The Development of Theory of Mind in Deaf People by Sylvia Glenn

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Development of Theory of Mind in Deaf People by Sylvia Glenn i The Development of Theory of Mind in Deaf People By Sylvia Glenn Thesis submitted to the University of Sheffield for the degree of Doctor of Clinical Psychology Januaq 2007 ii Declaration The Nvorkhas not been submitted for any other degreeor to any other institution iii Structure and Word Counts Literature Review - Prepared according to the guidance of the British Journal of Developmental Psychology Research Report - Preparedaccording to Option B and the guidance of the Journal of Deaf Studiesand Deaf Education' Section I- Literature Review Word Count Literatu.re Review 7995 References 1526 Section Il - Research Report Research Report 8084 References 973 Section III - Critical Appraisal Critical Appraisal 4997 References 106 Section IV - Appendices Appendices 7095 Total word count excluding references and appendices 21076 Total word count including references and appendices 30776 1 Copies of the letter of approval and guidance notes for contributors are included in Section IV: Appendix A 1. iv Thesis Abstract Section I. Literature Review Several authors claim that deaf children with hearing parents do not develop theory of mind (ToM) until adolescence,but this is largely basedon false-belief studies.Peterson & Siegal's (2000) review of false-belief studiesis updated and the section also reviews research using a wider range of methodologies with deaf children and adults. Deaf children have difficulties in false-belief, perspective-taking and emotional responses/reactions,but these skills may develop later. In contrast, deaf children do not appear to have deficits in mental-state attribution or emotional recognition. More researchwith deaf adults is needed. Section H. ResearchReport This study explores whether Deaf adults have impairments in ToM and empathy compared to hearing adults, and if Deaf forensic patients have further impairments. Tests were adaptedand translated for the purpose of the study. The Deaf community scored lower than the hearing community on the Reading the Mind in the Eyes test and produced fewer mental states. The Deaf forensic group was too small for reliable statistical analysis.Deaf people may continue to have ToM impairments into adulthood, but the results could be due to methodological, linguistic and social factors. Assessmentsmust be developed specifically for use with Deaf populations rather than relying on interpretedmeasures. Section III. Critical Appraisal T his section gives a commentary, and personal reflections, on the project. The challengesfaced during the progressionthrough the thesis and of researchingwithin the Deaf culture are explored. Methodological problems, clinical implications and areasfor further researchare discussedand key learning points are identified. V Dedication For Nana Lorna and Dolores vi Acknowledgments Firstly I would like to thank the 25 people who participated in this project for generouslyand freely giving their time to take part. Many thanks to Alpha Hospitals, Bury, who supported the project by providing me access to staff, patients and interpreters. Thanks also to the Sheffield Research Consortium and the Clinical Psychology Unit for funding this project. I would like to express my thanks to my supervisors Prof. Nigel Beail and Dr Sue O'Rourke for their patience, guidance and support through the whole researchprocess, and for giving me the opportunity to do researchin Deafness. I'd like to extend my particular thanks to my two interpreters, Claire Shard and Anna Wil.liams, for all the time, effort and wisdom they brought - especially for sticking with it when it took longer than planned! Many thanks to all the membersof the focus group for your advice, guidance,ideas and feedback throughout the project: SarahPowell, Trevor Bothwick-Hare, Dr Fiona Fitch, Dr Alun Thomas, Claire Shard, Anna Williams, James Lea and Dr Sue O'Rourke. A special thanks to Fiona for taking me under your wing after Sue left. Thank you to all the staff at Alpha for welcoming me - it was great working alongsideyou all. I would like to extend my thanks to the following people: Simon Baron-Cohen and Peter Kinderman for allowing me to use their tests; Adrian Simpson for your patience and for being so approachableand willing to explain things so many times until it sinks in! Dr Kath Boon -a very big thank you for all your support