Brain Size in Cetaceans? Lori Marino Emory University Daniel Sol McGill University Kristen Toren Emory University Louis Lefebvre McGill University Follow this and additional works at: https://www.wellbeingintlstudiesrepository.org/acwp_vsm Part of the Animal Structures Commons, Animal Studies Commons, and the Veterinary Anatomy Commons Recommended Citation Marino, L., Sol, D., Toren, K., & Lefebvre, L. (2006). Does diving limit brain size in cetaceans?. Marine Mammal Science, 22(2), 413-425. This material is brought to you for free and open access by WellBeing International. It has been accepted for inclusion by an authorized administrator of the WBI Studies Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Does Diving Limit Brain Size in Cetaceans? Lori Marino,1,2 Daniel Sol,3,4 Kristen Toren,1 and Louis Lefebvre3 1 Emory University 2 Yeres Regional Primate Center 3 McGill University 4 Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona KEYWORDS brain size, diving, oxygenation, cetacean ABSTRACT We test the longstanding hypothesis, known as the dive constraint hypothesis, that the oxygenation demands of diving pose a constraint on aquatic mammal brain size.Using a sample of 23 cetacean species we examine the relationship among six different measures of relative brain size, body size, and maximum diving duration. Unlike previous tests we include body size as a covariate and perform independent contrast analyses to control for phylogeny. We show that diving does not limit brain size in cetaceans and therefore provide no support for the dive constraint hypothesis. Instead, body size is the main predictor of maximum diving duration in cetaceans.
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolution of Self-Control Evan L
    University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln USDA National Wildlife Research Center - Staff U.S. Department of Agriculture: Animal and Plant Publications Health Inspection Service 2014 The evolution of self-control Evan L. MacLean Duke University, [email protected] Brian Hare Duke University Charles L. Nunn Duke University Elsa Addessi Istituto di Scienze e Tecnologie della Cognizione Consiglio Nazionale delle Ricerche Federica Amici Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology See next page for additional authors Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/icwdm_usdanwrc Part of the Life Sciences Commons MacLean, Evan L.; Hare, Brian; Nunn, Charles L.; Addessi, Elsa; Amici, Federica; Anderson, Rindy C.; Aureli, Filippo; Baker, Joseph M.; Bania, Amanda E.; Barnard, Allison M.; Boogert, Neeltje J.; Brannon, Elizabeth M.; Bray, Emily E.; Bray, Joel; Brent, Lauren J. N.; Burkart, Judith M.; Call, Josep; Cantlon, Jessica F.; Cheke, Lucy G.; Clayton, Nicola S.; Delgado, Mikel M.; DiVindenti, Louis J.; Fujita, Kazuo; Herrmann, Esther; Hiramatsu, Chihiro; Jacobs, Lucia F.; Jordan, Kerry E.; Laude, Jennifer R.; Leimgruber, Kristin L.; Messer, Emily J. E.; de A. Moura, Antonio C.; Ostojic, Ljerka; Picard, Alejandra; Platt, Michael L.; Plotnik, Joshua M.; Range, Friederike; Reader, Simon M.; Reddy, Rachna B.; Sandel, Aaron A.; Santos, Laurie R.; Schumann, Katrin; Seed, Amanda M.; Sewall, Kendra B.; Shaw, Rachael C.; Slocombre, Katie E.; Su, Yanjie; Takimoto, Ayaka; Tan, Jingzhi; Tao, Ruoting; van Schaik, Carel P.; Viranyi, Zsofia; Visalberghi, Elisabetta; Wade, Jordan C.; Watanabe, Arii; Widness, Janes; Young, Julie K.; Zentall, Thomas R.; and Zhao, Yini, "The ve olution of self-control" (2014). USDA National Wildlife Research Center - Staff Publications.
    [Show full text]
  • The Cognitive Bases of Human Tool Use
    BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2012) 35, 203–262 doi:10.1017/S0140525X11001452 The cognitive bases of human tool use Krist Vaesen Philosophy & Ethics, School of Innovation Sciences, Eindhoven University of Technology, 5612 AZ Eindhoven, The Netherlands [email protected] http://home.ieis.tue.nl/kvaesen Abstract: This article has two goals. The first is to assess, in the face of accruing reports on the ingenuity of great ape tool use, whether and in what sense human tool use still evidences unique, higher cognitive ability. To that effect, I offer a systematic comparison between humans and nonhuman primates with respect to nine cognitive capacities deemed crucial to tool use: enhanced hand-eye coordination, body schema plasticity, causal reasoning, function representation, executive control, social learning, teaching, social intelligence, and language. Since striking differences between humans and great apes stand firm in eight out of nine of these domains, I conclude that human tool use still marks a major cognitive discontinuity between us and our closest relatives. As a second goal of the paper, I address the evolution of human technologies. In particular, I show how the cognitive traits reviewed help to explain why technological accumulation evolved so markedly in humans, and so modestly in apes. Keywords: cognition; evolution; great apes; technology; tool use; cumulative culture 1. Introduction detailed and balanced overview), I conclude that human tool use still marks a major cognitive discontinuity Ever since Jane Goodall’s observations of ant-dipping chim- between us and our closest relatives; and relatedly, that panzees some 50 years ago, the idea that human tool use evi- no individual cognitive trait can be singled out as the key dences “humanique” cognitive ability has lost popularity.
    [Show full text]