Second-Order Discrimination Learning in Humans: Effect

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Second-Order Discrimination Learning in Humans: Effect SECOND-ORDER DISCRIMINATION LEARNING IN HUMANS: EFFECT OF PHYSICAL ARRANGEMENTS WITH COMPARISON STIMULI A Thesis Presented to the Faculty of California State University, Stanislaus In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Arts in Psychology By Richard Radliff June 2014 CERTIFICATION OF APPROVAL SECOND-ORDER DISCRIMINATION LEARNING IN HUMANS: EFFECT OF PHYSICAL ARRANGEMENTS WITH COMPARISON STIMULI By Richard Radliff Signed Certification of Approval Page is on file with the University Library ___________________________ ______________________ Dr. William Potter Date Associate Professor of Psychology ___________________________ ______________________ Dr. Bruce Hesse Date Professor of Psychology ___________________________ ______________________ Dr. Carey Dempsey Date Assistant Professor of Psychology ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS First and foremost, I would like to thank my thesis chairperson, advisor and friend, Dr. Bill Potter for the many hours he spent assisting in the development of the software necessary to run this study, and for his encouragement when I had come very close to abandoning the project. Second, I would like to thank my research assistants, Lacee Ann Johnson, Omar Garcia, Sandra Oshana, and Zang Xiong. Without their dedication, diligence, and help running participant sessions, the crucial process of collecting the data would not have been completed. Lastly, I would like to thank the CSU Stanislaus graduate committee for allowing me to carry on with the project after a long absence, during which completion of my thesis had become a faint hope. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS PAGE Acknowledgements........................................................................................ iii List of Figures ................................................................................................ v Abstract.......................................................................................................... vi CHAPTER I. Introduction to the Study............................................................ 1 Overview................................................................................ 1 II. Review of the Literature............................................................. 3 Current Research.................................................................... 3 Symbolic Matching to Sample............................................... 5 Second-Order Conditional Discrimination ............................ 8 Human Discrimination Research ........................................... 11 Purpose of Study.................................................................... 15 III. Methodology .............................................................................. 17 Participants............................................................................. 17 Apparatus ............................................................................... 17 Stimuli.................................................................................... 18 Procedure ............................................................................... 21 IV. Results ........................................................................................ 30 First-order Discrimination Training ...................................... 30 Second-Order Baseline .......................................................... 33 Second-Order Discrimination Training ................................. 35 V. Discussion and Summary ........................................................... 41 Limitations ............................................................................ 48 Future Research ..................................................................... 50 Summary................................................................................ 51 References...................................................................................................... 54 iv LIST OF FIGURES FIGURE PAGE 1. Mixed element phase configuration............................................... 25 2. Second-order partitioned configurations........................................ 27 3. Second-order integrated configuration .......................................... 28 4. First-order trials to mastery............................................................ 30 5. Baseline results by condition for participants 1, 2, & 3................. 32 6. Baseline results by condition for participants 4, 5, & 6................. 33 7. Participant 1 and 2 second-order training ...................................... 35 8. Participant 3 and 4 second-order training ...................................... 36 9. Participant 5 and 6 second-order training ...................................... 37 10. Integrated and partitioned comparison configurations ................ 