Threat-Sensitive Learning and Generalization of Predator Recognition by Aquatic Vertebrates

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Threat-Sensitive Learning and Generalization of Predator Recognition by Aquatic Vertebrates THREAT-SENSITIVE LEARNING AND GENERALIZATION OF PREDATOR RECOGNITION BY AQUATIC VERTEBRATES A Thesis Submitted to the College of Graduate Studies and Research in Partial Fulfilment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Department of Biology University of Saskatchewan Saskatoon By Maud Chloé Olivia Ferrari Winter 2009 © Copyright Maud Chloé Olivia Ferrari, 2009. All rights reserved. Permission to Use In presenting this thesis in partial fulfilment of the requirements for a Postgraduate degree from the University of Saskatchewan, I agree that the Libraries of this University may make it freely available for inspection. I further agree that permission for copying of this thesis in any manner, in whole or in part, for scholarly purposes may be granted by the professor or professors who supervised my thesis work or by the Head of the Department or the Dean of the College in which my thesis work was done. It is understood that due recognition shall be given to me and to the University of Saskatchewan in any scholarly use which may be made of any material in my thesis. Requests for permission to copy or to make other use of material in this thesis in whole or part should be addressed to: Head of the Department of Biology 112 Science Place, University of Saskatchewan Saskatoon, Saskatchewan S7N 5E2, Canada i Abstract Many prey species lack innate recognition of their potential predators. Hence, learning is required for them to recognize and respond to predation threats. When wild-caught, these same species may show amazing sophistication in their responses to predator cues. They are able to adjust the intensity of their antipredator responses to a particular predator according to the degree of threat posed by that predator. This ability is therefore acquired through learning. While many studies have shown that prey can learn to respond to predator cues through different learning modes, little is known about what the prey are actually learning. The results presented in this thesis show that learned predator recognition goes beyond the simple labelling of predators as dangerous. Using fathead minnows (Pimephales promelas), woodfrog (Rana sylvatica) tadpoles and boreal chorus frog (Pseudacris maculata) tadpoles, I demonstrated that a one time learning event, either through pairing with alarm cues or through social learning, was enough for prey to learn the level of threat associated with the novel predator cues. I showed that the level of danger associated with the predator cues was determined by the concentration of alarm cues when learning through pairing of alarm cues, or by the intensity of antipredator response displayed by the tutors and by the tutor-to-observer ratio when learning occurred through cultural transmission. Moreover, when subsequently exposed to predator cues, prey adjusted their antipredator responses according to the change in concentration of predator cues between the learning event and the subsequent exposure. Prey displayed stronger antipredator responses when exposed to higher concentrations of predator cues and vice versa. When minnows were provided with conflicting information about the danger level associated with a predator, they displayed a safety strategy and used the most recent information available to respond to predation threats. On a longer time scale, the data also suggest that woodfrog tadpoles are able to learn to respond to predation threats according to the risk posed by the predator at different times of day. Finally, I showed that prey learn to recognize particular characteristics of predators and can generalize their antipredator responses to novel species sharing those characteristics. However, generalization of predator recognition is dependent on the level of risk associated with the predator. Threat-sensitive learning is an extremely complex process shaped by the millions of years of selection imposed by predators on prey. ii Acknowledgements Je dédie cette thèse à mes parents, qui ont supporté tous les choix que j’ai faits au cours de ma vie, incluant celui de déménager au bout du monde pour faire peur à des petits poissons. I dedicate this thesis to my parents, who have supported all the choices I made throughout my life, including moving halfway across the world to scare little fishes. This five-year accomplishment would not have been possible without a number of people, starting with Dr. Grant Brown, who gave me the taste of science and research and convinced me, not without great difficulty, to move to the flat, empty and cold Prairies of Saskatchewan for a Master‟s degree. I would like to thank Dr. François Messier for his help, support and guidance as a supervisor, and for the influence he had on me as a teacher. François endowed me with a deep understanding of study design and biostatistics, which doubtlessly contributed to my success as a scientist. Merci d‟avoir accepté de prendre la relève en