through my training; Dr Gill Crow, Dr Susie Black, Alison Wray and all my colleagues at SCH and West CAMHS - thank you for all your understanding,*support and good humour; Carolyn Lovelock - thank you for introducing me to Alpha. Thank you to everyoneelse who has helped and supportedme along the way! And finally a big thank you to my family and friends for all your support through this long researchprocess; in particular Dad, Hclen and Nan for your love and care, Uncle Brian for proof-reading, Ruth for being a fantastic friend, Alexci for being there, to other trainees for sharing the journey and to all my friends at St Johns church for being a wonderful extended family to me! And of course, thanks to God for always being there and for giving me purpose! vii List of Contents SECTION 1: LITERATURE REVIEW THE DEVELOPMENT OF THEORY OF MIND AND EMOTIONAL UNDERSTANDINGIN DEAF PEOPLE: CONSIDERINGPROCEDURAL ISSUES 1 ABSTRACT 2 DEAFNESS 3 THEORY OF MIND 4 THE CURRENT REVIEW 7 FALSE-BELIEF TESTS 8 OTHER MEASURES OF TOM 13 DISCUSSION 24 CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS 26 CONCLUSIONS 28 REFERENCES 30 SECTION II: RESEARCH REPORT 35 TiIEORY OF MIND AND ENiPATIIY IN DEAF ADULTS IN COMMUNITY AND FORENSIC SETTINGS: AN EXPLORATORY STUDY 35 ABSTRACT 36 METHOD 40 RESULTS 48 DISCUSSION 57 CONCLUSIONS 62 REFERENCES 64 SECTION III: CRITICAL APPRAISAL 67 CRITICAL APPRAISAL 68 ORIGINS OF THE PROJECT 68 DEVELOPMENT OF THE RESEARCH PROPOSAL 69 RESEARCHING IN A HOST-COMM UNITY 70 GAINING APPROVALS 70 CONTRACTING INTERPRETERS 71 ADAPTATION OF MEASURES FOR DEAF POPULATIONS 72 THE CHALLENGES OF RECRUITING CROSS- CULTURALLY 72 MEETING OTHER RESEARCHERS IN DEAFNESS 76 ANALYSIS OF DATA 76 DRAWING CONCLUSIONS 77 WRITING UP AND MAINTAINING MOTIVATION 78 METHODOLOGICAL LIMITATIONS OF THE RESEARCH STUDY 78 CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS 80 FURTHER RESEARCH 82 KEY LEARNING POINTS 83 REFERENCES 86 SECTION IV: APPENDICES 87 APPENDix A 88 LFORMAT 89 2. ETI IICAL APPROVALS 96 3. MEASURES 105 4. OTHER 112 viii APPENDIX B 121 APPENDicEs To LITERATURE REVIEff" 121 APPENDIX C 132 APPENDicEs To RESEA RCII REPOR T 132 Section 1: Literature Review I Running head: DEVELOPMENT OF ToM IN DEAF PEOPLE SECTION 1: Literature Revie-w The Development of Theory of Mind and Emotional Understanding in Deaf People: Considering Procedural Issues Section 1: Literature Review 2 Abstract Basedexclusively on the false-belief paradigm, several authors claim that deaf children with hearing parents do not develop theory of mind (ToM) until adolescence.This section provides an updated review of studies using false-belief tests published since Peterson & Siegal's (2000) review. It also reviews research using a range of methodologies investigating ToM in deaf children since 2000 and deaf adults since 1970. The results of false-belief studiesprovide support for a conversationalaccount for the developmentof ToM, as performanceappears to dependon accessto a fluent signer in early childhood. Narrative methodologies show that deaf children appear to have difficulties in perspective-takingand emotional reactions, but these skills may develop later. In contrast,deaf children produce mental-statesequally well as, if not better than, hearing children. Emotional recognition may also be intact but more studies arc needed to clarify this. More research with deaf adults is needed. Theoretical and clinical implications are discussed. Section1: LiteratureReview 3 The Development of Theory of Mind and Emotional Understanding in Deaf People: Considering Procedural Issues Deafness In the United Kingdom approximately 840 babies are born moderately- to profoundly-deae every year3 (Royal National Institute for the Deaf, 2006). Causesof deafnessinclude genetics, pre- and post-natal complicationsý, head injuries and loud noise (National Deaf Children's Society, 2006), therefore additional impairments are possible. Neonatal screening-programmes for hearing-impairmentS5 improve opportunities for communication developmentthrough hearing aids, cochlea implants, and importantly, adaptations in parental communication. Deaf adult populations may have been undiagnoseduntil a later age6,which may have affected their development. There are two main models of deafness;the medical model aims to reduce the disability of deafness,and the cultural model acceptsdeafness as a part of the person's Deaf identity. Deaf culture uses a distinct language; British Sign Language (BSL) has its own grammatical structure and its signs do not match each English word. For this paper, the term deaf will be used to include the medical condition of deafnessand those who identify themselvesas culturally Deaf. Language choices depend on local provision and family preference: Children taught sign language (SL) either have classesdelivered in sign or use interpreters to access mainstream education. Oral education aims to teach a child to use speech, 2 The level of an individual's deafnessis describedin decibels (dB) by the quietestsound they can hear in their best ear, or by terms of 'mild' (24-40 dB), 'moderate' (41-70 dB), 'severe' (71-95 dB) or 'profound' (>95 dB). 