Argument Structure in Language Shift: Morphosyntactic Variation and Grammatical Resilience in Modern Chukchi

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Argument Structure in Language Shift: Morphosyntactic Variation and Grammatical Resilience in Modern Chukchi THE UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO ARGUMENT STRUCTURE IN LANGUAGE SHIFT: MORPHOSYNTACTIC VARIATION AND GRAMMATICAL RESILIENCE IN MODERN CHUKCHI A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF THE DIVISION OF THE HUMANITIES IN CANDIDACY FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY DEPARTMENT OF LINGUISTICS BY JESSICA KANTAROVICH CHICAGO, ILLINOIS AUGUST 2020 Copyright © 2020 by Jessica Kantarovich All Rights Reserved Dedicated to the Chukchi people, who exemplify resilience Table of Contents List of Figures ......................................... ix List of Tables .......................................... x Acknowledgments ....................................... xii Abstract ............................................. xv Glossing Conventions .....................................xvii 1 Introduction ........................................ 1 1.1 The position of endangered language grammars in linguistics . .1 1.1.1 Questions of language contact and change in shifting communities . .2 1.1.2 Mixed grammars and syntactic theory . .5 1.1.3 L1 and L2 acquisition and heritage speech . .7 1.2 The encoding of argument structure in Chukchi . .9 1.3 Findings among modern Chukchi speakers . 13 1.3.1 Significant conclusions beyond Chukchi . 16 1.4 A sociolinguistic introduction to Chukchi . 17 1.4.1 A historical and typological profile of the Chukchi language . 17 1.4.2 Documentation and variation . 19 1.4.3 Social change in the 20th century and its effects on language maintenance . 26 1.4.4 Speaker groups . 31 1.5 Methodology . 35 1.5.1 Targeted text collection . 36 1.5.2 Experimental production tasks . 38 1.5.3 Challenges in employing experimental methods in the field . 39 1.6 A note on phonology and transcription . 43 iv 1.7 A note on Chukchi glosses . 44 1.8 The roadmap . 44 2 Language contact, argument structure, and polysynthesis . 46 2.1 Theories of language contact . 46 2.1.1 Language contact involving Chukchi . 54 2.1.2 Outstanding theoretical questions about language contact . 62 2.2 Argument structure in contact . 66 2.2.1 Expected effects in Chukchi . 71 2.3 Implications for theories of polysynthesis . 74 2.4 Alignment and argument structure of Chukchi . 85 2.4.1 Morphological and syntactic ergativity . 85 2.5 Valency-changing operations . 95 2.5.1 Antipassives . 96 2.5.2 Applicatives . 97 2.5.3 Noun incorporation . 99 2.5.4 Expected changes . 101 2.6 Syntactic operations: the interaction of word order, argument drop, and agreement . 104 2.6.1 Expected changes in degree of polysynthesis . 107 2.6.2 Potential effects of argument drop . 107 2.6.3 Inverse marking . 109 2.6.4 Implications for a feature-copying analysis . 109 2.6.5 Case study: The linguistic system of one Chukchi speaker in Yakutsk . 110 2.7 Conclusion . 116 2.7.1 Other signs of language shift . 116 2.7.2 A broader survey of argument structure . 118 v 3 Ergativity and transitivity phenomena in Modern Chukchi morphology . 119 3.1 Loss and maintenance of ergative patterns . 119 3.1.1 The status of ergativity in Chukchi . 119 3.1.2 Other transitivity phenomena . 122 3.2 Case marking in modern Chukchi . 124 3.2.1 Animacy and noun class membership . 126 3.2.2 Changes to the alignment of core grammatical case marking . 130 3.3 Agreement marking in modern Chukchi . 131 3.3.1 Syntax of agreement marking in traditional Chukchi . 131 3.3.2 Alignment of agreement affixes in modern Chukchi . 150 3.3.3 Evaluating theories of Chukchi agreement marking in light of modern speech patterns . 167 3.4 Predicate semantics and semantic role assignment . 171 3.5 Conclusion: evaluating the mechanisms of change in Chukchi transitivity phenomena178 3.5.1 Review of changes to ergative-absolutive morphological phenomena . 180 3.5.2 Loss of ine- and changes to syntactic ergativity . 182 4 Valency-changing operations in Modern Chukchi . 184 4.1 Variation in valency-changing operations in existing Chukchi descriptions . 184 4.1.1 Valency-increasing derivational morphology . 185 4.1.2 Valency-decreasing and valency-rearranging derivational morphology . 187 4.1.3 Noun incorporation . 192 4.2 Verbal derivation and productivity in language shift . 196 4.3 Maintenance of voice morphology . 204 4.3.1 Maintenance of valency-increasing (causative and applicative) morphology 204 4.3.2 Maintenance of valency-rearranging morphology (ine- applicative) . 212 4.3.3 Maintenance of valency-reducing morphology (antipassives) . 