tant que superviseur, et m‟avoir donné les moyens et la liberté de recherche dont j‟avais besoin pour réussir. I thank my committee members, Drs. Grant Brown, Cedric Gillott, Jack Gray, Dick Neal and Joe Stookey, as well as my external examiner, Dr. Dan Blumstein, for their effort on my thesis work. A special thank you goes to Joe for his amazing enthusiasm for science and his infectious optimism in life, which inspired me for the past 3 years. I would like to thank Mike and Robyn Pollock for their advice, help and support during the past 5 years. Thanks to all my lab mates, who made this a fun experience. I would also like to thank Jean and Glen Chivers for letting me invade their home and wetlands for three field seasons of fun with the frogs and the mosquitoes. Finally, I would like to thank my family. Merci papa et maman pour m‟avoir fait confiance. Merci à tous les Olivier, spécialement ma mémé Dédé, pour leur support et encouragements au cours des années, sans oublier mon pépé Maurice, qui aurait été très fier de moi. Merci à la Minette Agen de Pouzillac, Filou et Titine pour m‟avoir enseigné quelques principes importants de la vie. Last but not least, thanks to Doug, for being the only person I know, who is as excited as I am about things eating other things. Thank you for your help, patience, guidance and support, for enabling me and for teaching me an important lesson about science: shit happens. Research funding for the work presented in this thesis was provided by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada Discovery Grants to D.P. Chivers and F. Messier. Additional funding was provided by the 2005 R.J.F. Smith Memorial Research Grant Award. Personal funding was provided by the University of Saskatchewan, the Malcolm A. Ramsay Memorial Award and the NSERC Discovery Grant to François Messier. All the work reported herein followed UCACS protocols no. 19920077, 20050136, 20060014, 20070083. iii Table of Contents Permission to Use ............................................................................................................................ i Abstract ........................................................................................................................................... ii Acknowledgments.......................................................................................................................... iii Table of Contents ........................................................................................................................... iv List of Tables ................................................................................................................................. vi List of Figures ............................................................................................................................... vii Chapter 1: Introduction ................................................................................................................... 1 1.1. General information ............................................................................................................. 1 1.2. Threat-sensitive predator avoidance..................................................................................... 2 1.3. Learned predator recognition ............................................................................................... 3 1.4. Study systems ....................................................................................................................... 7 1.5. Research objectives .............................................................................................................. 9 1.6. Anticipated significance ..................................................................................................... 10 Chapter 2: The development of threat-sensitive predator avoidance through pairing of predator odour and alarm cues. ................................................................................................................... 12 2.1. Introduction ........................................................................................................................ 12 2.2. Methodology ...................................................................................................................... 14 2.2.1. Prey learn to match their response intensity to a novel threat according to alarm cue concentrations ........................................................................................................................ 16 2.2.2. Prey learn to match their response intensity to a novel threat according to predator
Recommended publications
  • Copyrighted Material