3 The current population of severely- and profoundly-deaf people in the UK is around 698,000 (RNID, 2006). 4Pre-natal causes include rubella,
Recommended publications
  • What Sign Language Creation Teaches Us About Language Diane Brentari1∗ and Marie Coppola2,3
    Focus Article What sign language creation teaches us about language Diane Brentari1∗ and Marie Coppola2,3 How do languages emerge? What are the necessary ingredients and circumstances that permit new languages to form? Various researchers within the disciplines of primatology, anthropology, psychology, and linguistics have offered different answers to this question depending on their perspective. Language acquisition, language evolution, primate communication, and the study of spoken varieties of pidgin and creoles address these issues, but in this article we describe a relatively new and important area that contributes to our understanding of language creation and emergence. Three types of communication systems that use the hands and body to communicate will be the focus of this article: gesture, homesign systems, and sign languages. The focus of this article is to explain why mapping the path from gesture to homesign to sign language has become an important research topic for understanding language emergence, not only for the field of sign languages, but also for language in general. © 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. How to cite this article: WIREs Cogn Sci 2012. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1212 INTRODUCTION linguistic community, a language model, and a 21st century mind/brain that well-equip the child for this esearchers in a variety of disciplines offer task. When the very first languages were created different, mostly partial, answers to the question, R the social and physiological conditions were very ‘What are the stages of language creation?’ Language different. Spoken language pidgin varieties can also creation can refer to any number of phylogenic and shed some light on the question of language creation.
    [Show full text]
  • Sign Order and Argument Structure in a Peruvian Home Sign System
    The Report Committee for Grace Kathleen Neveu Certifies that this is the approved version of the following report: Sign order and argument structure in a Peruvian home sign system APPROVED BY SUPERVISING COMMITTEE: Richard P. Meier, Supervisor David Quinto-Pozos, Co-Supervisor Sign order and argument structure in a Peruvian home sign system by Grace Kathleen Neveu, B.A. REPORT Presented to the Faculty of the Graduate School of The University of Texas at Austin in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of MASTER OF ARTS THE UNIVERSITY OF TEXAS AT AUSTIN May 2016 Acknowledgments I wish to thank Professor Richard P. Meier and Professor David Quinto- Pozos. Thank you both for your guidance and patience during the writing of this report. I thank you for your feedback concerning everything from methodology to writing structure. I am grateful for the opportunity to work under such dedicated and talented academics. I extend my gratitude to the sign language graduate students at UT Austin. Your feedback and suggestions during research meetings were invaluable. My graduate school experience has been immeasurably improved by these talented col- leagues. I reserve my final thanks to RCM, LTN, OMT, ACC and the Ma´´ıj`ık`ı com- munity. The research presented in this paper would not be possible without their willingness to work with me and share their knowledge. iii Sign order and argument structure in a Peruvian home sign system Grace Kathleen Neveu, M.A. The University of Texas at Austin, 2016 Supervisors: Richard P. Meier David Quinto-Pozos Home sign systems are gestural communication systems that arise when a deaf child is deprived of manual communication, but not social interaction.