44 v ABSTRACT Conditional discrimination learning in animals has been the source of research for many decades. However, primary factors involving human discrimination learning remain obscure. It is the goal of the current research to investigate the effect physical properties of comparison stimuli have on the learning process in humans. This study extends to humans a line of research that utilized pigeons in second-order discrimination training, conducted on the California State University, Stanislaus campus. Six undergraduate students were recruited to test the effects of second-order comparison arrangements under two conditions. The integrated (superimposed) and partitioned (non-superimposed) conditions were presented after first-order training with 3 elements (color, shape, and pattern). As in the prior research, a symbolic- match-to-sample methodology is used to train, and test, participants in first and second-order conditional discriminations. Results from second-order trials are inconclusive, but demonstrate a pattern of responding which is affected by the comparison stimuli conditions. All participants demonstrated generalization of second-order conditional discrimination from first-order training. Results of first- order training confirmed the results of animal research, where significantly more trials were required to attain mastery with pattern comparisons. vi CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION TO THE STUDY Overview The capacity to adapt to environmental changes, or learn, is a fundamental characteristic of all organisms. Skinner (1953) proposed that learning was a “reassortment of responses in a complex situation” (p. 65). He used this definition in describing principles supporting operant behavior. The underlying suggestion refers to an inherent flexibility to operant behavior. Because operant behavior affects the environment in a way that produces a particular effect, consequence, or reinforcement, the operant is characterized by the attributes upon which reinforcement is contingent (Skinner, 1953). However, the attributes of a behavior can vary and/or adapt. The variability of behavior may lead to alternate reinforcing consequences, further refining operant behavior. Adaptation occurs as a result of this refinement when more effective forms of behavior replace older ones (Skinner, 1953). Changes in the environment also produce changes in our behavior. Accordingly, operants are selected by the consequences (Pierce and Cheney, 2004). This relation between behavioral variability and contingent consequences comprise what may be generically defined as learning. That is, behavior in organisms is shaped, to a greater or lesser degree, by conditions that produce reinforcement in a variety of situations and environments. Examining conditions under which behavioral 1 2 adaptation is promoted or inhibited is fundamental to understanding behavior and learning. Operant behavior can be described without mention of antecedent conditions. However, to understand probabilities of operant behavior requires an understanding of functional connections to stimuli which evoke such behavior (Skinner, 1953). Skinner offers the example of a pigeon, where the presence of a light (stimulus) sets the occasion for a response (stretching the neck) which is followed by reinforcement. In the presence of the light, the pigeon’s neck-stretching is more probable because the light occasions the availability of reinforcement. The behavior is not emitted spontaneously in the presence of the light. The pigeon’s behavior develops from repeated exposure to the antecedent stimulus and subsequent availability of reinforcement. Skinner describes the process through which this occurs as discrimination. This example also describes the basis of the three-term or operant contingency, stimulus-response-consequence (S → R → S r). The process of discrimination is fundamental to most human behaviors. The principle of discrimination suggests that we will respond differentially to dissimilar situations if behavior is reinforced in one situation not another (Pierce and Cheney, 2004). It is through the process of discrimination, and the selection of operant behavior by consequence, that complex behaviors develop. Verbal behavior is a prime example of a complex behavior that is shaped by these principles. As Skinner (1957) notes, the selection of verbal behavior by contingent social consequences is a significant process fundamental to human communication. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE Current Research In order to understand complex behaviors it is essential to examine the most fundamental aspects first. A review of behavior research reveals a substantial amount of material involving discrimination behavior (e.g., Blough, 1959; Carter
Recommended publications
  • CHAPTER Personality TEN Psychology