    Index INDEX Note: page numbers in italics refer to fi gures, those in bold refer to tables and boxes. abducens nerve 55 activity cycles 499–522 inhibition 485 absorption effi ciency 72 annual patterns 515, 516, 517–22 interactions 485–6 abyssal zone 393 circadian rhythms 505 prey 445 Acanthaster planci (Crown-of-Thorns Starfi sh) diel patterns 499, 500, 501–2, 503–4, reduction 484 579 504–7 aggressive mimicry 428, 432–3 Acanthocybium (Wahoo) 15 light-induced 499, 500, 501–2, 503–4, aggressive resemblance 425–6 Acanthodii 178, 179 505 aglomerular 52 Acanthomorpha 284–8, 289 lunar patterns 507–9 agnathans Acanthopterygii 291–325 seasonal 509–15 gills 59, 60 Atherinomorpha 293–6 semilunar patterns 507–9 osmoregulation 101, 102 characteristics 291–2 supra-annual patterns 515, 516, 517–22 phylogeny 202 distribution 349, 350 tidal patterns 506–7 ventilation 59, 60 jaws 291 see also migration see also hagfi shes; lampreys Mugilomorpha 292–3, 294 adaptive response 106 agnathous fi shes see jawless fi shes pelagic 405 adaptive zones 534 agonistic interactions 83–4, 485–8 Percomorpha 296–325 adenohypophysis 91, 92 chemically mediated 484 pharyngeal jaws 291 adenosine triphosphate (ATP) 57 sound production 461–2 phylogeny 292, 293, 294 adipose fi n 35 visual 479 spines 449, 450 adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) 92 agricultural chemicals 605 Acanthothoraciformes 177 adrianichthyids 295 air breathing 60, 61–2, 62–4 acanthurids 318–19 adult fi shes 153, 154, 155–7 ammonia production 64, 100–1 Acanthuroidei 12, 318–19 death 156–7 amphibious 60 Acanthurus bahianus
    [Show full text]
  • CONDITIONED ALARM BEHAVIOR in FATHEAD MINNOWS (Pimephales Promelas) RESULTING from ASSOCIATION of CHEMICAL ALARM PHEROMONE with a NONBIOLOGICAL VISUAL STIMULUS
    Journal of Chemical Ecology, Vol. 25, No. 12, 1999 CONDITIONED ALARM BEHAVIOR IN FATHEAD MINNOWS (Pimephales promelas) RESULTING FROM ASSOCIATION OF CHEMICAL ALARM PHEROMONE WITH A NONBIOLOGICAL VISUAL STIMULUS WARREN K. YUNKER, DAN E. WEIN, and BRIAN D. WISENDEN* Department of Biological Sciences University of Alberta Edmonton, Alberta, Canada T6G 2E9 (Received April 20, 1998; accepted July 31, 1999) Abstract—Fathead minnows (Pimephales promelas) adopt antipredator (alarm) behavior when they detect alarm pheromone released from an injured conspecific. This is an adaptive response since alarm pheromone is generally released only in the context of a predation event. Alarm reactions may also occur in response to chemical and visual stimuli that minnows learn to associate with release of alarm pheromone. Here, we tested if fathead minnows can learn to associate a nonbiological, visual stimulus with predation risk. Minnows were simultaneously exposed to red light and conspecific alarm pheromone, inducing an alarm reaction. When retested using red light alone, small shoals of minnows displayed an antipredator response: dashing movements and disorganized swimming followed by decreased height in the water column and increased shoal cohesion. This resulted from a single-trial exposure to the combined cues and demonstrates a robust ecological mechanism by which minnows learn to recognize indicators of predation risk that may vary in space and time. However, learning to associate risk with biologically irrelevant stimuli may be an ecological liability. How minnows discern between relevant and irrelevant stimuli in nature is not known. Key Words—Fathead minnow, Pimephales promelas, alarm pheromone, Schreckstoff, learned recognition of predation risk, red light. * To whom correspondence should be addressed at Department of Biology, Moorhead State Univer- sity, 1104 7th St.
    [Show full text]
  • Diversity and Phylogeny of Neotropical Electric Fishes (Gymnotiformes)
    Name /sv04/24236_u11 12/08/04 03:33PM Plate # 0-Composite pg 360 # 1 13 Diversity and Phylogeny of Neotropical Electric Fishes (Gymnotiformes) James S. Albert and William G.R. Crampton 1. Introduction to Gymnotiform Diversity The evolutionary radiations of Neotropical electric fishes (Gymnotiformes) pro- vide unique materials for studies on the evolution of specialized sensory systems and the diversification of animals species in tropical ecosystems (Hopkins and Heiligenberg 1978; Heiligenberg 1980; Heiligenberg and Bastian 1986; Moller 1995a; Crampton 1998a; Stoddard 1999; Albert 2001, 2002). The teleost order Gymnotiformes is a clade of ostariophysan fishes most closely related to cat- fishes (Siluriformes), with which they share the presence of a passive electro- sensory system (Fink and Fink 1981, 1996; Finger 1986). Gymnotiformes also possess a combined electrogenic–electroreceptive system that is employed for both active electrolocation, the detection of nearby objects that distort the self- generated electric field, and also electrocommunication, the signaling of identity or behavioral states and intentions to other fishes (Carr and Maler 1986). Active electroreception allows gymnotiforms to communicate, navigate, forage, and ori- ent themselves relative to the substrate at night and in dark, sediment-laden waters, and contributes to their ecological success in Neotropical aquatic eco- systems (Crampton and Albert 2005). The species-specific electric signals of gymnotiform fishes allow investigations of behavior and ecology that are simply unavailable in other groups. Because these signals are used in both navigation and mate recognition (i.e., prezygotic reproductive isolation) they play central roles in the evolutionary diversification and ecological specialization of species, as well as the accumulation of species into local and regional assem- blages.