    [Show full text]
  • Emerging Sign Languages
    Emerging Sign Languages Emerging Sign Languages Irit Meir Department of Hebrew Language And Department of Communication Disorders The University of Haifa 31905 Haifa, Israel Wendy Sandler Department of English Language and Literature The University of Haifa 31905 Haifa, Israel Carol Padden Department of Communication University of California, San Diego La Jolla, CA 92093-0503 Mark Aronoff Department of Linguistics SUNY Stony Brook Stony Brook, NY 11794-4376 Corresponding author: Irit Meir [email protected] * Our work is supported by grants from the National Institute on Deafness and other Communication Disorders (R01 DC 6473) and the Israel Science Foundation (#553/04). 1 Emerging Sign Languages Emerging Sign Languages Irit Meir, Wendy Sandler, Carol Padden and Mark Aronoff <1>Introduction Herodotus tells the story of the Egyptian king Psammetichos’s effort to answer the question, Who were the first people in the world? He placed newborn twins in the custody of a shepherd on an uninhabited island, with instructions not to speak to them. After two years, he returned to learn that the children’s first recognizable word was ‘bekos,' the Phrygian word for bread, and so concluded that the Phrygians were the first. The tale of Psammetichos has long been one of the best remembered of Herodotus’s stories because it strikes a nerve. Language is the most human of all behaviors and it is natural to want to know how it all started. But we cannot go back in time and there is little if any evidence in the fossil record that can tell us about the origin of language.
    [Show full text]
  • I the Emergence of Grammatical Categories in Home Sign: Evidence
    i The Emergence of Grammatical Categories in Home Sign: Evidence from Family-based Gesture Systems in Nicaragua by Marie Coppola Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy Supervised by Professor Elissa L. Newport Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences The College Arts and Sciences University of Rochester Rochester, New York 2002 ii Dedication I dedicate this dissertation to my grandmother, Mary Filomena Faraco Eibel, who always patiently answered my many questions and instilled in me a love of learning. Her models of generosity and dedication will be with me always. iii Curriculum Vitae Marie Coppola was born in Philadelphia, Pennsylvania on December 4, 1968. She attended the Massachusetts Institute of Technology from 1986 to 1990 and graduated with a Bachelor of Science degree in 1990. She came to the University of Rochester in the fall of 1995 and began graduate studies in the field of Brain and Cognitive Sciences. She received a Graduate Research Fellowship from the National Science Foundation in 1996. She pursued her research in the acquisition of sign languages under the direction of Professor Elissa L. Newport and received the Master of Arts degree in 2000. iv Acknowledgments I would like to thank my advisor, Elissa Newport, for her many and varied contributions to this work. I deeply respect her vision for the field of language acquisition, her clarity of thought, and her brilliant insights. Lissa’s confidence in me and in this project often sustained me when my own confidence waned; I could not have completed this dissertation without her patient support.
    [Show full text]
  • Evidence from Homesign and Nicaraguan Sign Language
    Cognition 203 (2020) 104332 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Cognition journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit The communicative importance of agent-backgrounding: Evidence from T homesign and Nicaraguan Sign Language ⁎ Lilia Rissmana, , Laura Hortonb, Molly Flahertye, Ann Senghasf, Marie Coppolag,i, Diane Brentarid,h, Susan Goldin-Meadowc,d a Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin – Madison, 1202 W. Johnson St., Madison, WI 53706, United States of America b Department of Linguistics, University of Texas at Austin, 305 E. 23rd Street, Austin, TX 78712, United States of America c Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, 5848 S. University Ave., Chicago, IL 60637, United States of America d Center for Gesture, Sign, and Language, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL 60637, United States of America e Department of Psychology, Swarthmore College, 500 College Avenue, Swarthmore, PA 19081, United States of America f Department of Psychology, Barnard College, 3009 Broadway, New York, NY 10027, United States of America g Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Connecticut, 406 Babbidge Road, Unit 1020, Storrs, CT 06269, United States of America h Department of Linguistics, University of Chicago, 1115 E. 58th Street, Chicago, IL 60637, United States of America i Department of Linguistics, University of Connecticut, 365 Fairfield Way, Unit 1145, Storrs, CT 06269, United States of America ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT Keywords: Some concepts are more essential for human communication than others. In this paper, we investigate whether Language emergence the concept of agent-backgrounding is sufficiently important for communication that linguistic structures for Semantics encoding this concept are present in young sign languages. Agent-backgrounding constructions serve to reduce Sign languages the prominence of the agent – the English passive sentence a book was knocked over is an example.