    10/14/08 CHAPTER Personality TEN Psychology Psychology 370 Sheila K. Grant, Ph.D. SKINNER AND STAATS: Professor California State University, Northridge The Challenge of Behaviorism Chapter Overview Chapter Overview RADICAL BEHAVIORISM: SKINNER Part IV: The Learning Perspective PSYCHOLOGICAL BEHAVIORISM: STAATS Illustrative Biography: Tiger Woods Reinforcement Behavior as the Data for Scientific Study The Evolutionary Context of Operant Behavior Basic Behavioral Repertoires The Rate of Responding The Emotional-Motivational Repertoire Learning Principles The Language-Cognitive Repertoire Reinforcement: Increasing the Rate of Responding The Sensory-Motor Repertoire Punishment and Extinction: Decreasing the Rate of Situations Responding Additional Behavioral Techniques Psychological Adjustment Schedules of Reinforcement The Nature-Nurture Question from the Applications of Behavioral Techniques Perspective of Psychological Behaviorism Therapy Education Radical Behaviorism and Personality Theory: Some Concerns Chapter Overview Part IV: The Learning Perspective Personality Assessment from a Ivan Pavlov: Behavioral Perspective Heuristic Accendental Discovery The Act-Frequency Approach to Personality Classical Conditioning Measurement John B. Watson: Contributions of Behaviorism to Personality Theory and Measurement Early Behaviorist B. F. Skinner: Radical Behaviorism Arthur Staats: Psychological Behaviorism 1 10/14/08 Conditioning—the process of learning associations . Ivan Pavlov Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning . 1849-1936
    [Show full text]
  • Learning Theory of Conditioning
    JOURNAL OF CRITICAL REVIEWS ISSN- 2394-5125 VOL 7, ISSUE 08, 2020 Learning Theory of Conditioning Hasan Basri1, Shofia Amin2, Mirsa Umiyati3, Hamid Mukhlis4, Rita Irviani5 1Universitas Islam 45, Bekasi, Indonesia. 2Universitas Jambi, Jambi, Indonesia. 3Universitas Warmadewa, Bali, Indonesia. 4Universitas Aisyah Pringsewu, Lampung, Indonesia 5STMIK Pringsewu, Lampung, Indonesia. E-mail: [email protected], [email protected], [email protected] Received: 11.03.2020 Revised: 12.04.2020 Accepted: 28.05.2020 ABSTRACT: This paper presents learning theory of conditioning. According to conditioning theory, learning is a process of change that occurs because of the conditions which then cause a reaction. To make that person study, we must give certain conditions. The most important thing in learning according to conditioning theory is continuous practice. Priority in this theory is learning that occurs automatically. This theory says that all human behavior is also the result of conditioning, that is the result of training or habit of reacting to certain conditions or stimuli experienced in life. The weakness of this theory is that learning only happens automatically and activeness and personal determination in certain learning such as learning about certain skills and habituation in young children. KEYWORDS: learning theory, conditioning, children, student, learning process I. INTRODUCTION In the learning process the priority is how the individual can adapt to environmental stimuli and then this individual can react. The reaction is an attempt to create activities as well as finish them, and finally get result that make changes to the individual as a new thing and increase knowledge. The behavioral change must be in the form of repetitive stimuli that are beneficial to individual and have a positive value in learning new thing.
    [Show full text]
  • Tutorial Stimulus Control: Part II James A
    The Behavior Analyst 1995, 18, 253-269 No. 2 (Fall) Tutorial Stimulus Control: Part II James A. Dinsmoor Indiana University The second part of my tutorial stresses the systematic importance of two parameters of discrimi- nation training: (a) the magnitude of the physical difference between the positive and the negative stimulus (disparity) and (b) the magnitude of the difference between the positive stimulus, in par- ticular, and the background stimulation (salience). It then examines the role these variables play in such complex phenomena as blocking and overshadowing, progressive discrimination training, and the transfer of control by fading. It concludes by considering concept formation and imitation, which are important forms of application, and recent work on equivalence relations. Key words: stimulus control, disparity, salience, blocking, overshadowing, transfer, fading, con- cept formation, imitation, equivalence relations The first part of this tutorial dealt a positive correlation between the S + with the basic principles that account and the primary reinforcer that selects for the acquisition of stimulus control the one relevant stimulus out of the under what is conventionally known as many that impinge upon the organism discrimination training. Among the sa- and transforms it into a conditioned re- lient points that were raised were sug- inforcer of observing behavior. Note gestions that (a) control by antecedent also that if the subject is exposed ex- stimuli is just as important in operant clusively to S + training or to a single as it is in respondent behavior; (b) Pav- period of S + training followed by a lov's conditional stimulus is a discrim- single period of S - training rather than inative stimulus; (c) stimulus general- to a continued alternation, control is ization is not a behavioral process in- less adequate (Honig, Thomas, & Gutt- dependent of and antagonistic to dis- man, 1959; Yarczower & Switalski, crimination but is simply another way 1969).