    [Show full text]
  • In Praise of Fishes: Précis of What a Fish Knows (Balcombe 2016)
    Balcombe, Jonathan (2016) In praise of fishes: Précis of What a fish knows (Balcombe 2016). Animal Sentience 8(1) DOI: 10.51291/2377-7478.1112 This article has appeared in the journal Animal Sentience, a peer-reviewed journal on animal cognition and feeling. It has been made open access, free for all, by WellBeing International and deposited in the WBI Studies Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Animal Sentience 2016.095: Balcombe on Fishes Know [Editorial Note: Multiple invited reviews of this book will be appearing in upcoming issues of Animal Sentience, together with the author’s Response.] Multiple Book Review: Animal Sentience publishes multiple reviews of selected books whose Précis is published in the journal along with the reviews and the author’s response(s). Most of the multiple book reviews are invited individually by the Editor but freely submitted reviews of the selected books whose Précis has appeared can also be considered for publication: Please write to the editor to request a review copy. Books can also be nominated and reviewers can be proposed by writing to the Editor. Instructions: http://animalstudiesrepository.org/animsent/guidelines.html In praise of fishes Précis of What a fish knows (Balcombe 2016) Jonathan Balcombe Humane Society Institute for Science and Policy The Humane Society of the United States Abstract: Our relationship to fishes in the modern era is deeply problematic. We kill and consume more of them than any other group of vertebrates. At the same time, advances in our knowledge of fishes and their capabilities are gaining speed.
    [Show full text]
  • 12 Electroreception and Electrogenesis
    2022_C012.fm Page 431 Tuesday, June 7, 2005 4:03 PM Electroreception and 12 Electrogenesis James S. Albert and William G.R. Crampton CONTENTS I. Introduction to Electroreception and Electrogenesis ........................................................432 II. Phylogeny of Electroreception and Electrogenesis...........................................................433 III. Passive Electroreception ....................................................................................................437 A. Electroreception and Mechanoreception: Vertebrate Laterosensory Systems..........438 B. Electroreception in Lampreys....................................................................................438 C. Electroreception in Gnathostomes.............................................................................439 1. Electroreception in Chondrichthyans ..................................................................439 2. Passive Electroreception in Electric Skates and Rays ........................................440 3. Nonteleost Bony Fishes.......................................................................................441 D. Electroreception in Teleosts ......................................................................................441 1. Passive Electroreception in Weakly Electrogenic Siluriformes..........................442 2. Transition from Electric Communication to Active Electroreception................442 IV. Active Electroreception......................................................................................................443
    [Show full text]
  • Epidermal 'Alarm Substance'
    Proc. R. Soc. B doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.0709 Published online Epidermal ‘alarm substance’ cells of fishes maintained by non-alarm functions: possible defence against pathogens, parasites and UVB radiation Douglas P. Chivers1,*, Brian D. Wisenden2, Carrie J. Hindman1, Tracy A. Michalak1, Robin C. Kusch1, Susan G. W.Kaminskyj1, Kristin L. Jack1, Maud C. O. Ferrari1, Robyn J. Pollock1, Colin F. Halbgewachs1, Michael S. Pollock1, Shireen Alemadi2, Clayton T. James3, Rachel K. Savaloja2, Cameron P.Goater3, Amber Corwin2, Reehan S. Mirza4, Joseph M. Kiesecker5, Grant E. Brown6, James C. Adrian Jr7, Patrick H. Krone8, Andrew R. Blaustein9 and Alicia Mathis10 1Department of Biology, University of Saskatchewan, 112 Science Place, Saskatoon, Saskatchewan, Canada S7N 5E2 2Department of Biosciences, Minnesota State University Moorhead, Moorhead, MN 56563, USA 3Department of Biological Sciences, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, Alberta, Canada T1K 3M4 4Department of Biology, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA 16802-5301, USA 5The Nature Conservancy, Wyoming Field Office, 258 Main Street, Suite 200, Lander, WY 82520, USA 6Department of Biology, Concordia University, 7141 Sherbrooke West, Montreal, Quebec, Canada H4B 1R6 7Department of Chemistry, Union College, 807 Union Street, Schenectady, NY 12308-3107, USA 8Department of Anatomy and Cell Biology, University of Saskatchewan, 107 Wiggins Road, Saskatoon, Saskatchewan, Canada S7N 5E5 9Department of Zoology, Oregon State University, Corvallis, OR 97331, USA 10Department of Biology, Missouri State University, 901 South National Avenue, Springfield, MO 65897, USA Many fishes possess specialized epidermal cells that are ruptured by the teeth of predators, thus reliably indicating the presence of an actively foraging predator. Understanding the evolution of these cells has intrigued evolutionary ecologists because the release of these alarm chemicals is not voluntary.