    [Show full text]
  • American Sign Language Council’S Own Patch Program
    American Sign Language Council’s Own Patch Program American Sign Language Council’s Own Patch Program Within our culture and community, lives a second culture that very few are familiar with and that is the community of the Deaf. To live in our society that relies primarily on hearing to navigate, the world can become a difficult place to live. To compensate, they use their own language – American Sign Language (ASL) – that is purely visual and has its own unique sentence structure apart from English. This patch program has been designed to educate the participants on the language itself, but also the culture and community that comes along with being Deaf or hard of hearing. To earn this patch, please complete the following number of items from each section: The Basics Communication Deaf Culture Applications Daisy 2 1 1 1 Brownie 2 1 1 1 Junior 3 2 2 2 Cadette 3 2 2 2 Senior 4 3 3 3 Ambassador 4 3 3 3 The Basics Learn the alphabet (Required) o Watch this video: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=a5BD8SjhPSg o Or, take this quiz: https://www.sporcle.com/games/jayrodss7/signlanguage_alphabet o By the end, you should be able to sign the whole alphabet on your own! Learn the numbers 1-10 (Required) o Watch this video: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=0LIV0miyxR8 o By the end, you should be able to sign numbers 1-10 on your own! A Visual Language 101 o While there are specific signs for many words, ASL also requires body movement and facial expressions to indicate the feelings and emotions of what the signer means to say.
    [Show full text]
  • Sign Language
    Sign language Ling 001 - Fall 2014 Jami Fisher - Beatrice Santorini Sign languages . Visual-spatial languages used by communities of Deaf people . Ethnologue lists 130 Deaf sign languages throughout the world . No universal sign language! How do sign languages arise? . Spontaneous emergence • Home sign • Village sign . Some examples • Nicaraguan Sign Language • Al-Sayyid Bedouin Sign Language • Martha’s Vineyard Sign Language . Comparable to the emergence of pidgins and creoles 3 Iconicity and arbitrariness, 1 All human languages consist of • Symbols (= form-meaning pairs) • Rules for possible forms (= phonology) • Rules for combining symbols into larger meaningful units (= morphology, syntax) 4 Iconicity and arbitrariness, 2 . In spoken languages, the form of symbols is mostly arbitrary (onomatopoeia is marginal). In sign languages, iconicity plays a bigger role. But even here, iconicity has its limits. • It underdetermines the form of signs. • Over time, it is lost. 5 TREE - Chinese Sign Language 6 TREE - Danish Sign Language 7 TREE - American Sign Language 8 Limits of iconicity - Synchronic . All three signs invoke the physical shape of the tree (= iconic). But the shape is invoked in different ways (= arbitrary), and the sign is fixed for each language (= conventional). The signer cannot decide to use a different sign – no matter how iconic. In other words, conventionality (and to some extent, arbitrariness) trump iconicity. 9 Limits of iconicity - Diachronic . Iconicity is lost over time. • HOME was a compound of EAT + BED. • In the current citation form, the handshape of BED has assimilated to that of EAT. • Current vernacular forms exhibit further articulatory reduction, making them even less iconic. • Comparable processes are found in spoken language.