    [Show full text]
  • Configural Associations in Discrimination Learning
    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1990 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Animal Behavior Processes 0097-7403/90,r$00.75 1990. Vol. 16, No. 3, 250-261 Configural Associations in Discrimination Learning John M. Pearce and Paul N. Wilson University of Wales, College of Cardiff, Cardiff, Wales In 4 experiments, Sprague-Dawley rats and homing pigeons received training with an A+ AB0 BC+ discrimination, in which food (+) accompanied trials with A and EtC. Food was not presented (0) on trials with the compound AB. Subsequent test trials revealed that responding during C by itself, or the compound ABC, was slower than during either A or BC. Responding during the ABC compound was also found to be slower after training with the A+ AB0 BC+ than an A0 AB+ BC+ discrimination. We argue that these findings demonstrate the importance of configural associations in discrimination learning. Two accounts for the way in which these associations exert their influence are considered. A major concern relating to theories of conditioning has Saavedra, 1975; Whitlow & Wagner, 1972; Woodbury, 1943~ been to identify the associations that are formed when a For example, animals can solve a negative patterning problem compound conditioned stimulus (CS) is paired with an un- in which the US follows two stimuli when they are presented conditioned stimulus (US). By far the most influential line of separately but not when they are presented together (Bel- theorizing has been to assume that this training provides the lingham et al., 1985; Rescorla, 1972; Woodbury, 1943). Ac- potential for each stimulus within the compound to enter cording to the Rescorla and Wagner (1972) model, without separately into an association with the US (Frey & Sears, the influence of confignral cues, this discrimination would be 1978; Mackintosh, 1975; Moore & Stickney, 1980; Pearce & insoluble, because it is impossible for a CS by itself to signal Hall, 1980; Rescorla & Wagner, 1972).
    [Show full text]
  • The Neuro-Philosophy of Archetype in Visual Aesthetics: from Plato to Zeki and Beyond
    D. Mukhopadhyay / PsyArt 18 (2014) 340–369 The neuro-philosophy of archetype in visual aesthetics: from Plato to Zeki and beyond Dyutiman Mukhopadhyay Indian Institute of Technology Abstract The definition of ‘Archetype’ typically refers to an original which has been imitated. The origin of the concept of Archetype in the traditional sense of the term refers to the primitive, universal perceptual imprint – a theory that dates back to Plato. The idea of the archetypal image is conceptually integrated with the aesthetics of visual arts and discussed under the guise of widely researched equivalent terms. The philosophical and scientific propositions on the concept of Archetype with reference to visual arts tended to have a neurological constitution even from ancient times. Starting from the 1990s, a radical outlook emerged in the field of cognitive aesthetics in the form of ‘neuroaesthetics’ which shows a significant potential in dealing with the problem of construction of the symbolic system in visual arts. Does the represented image of an aesthetically appealing artwork have structured within it the roots of an archetype? Is it innately constructed? And finally, is there at all any difference between pattern recognition among humans and other animals and the philosophical descriptions of Ideal, Form and Archetype? The review tries to interpret the development To cite as Mukhopadhyay. D., 2014, ‘The neuro-philosophy of archetype in visual aesthetics: from Plato to Zeki and beyond’, PsyArt 18, pp. 340–369. I. Introduction ‘...forms do not have an existence without a brain.’ (Zeki 1998) ‘...it is possible that some types of art...are activating brain mechanisms in such a way as to tap into...certain innate form primitives which we do not yet fully understand.’ (Ramachandran and Hirstein 1999) 340 D.