    [Show full text]
  • Fish Intelligence, Sentience and Ethics
    WellBeing International WBI Studies Repository 1-2015 Fish Intelligence, Sentience and Ethics Culum Brown Macquarie University Follow this and additional works at: https://www.wellbeingintlstudiesrepository.org/acwp_asie Part of the Animal Studies Commons, Comparative Psychology Commons, and the Other Animal Sciences Commons Recommended Citation Brown, C. (2015). Fish intelligence, sentience and ethics. Animal cognition, 18(1), 1-17. This material is brought to you for free and open access by WellBeing International. It has been accepted for inclusion by an authorized administrator of the WBI Studies Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Fish Intelligence, Sentience and Ethics Culum Brown Macquarie University KEYWORDS fish cognition, sentience, welfare, pain, intelligence, ethics ABSTRACT Fish are one of the most highly utilised vertebrate taxa by humans; they are harvested from wild stocks as part of global fishing industries, grown under intensive aquaculture conditions, are the most common pet and are widely used for scientific research. But fish are seldom afforded the same level of compassion or welfare as warm-blooded vertebrates. Part of the problem is the large gap between people’s perception of fish intelligence and the scientific reality. This is an important issue because public perception guides government policy. The perception of an animal’s intelligence often drives our decision whether or not to include them in our moral circle. From a welfare perspective, most researchers would suggest that if an animal is sentient, then it can most likely suffer and should therefore be offered some form of formal protection. There has been a debate about fish welfare for decades which centres on the question of whether they are sentient or conscious.
    [Show full text]
  • Predator Recognition and Anti-Predator Behavior in Juvenile Asian Carp
    PREDATOR RECOGNITION AND ANTI-PREDATOR BEHAVIOR IN JUVENILE ASIAN CARP BY JARED CLAYTON WILSON THESIS Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science in Natural Resources and Environmental Sciences in the Graduate College of the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, 2018 Urbana, Illinois Master’s Committee: Adjunct Professor David H. Wahl, Director of Research Assistant Professor Eric R. Larson Adjunct Assistant Professor Andrew F. Casper ABSTRACT Predation is a determining factor influencing the strength and success of a year class in many prey fishes. Chemical, social, and similar predator cues may all inform an individual of a possible impending predatory threat. Bighead (Hypophthalmichthys nobilis) and silver carp (H. molitrix) widespread invasive fishes, and can be vulnerable to predation during their first year. Because they now overlap the niches of many native prey species, they may shoal with these species and learn about predation while doing so. Additionally, generalizing predators that have similar characteristics (e.g. odors or body morphometry) may increase the survival of an individual. The objective of this study was to evaluate how juvenile bighead and silver carp respond to threatening situations, as well as how adaptive and plastic are these responses. To achieve these objectives, I performed three separate studies that aimed to define the alarm response of these fishes, assess how group composition may influence the alarm response, and determine if the response could be generalized when exposed to taxonomically similar predators. Bighead and silver carp responded to conspecific alarm cues and both species reduced their activity, however bighead carp did so in much tighter schools than silver carp.
    [Show full text]
  • Origin and Phylogenetic Interrelationships of Teleosts Honoring Gloria Arratia
    Origin and Phylogenetic Interrelationships of Teleosts Honoring Gloria Arratia Joseph S. Nelson, Hans-Peter Schultze & Mark V. H. Wilson (editors) TELEOSTEOMORPHA TELEOSTEI TELEOCEPHALA s. str. Leptolepis Pholidophorus † Lepisosteus Amia †? †? † †Varasichthyidae †Ichthyodectiformes Elopidae More advanced teleosts crown- group apomorphy-based group stem-based group Verlag Dr. Friedrich Pfeil • München Contents Preface ................................................................................................................................................................ 7 Acknowledgments ........................................................................................................................................... 9 Gloria Arratia’s contribution to our understanding of lower teleostean phylogeny and classifi cation – Joseph S. Nelson ....................................................................................... 11 The case for pycnodont fi shes as the fossil sister-group of teleosts – J. Ralph Nursall ...................... 37 Phylogeny of teleosts based on mitochondrial genome sequences – Richard E. Broughton ............. 61 Occipito-vertebral fusion in actinopterygians: conjecture, myth and reality. Part 1: Non-teleosts – Ralf Britz and G. David Johnson ................................................................................................................... 77 Occipito-vertebral fusion in actinopterygians: conjecture, myth and reality. Part 2: Teleosts – G. David Johnson and Ralf Britz ..................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Fishes of the World
    Fishes of the World Fishes of the World Fifth Edition Joseph S. Nelson Terry C. Grande Mark V. H. Wilson Cover image: Mark V. H. Wilson Cover design: Wiley This book is printed on acid-free paper. Copyright © 2016 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc. All rights reserved. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Inc., Hoboken, New Jersey. Published simultaneously in Canada. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, scanning, or otherwise, except as permitted under Section 107 or 108 of the 1976 United States Copyright Act, without either the prior written permission of the Publisher, or authorization through payment of the appropriate per-copy fee to the Copyright Clearance Center, 222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers, MA 01923, (978) 750-8400, fax (978) 646-8600, or on the web at www.copyright.com. Requests to the Publisher for permission should be addressed to the Permissions Department, John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 111 River Street, Hoboken, NJ 07030, (201) 748-6011, fax (201) 748-6008, or online at www.wiley.com/go/permissions. Limit of Liability/Disclaimer of Warranty: While the publisher and author have used their best efforts in preparing this book, they make no representations or warranties with the respect to the accuracy or completeness of the contents of this book and specifically disclaim any implied warranties of merchantability or fitness for a particular purpose. No warranty may be createdor extended by sales representatives or written sales materials. The advice and strategies contained herein may not be suitable for your situation.