    [Show full text]
  • Indian Sign Language: a Linguistic Analysis of Its Grammar
    Chapter 1 Introduction Despite the presence of deaf people at all times and in all societies, a systematized linguistic, political, and theoretical reflection on deafness and deaf people began to emerge only in the mid-twentieth century. In shaping this project—which has changed both the history of humanity and humanity itself—the study of sign language has played a very sig- nificant role. Unlike the centuries-old tradition of the study of spoken language, the linguistic study of signed language is just about sixty years old; nevertheless, it has had a tremendous impact on linguistic theory. As the Deaf community strives to achieve its human rights in general and linguistic rights in particular, sign language studies have set off social and political reverberations among its members. In this introductory chapter I explore those effects. THOUGHTS ON DEAFNESS, DEAF EDUCATION, AND SIGN LANGUAGE Before 1970, particularly in India, “sign” and “sign language” referred to symbolic gestures used in the theater, dance, secret codes, court eti- quette, and so on, as well as those used by deaf people. Although “almost any statement about signs, gestural communication and sign language of deaf people” (Miles 2001b, 3) is strongly debated by different interest groups, these views relate to deafness, deaf education, and signs used by deaf people rather than to linguistic studies. However, as a result of modern linguistic study of various sign lan- guages, the notion of Indian Sign Language (henceforth, ISL) came to the forefront. This very coinage led to a sense of community formation, solidarity, and linguistic empowerment, and it actuated cultural, politi- cal, educational, and artistic movement in India as well.
    [Show full text]
  • Exploring the Ancestral Roots of American Sign Language: Lexical Borrowing from Cistercian Sign Language and French Sign Language." (2010)
    University of New Mexico UNM Digital Repository Language, Literacy, and Sociocultural Studies ETDs Education ETDs 6-25-2010 EXPLORING THE ANCESTRAL OR OTS OF AMERICAN SIGN LANGUAGE: LEXICAL BORROWING FROM CISTERCIAN SIGN LANGUAGE AND FRENCH SIGN LANGUAGE Keith Martin Cagle Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalrepository.unm.edu/educ_llss_etds Recommended Citation Cagle, Keith Martin. "EXPLORING THE ANCESTRAL ROOTS OF AMERICAN SIGN LANGUAGE: LEXICAL BORROWING FROM CISTERCIAN SIGN LANGUAGE AND FRENCH SIGN LANGUAGE." (2010). https://digitalrepository.unm.edu/ educ_llss_etds/7 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Education ETDs at UNM Digital Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Language, Literacy, and Sociocultural Studies ETDs by an authorized administrator of UNM Digital Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. EXPLORING THE ANCESTRAL ROOTS OF AMERICAN SIGN LANGUAGE: LEXICAL BORROWING FROM CISTERCIAN SIGN LANGUAGE AND FRENCH SIGN LANGUAGE BY KEITH MARTIN CAGLE B.A., Social Work, Rochester Institute of Technology, 1982 M.A., Educational Administration, California State University at Northridge, 1991 DISSERTATION Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy Educational Linguistics The University of New Mexico Albuquerque, New Mexico May 2010 ©2010, Keith Martin Cagle iii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I owe my deepest gratitude to my grandmother Olive E. Brown, my mother LaVerne J. Cagle, and my brother Robert L. Cagle, Jr. for their constant encouragement throughout my education and in my efforts to attain a doctorate degree. With a loving heart, this document is dedicated to them. It is with pleasure that I thank the Supalla Family: Clarence Supalla, Dr.