    [Show full text]
  • Classical Fear Conditioning in the Anxiety Disorders
    ARTICLE IN PRESS Behaviour Research and Therapy 43 (2005) 1391–1424 www.elsevier.com/locate/brat Classical fear conditioningin the anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis$ Shmuel Lisseka,Ã, Alice S. Powersb, Erin B. McClurea, Elizabeth A. Phelpsc, Girma Woldehawariata, Christian Grillona, Daniel S. Pinea aMood and Anxiety Disorders Program, National Institute of Mental Health, 15K North Drive, Bldg 15k, MSC 2670, Bethesda, MD 20892-2670, USA bDepartment of Psychology, St. John’s University, USA cDepartment of Psychology, New York University, USA Received 23 October 2003; received in revised form 11 October 2004; accepted 18 October 2004 Abstract Fear conditioningrepresents the process by which a neutral stimulus comes to evoke fear followingits repeated pairingwith an aversive stimulus. Althoughfear conditioninghas longbeen considered a central pathogenic mechanism in anxiety disorders, studies employing lab-based conditioning paradigms provide inconsistent support for this idea. A quantitative review of 20 such studies, representingfear-learningscores for 453 anxiety patients and 455 healthy controls, was conducted to verify the aggregated result of this literature and to assess the moderatinginfluences of study characteristics. Results point to modest increases in both acquisition of fear learningand conditioned respondingduringextinction amonganxiety patients. Importantly, these patient-control differences are not apparent when lookingat discrimination studies alone and primarily emerge from studies employing simple, single-cue paradigms where only danger cues are presented and no inhibition of fear to safety cues is required. Published by Elsevier Ltd. Keywords: Classical conditioning; Fear conditioning; Anxiety disorders; Psychophysiology $This work was supported by ‘Intramural Research Program of the National Institute of Mental Health’. ÃCorrespondingauthor. Tel.: +1 301 402 7219; fax: +1 301 402 6353.
    [Show full text]
  • Progress Test 2: Unit 6: Learning
    PROGRESS TEST 2: UNIT 6: LEARNING MULTIPLE CHOICE QUESTIONS 1. During extinction, the ___________ is omitted; as a result, the 7. In distinguishing between negative reinforcers and punishment, __________ seems to disappear. we note that: A. US, UR A. punishment, but not negative reinforcement, involves B. CS; CR use of an aversive stimulus. C. US; CR B. in contrast to punishment, negative reinforcement D. CS; UR decreases the likelihood of a response by the presentation of an aversive stimulus. 2. In Watson and Rayner’s experiment, the loud noise was the C. in contrast to punishment, negative reinforcement __________ and the white rat was the __________. increases the likelihood of a response by the A. CS; CR presentation of an aversive stimulus. B. US; CS D. in contrast to punishment, negative reinforcement C. CS; US increases the likelihood of a response by the D. CS; UR termination of an aversive stimulus. 3. In which of the following may classical conditioning play a role? 8. The “piecework,” or commission, method of payment is an A. emotional problems example of which reinforcement schedule? B. the body’s immune system A. fixed-interval C. helping drug addicts B. variable-interval D. all of the above C. fixed-ratio D. variable-ratio 4. Shaping is a(n)____________ technique for ____________ a behavior. 9. Putting on your coat when it is cold outside is a behavior that is A. operant; establishing maintained by: B. operant; suppressing A. discrimination learning. C. respondent; establishing B. punishment . D. respondent; suppressing C. negative reinforcement. D. classical conditioning. 5.