    [Show full text]
  • Fish Family Tree Benjamin Walther Undergraduate Lesson Plan 4/25
    Fish Family Tree Benjamin Walther Undergraduate Lesson Plan 4/25/06 This lesson plan is designed to convey the central concepts behind constructing phylogenetic trees using primitive and derived fishes as examples. This lesson was conceived as an activity to engage students during a laboratory session that might accompany a lecture course that has described basic fish anatomy and some background biology of fishes, and thus much of the specific physiological vocabulary has already been introduced. The lesson is geared to either a highschool or introductory undergraduate class level. Central Concept: Phylogenies graphically represent how groups of organisms are related to each other. Phylogenies can be constructed by comparing anatomical structures to determine whether groups are closely related. ONLY shared derived characters (synapomorphies) can be used to create a phylogeny. Vocabulary: Phylogeny (true evolutionary relationship among groups of organisms) Synapomorphy (shared derived character) Homoplasy (non-homologous similarity, due to convergence or reversal) Invitation “Today we’re going to play a game to figure out how a bunch of fish are related to one another. Everyone in this room is now (*shazam*) a different fish species, and you’re all each other’s relatives. Your job is to figure out how you’re related and to create a family tree.” Each student will be given a card with a picture and some information about “themselves” as a fish. Before the cards are distributed, students are instructed to look at their cards, study the picture and the information for a couple minutes and come up with a name for their fish. The names should be as creative and memorable as possible.
    [Show full text]
  • Chemical Alarm Signaling by Reticulate Sculpins, Cottus Perplexus
    Environmental Biology of Fishes 57: 347–352, 2000. © 2000 Kluwer Academic Publishers. Printed in the Netherlands. Chemical alarm signaling by reticulate sculpins, Cottus perplexus Douglas P. Chiversa,b, Matthew H. Puttlitza & Andrew R. Blausteinc aDepartment of Biological Sciences, University of Maine, 5751 Murray Hall, Orono, ME 04469-5751, U.S.A. bPresent address: Department of Biology, 112 Science Place, University of Saskatchewan, Saskatoon S7N 5E2, Canada (e-mail: [email protected]) cDepartment of Zoology, Oregon State University, 3029 Cordley Hall, Corvallis, OR 97331-2914, U.S.A. Received 6 January 1999 Accepted 23 July 1999 Key words: alarm signals, Schreckstoff, chemical cues, anti-predator behavior Synopsis The distribution and extent of chemical alarm signaling systems among some families of fishes, including the Cottidae, remains unclear. In laboratory experiments, we tested whether reticulate sculpins, Cottus perplexus, respond to chemical alarm signals released by injured conspecifics. Sculpins decreased movement following expo- sure to skin extracts from conspecifics, but did not respond to cues of syntopic speckled dace, Rhinichthyes osculus, or allotopic swordtails, Xiphophorous helleri. Additional tests demonstrated that the responses of sculpins to alarm cues were dependent on the hunger level of the test fish. Sculpins deprived of food for 2 days failed to respond to conspecific alarm cues, however, the same individuals fed to satiation did respond to alarm cues. Introduction be common in other groups of fishes, including sticklebacks (Mathis & Smith 1993b, Chivers & Smith A wide diversity of fishes have been found to release 1994, Brown & Godin 1997), gobies (Smith 1989, chemical alarm signals upon detecting or being cap- Smith & Lawrence 1992), darters (Smith 1979, Smith tured by a predator (reviews by Smith 1992, Chivers & 1982), poecilids (Garc´ıa et al.
    [Show full text]