    [Show full text]
  • Deaf Ni-Vanuatu and Their Signs: a Sociolinguistic Study
    DEAF NI-VANUATU AND THEIR SIGNS: A SOCIOLINGUISTIC STUDY BY JACQUELINE GEORGA ISELI A Thesis submitted to Victoria University of Wellington In fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts in Linguistics by Thesis Victoria University of Wellington 2018 Abstract This thesis provides the first documentation and description of the signs created and used by deaf individuals in Vanuatu. The specific aims of this research were as follows: to establish the sociolinguistic context experienced by deaf people in Vanuatu; to identify the repertoire and characteristics of signs used by the deaf participants; to compare features of participants’ individual signs with the characteristics of home signs and emerging sign languages; and to consider the degree of similarity and potential similarity of signs between participants and how this reflects individuals’ opportunities for contact with other deaf people and signing interlocutors. The limitations of this study are that field methodology for data collection was developed in situ as conditions allowed. The sociolinguistic context for deaf Ni-Vanuatu confirms that language isolation leads to marginalisation from community and society. The study established that these home sign lexicons were limited in quantity and conceptual range, and that shared background knowledge was essential for comprehension. Overall, 22 handshapes were documented, and the predominant handshapes unmarked. Most participants preferred handling strategy for depicting signs. Some evidence of noun-verb distinction was noted in the repertoire of some participants. However, across this range of formational characteristics, results showed significant individual variations. Furthermore, multiple barriers have precluded development of a shared sign language and any form of deaf community.
    [Show full text]
  • Successful Communication Does Not Drive Language Development: Evidence from Adult Homesign ⇑ Emily M
    Cognition 158 (2017) 10–27 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Cognition journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/COGNIT Original Articles Successful communication does not drive language development: Evidence from adult homesign ⇑ Emily M. Carrigan , Marie Coppola University of Connecticut, United States article info abstract Article history: Constructivist accounts of language acquisition maintain that the language learner aims to match a target Received 1 December 2014 provided by mature users. Communicative problem solving in the context of social interaction and Revised 7 July 2016 matching a linguistic target or model are presented as primary mechanisms driving the language devel- Accepted 28 September 2016 opment process. However, research on the development of homesign gesture systems by deaf individuals who have no access to a linguistic model suggests that aspects of language can develop even when typical input is unavailable. In four studies, we examined the role of communication in the genesis of homesign Keywords: systems by assessing how well homesigners’ family members comprehend homesign productions. In Language development Study 1, homesigners’ mothers showed poorer comprehension of homesign descriptions produced by Constructivism Communication their now-adult deaf child than of spoken Spanish descriptions of the same events produced by one of Comprehension their adult hearing children. Study 2 found that the younger a family member was when they first inter- Homesign acted with their deaf relative, the better they understood the homesigner. Despite this, no family member Gesture comprehended homesign productions at levels that would be expected if family members co-generated homesign systems with their deaf relative via communicative interactions. Study 3 found that mothers’ poor or incomplete comprehension of homesign was not a result of incomplete homesign descriptions.
    [Show full text]
  • Pointing in Gesture and Sign
    Chapter forthcoming in: Gesture in language: Development across the lifespan, edited by A. Morgenstern & S. Goldin-Meadow Pointing in gesture and sign Kensy Cooperrider1 Kate Mesh2 1 University of Chicago, Department of Psychology 2 Lund University, Lund University Humanities Lab 1. Introduction Human communication is composite (e.g., Clark 2016; Enfield 2009; Ferrara and Hodge 2018; Holler and Levinson 2019). It involves the voice, face, hands, and rest of the body. It integrates categorical elements and gradient ones; highly conventional and ad hoc forms; arbitrary symbols and motivated signals. This is true of spoken communication and it is true—in equal measure—of signed communication. Both speakers and signers stitch these different types of components into a seamless whole. Some of these components are historically considered a core part of language, others marginal, and still others are thought to be something else entirely— gestural, expressive, paralinguistic (for discussion, see, e.g., Dingemanse 2018; Goldin-Meadow and Brentari 2017; Müller 2018). Regardless of whether one considers the language/ non-language divide fundamental, fuzzy, or fictitious, there is widespread agreement that certain communicative phenomena haunt the boundaries in ways that prove revealing. Chief among these are cases where both speakers and signers make use of the same bodily raw material, but in putatively very different ways: flashes and furrows of the brow; imitations of actions; depictions of size, shape, and arrangement. Here, we analyze one of these similar-looking forms in detail: pointing. The case of pointing shows, first, how a single semiotic tool can be put to many uses and, second, how speakers and signers use this tool in some ways that are similar and other ways that are different.
    [Show full text]