    [Show full text]
  • Conditioning Methods for Animals in Agriculture: a Review 359
    Conditioning methods for animals in agriculture: a review 359 DOI: 10.1590/1089-6891v17i341981 PRODUÇÃO ANIMAL CONDITIONING METHODS FOR ANIMALS IN AGRICULTURE: A REVIEW MÉTODOS DE CONDICIONAMENTO PARA ANIMAIS EM AGRICULTURA: UMA REVISÃO Charles Ira Abramson1* Emily Kieson¹ 1Laboratory of Comparative Psychology and Behavioral Biology, Departments of Integrative Biology and Psychology, Oklahoma State University, Stillwater, OK , USA. * Corresponding author - [email protected]. Abstract This article briefly describes different conditioning techniques used to help understand learning in farm livestock and economically important animals. A basic overview of conditioning is included along with the importance of different conditioning methods, associative and non-associative learning, and how these principles apply to chickens, horses, cows, goats, pigs, and sheep. Additional information on learning theory specific for each animal is also provided. Keywords: cattle; classical; conditioning; goats; horses; operant; pigs; sheep; training. Resumo Este artigo descreve brevemente diferentes técnicas de condicionamento usadas para ajudar a compreender a aprendizagem em animas de criação e animais economicamente importantes. Uma visão geral básica de condicionamento está incluída juntamente com a importância de diferentes métodos de condicionamento, aprendizagens associativas e não-associativas e como esses princípios se aplicam para as galinhas, cavalos, vacas, suínos, caprinos e ovinos. Informações adicionais sobre a teoria de aprendizagem específica para cada animal também são fornecidas. Palavras-chave: bovinos; caprinos; cavalos; clássico; condicionamento; operante; ovinos; suínos; treinamento. Received on: June, 15th, 2016 Accepted on: June, 20th, 2016 Introduction The senior author has been conducting research on animal behavior in Brazil since the late 1990s. During this time he has had the opportunity to interact with many Brazilian researchers interested in the behavior of farm animals.
    [Show full text]
  • Biconditional Discrimination Learning in Rats with 192 Igg-Saporin Lesions of the Nucleus Basalis Magnocellularis
    California State University, San Bernardino CSUSB ScholarWorks Theses Digitization Project John M. Pfau Library 2006 Biconditional discrimination learning in rats with 192 IgG-saporin lesions of the nucleus basalis magnocellularis Michael Ryan Kitto Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.lib.csusb.edu/etd-project Part of the Biological Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Kitto, Michael Ryan, "Biconditional discrimination learning in rats with 192 IgG-saporin lesions of the nucleus basalis magnocellularis" (2006). Theses Digitization Project. 3002. https://scholarworks.lib.csusb.edu/etd-project/3002 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the John M. Pfau Library at CSUSB ScholarWorks. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses Digitization Project by an authorized administrator of CSUSB ScholarWorks. For more information, please contact [email protected]. BICONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATION LEARNING IN RATS WITH 192 IgG-SAPORIN LESIONS OF THE NUCLEUS BASALIS MAGNOCELLULARIS A Thesis Presented to the Faculty of California State University, San Bernardino In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Arts in Psychology: General-Experimental by Michael Ryan Kitto September 2006 BICONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATION LEARNING IN RATS WITH 192 IgG-SAPORIN LESIONS OF THE NUCLEUS BASALTS MAGNOCELLULARIS A Thesis Presented to the Faculty of California State University, San Bernardino by Michael Ryan Kitto September 2006 Approved by: Dr. Allen E. Butt, Chair, Psychology Date Dr. Robert Cramer Dr. Sanders A. McDougall ABSTRACT The current experiment tested the hypothesis that 192 IgG-saporin lesions of the nucleus basalis magnocellularis (NBM) in rats would impair performance in a biconditional visual discrimination task, which requires configural association learning.
    [Show full text]
  • Influences of John B. Watson's Behaviorism on Child Psychology
    REVISTA MEXICANA DE ANÁLISIS DE LA CONDUCTA 2013 NÚMERO 2 (SEPTIEMBRE) MEXICAN JOURNAL OF BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS VOL. 39, 48-80 NUMBER 2 (SEPTEMBER) INFLUENCES OF JOHN B. WATSON’S BEHAVIORISM ON CHILD PSYCHOLOGY LAS INFLUENCIAS DEL CONDUCTISMO DE JOHN B. WATSON EN LA PSICOLOGÍA INFANTIL HAYNE W. REESE WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY Abstract Watson’s 1913 manifesto, and later elaborations of it, changed child psychology into a natural science based on experimental research and stimulus-response theorizing. These influences probably resulted partly from the philosophical and theoretical at- tractiveness of a natural science approach, partly from the objectivity and persuasive- ness of an experimental approach, and partly from misunderstandings and misrepresentations of his behaviorism. These points are discussed in the first two ma- jor sections of this paper, respectively on Watson’s influence on child psychology in general and, as a concrete illustration, his influence specifically in the domain of emo- tions and emotional development. The latter section shows, for example, that misin- terpretations of Watson’s theory of emotions led to many experimental investigations in an area that had been overwhelmingly nonexperimental. The final section is a ru- minative summary in that its conclusions come largely from considerations given in the first two sections but also partly from considerations not covered there. Keywords: John B. Watson, behaviorism, child psychology, emotions, emotional development Resumen El manifiesto de Watson de 1913, y las elaboraciones posteriores de éste, cam- biaron la psicología infantil a una ciencia natural basada en investigación experi- mental y la elaboración de teorías de estímulo-respuesta. Estas influencias Hayne W.
    [Show full text]
  • DISCRIMINATION LEARNING and ATTENTION (1972) Sunnie D. Kidd
    DISCRIMINATION LEARNING AND ATTENTION (1972) Sunnie D. Kidd James W. Kidd “Discrimination implies a capacity for variability in behavior and a correlation between changes in behavior and changes in environmental stimulation. The process by which these correlations arise, where they otherwise were absent, is called discrimination learning” (Trabasso and Bower, p. 1). In experimental analysis there are four types of discrimination situations which share the common feature that: two stimuli which are sufficiently similar that generalization between them occurs are treated differently. The organism must learn to distinguish between them and respond differently. Actually, any number of stimuli which are similar might be held within the framework of several types of discrimination situations existing in complex combinations; the situation need not be restricted to only two stimuli to hold true, although since this is the simplest illustration of the basic processes involved, it is most commonly used for demonstration. “Discrimination learning is the process resulting from differential reinforcement of somewhat similar stimuli” (Logan, p. 137). In the classical case, one CS might be followed by one US while a second CS is followed by another US. For simplicity, however, studies are generally limited to the case of differential reinforcement of something versus nothing. The four contexts in which the process of discrimination learning can be studied are: 1) Differential Classical Conditioning: When two conditioned stimuli occur in an irregular and unpredictable order and one is followed by the unconditioned stimulus and the other is not, this is differential classical conditioning. Pavlov, in a number of experiments, trained dogs to salivate when another member of the same class was presented.
    [Show full text]
  • Preserved Learning in Monkeys with Medial Temporal Lesions: Sparing of Motor and Cognitive Skills’
    0270-6474/84/0404-1072$02.00/O The Journal of Neuroscience Copyright 0 Society for Neuroscience Vol. 4, No. 4, pp. 1072-1085 Printed in U.S.A. April 1984 PRESERVED LEARNING IN MONKEYS WITH MEDIAL TEMPORAL LESIONS: SPARING OF MOTOR AND COGNITIVE SKILLS’ STUART ZOLA-MORGAN2 AND LARRY R. SQUIRE Veterans Administration Medical Center, San Diego and Department of Psychiatry, University of California, School of Medicine, La Jolla, California, 92093 December 16, 1982; Revised October 25, 1983; Accepted October 28, 1983 Abstract In an effort to bring into correspondence the findings from human amnesic patients and the findings from monkeys with surgical lesions of those brain regions thought to be affected in the human cases, we have addressed in three experiments the implication of findings that human amnesia spares motor and cognitive skills. In the first experiment, monkeys with conjoint lesions of hippocampus and amygdala (H-A), which reproduced the surgical removal sustained by the noted amnesic case H. M., were only mildly impaired in learning relatively difficult pattern discrimination tasks. Monkeys with lesions of temporal stem matter (TS) were severely impaired on the same tasks, due to an apparent deficiency in visual information processing. In the second experiment, monkeys with H-A lesions were severely impaired at learning relatively easy discrimination tasks that could be acquired rapidly by normal monkeys. Monkeys with TS lesions were not impaired. In the third experiment, monkeys with H-A lesions exhibited normal acquisition of two motor skill tasks. These data can be understood in the light of a distinction between kinds of memory, founded in recent studies of the neuropsychology of human amnesia.
    [Show full text]