216 4.3.4 Overall changes to the productivity of voice morphology . 222 vi 4.4 Patterns of syntactic noun incorporation in Modern Chukchi . 223 4.4.1 Type I noun incorporation . 224 4.4.2 Type II noun incorporation . 227 4.4.3 Type III noun incorporation . 229 4.4.4 Productivity of noun incorporation with different argument types . 235 4.4.5 Assessing variation and change in noun incorporation . 237 4.5 Changes to syntactic ergativity . 239 4.5.1 Cross-clausal co-reference in coordination . 240 4.5.2 Verbal participles . 246 4.6 Evaluating theories of noun incorporation and antipassivization . 251 4.6.1 The relationship between noun incorporation and the antipassive in Chukchi 251 4.6.2 Lexicalist vs. syntactic theories of noun incorporation: answers from Chukchi . 255 4.7 Conclusion: conditioning factors in derivational variation and change . 260 5 Changes to polysynthesis in Modern Chukchi . 265 5.1 Features of polysynthesis . 265 5.2 Changes to the prevalence of argument drop . 267 5.2.1 The alternative: strong maintenance of core case marking . 274 5.3 Derivational productivity . 275 5.3.1 Analytic phenomena in traditional Chukchi . 276 5.3.2 Differences in the use of productive derivational morphology . 277 5.3.3 Modifier incorporation vs. predicative modification . 280 5.3.4 Use of non-finite morphosyntax . 284 5.3.5 Other emergent analytic phenomena . 288 5.4 Degrees of polysynthesis in verbs and nouns . 292 5.5 Implications for theories of polysynthesis . 299 5.6 Explaining changes to polysynthesis . 307 vii 6 The syntax of language endangerment in context . 311 6.1 Revisiting the shift-driven changes to Chukchi argument encoding . 311 6.1.1 The nature of morphosyntactic variation in an endangered language . 316 6.2 Disentangling different sources of change in language endangerment . 319 6.3 Implications for typology and language universals . 324 6.4 Implications for syntactic theory . 327 6.5 Future work . 329 A Experimental design ....................................333 A.1 General production task stimuli and conditions . 333 A.1.1 Notes on abbreviations and other conventions . 336 References ...........................................338 viii List of Figures 1.1 The Chukotko-Kamchatkan language family . 19 1.2 Regional dialects of Chukchi (based on Pupynina 2018: 114) . 24 2.1 Linguistic results of language contact (adapted from Thomason and Kaufman 1988, Table 3) . 50 3.1 Production task stimulus . 153 3.2 Production task stimuli with an inanimate agent and animate undergoer . 173 3.3 Production task stimulus with an intransitive and oblique arguments . 176 4.1 Production task stimulus with valency-rearranging applicative morphology . 213 4.2 Stimulus targeting antipassives in valency-alternation production task . 219 4.3 Cline of loss of derivational productivity in valency-changing processes in Modern Chukchi . 251 ix List of Tables 1.1 Study participants by group . 33 2.1 Yupik Village Names Borrowed into Chukchi (Krupnik and Chlenov 2013) . 56 2.2 Examples of Chukchi adverbial particles borrowed into Central Siberian Yupik (Com- rie 1996, Menovsˇcikovˇ 1967) . 58 2.3 Number of Speakers of Siberian Languages (Russian 2010 Census) . 60 2.4 Case marking in Chukchi (adapted from Dunn 1994 & Dunn 1999) . 86 2.5 Absolutive Personal Pronouns in Chukchi (Dunn 1999) . 86 2.6 Agreement affixes in Chukchi transitive aorist and intransitive intentional, with inverse marking highlighted in bold (adapted from Fortescue 1997) . 89 2.7 Case marking in Russian (Wade 1992: 53) . 93 2.8 Agreement affixes in Chukchi transitive aorist and intransitive intentional, with inverse marking highlighted in bold (adapted from Fortescue 1997) . 104 3.1 Select cases in conservative Chukchi, by noun class . 126 3.2 Summary of declension patterns of nouns tested in production task . 128 3.3 Non-future non-progressive transitive agreement in Traditional Chukchi . 140 3.4 Transitive perfect paradigm in traditional Chukchi . 147 3.5 Transitive habitual paradigm in traditional Chukchi . 147 3.6 Non-future neutral (aorist) active paradigm from an attriting speaker . 156 3.7 Non-future neutral (aorist) active inflection in Traditional Chukchi . 156 3.8 Verbal agreement with subject vs. object in languages of the world (Siewierska 2013) . 159 3.9 Habitual inflection provided by a semi-speaker . ..
Recommended publications
  • The Strategy of Case-Marking
    Case marking strategies Helen de Hoop & Andrej Malchukov1 Radboud University Nijmegen DRAFT January 2006 Abstract Two strategies of case marking in natural languages are discussed. These are defined as two violable constraints whose effects are shown to converge in the case of differential object marking but diverge in the case of differential subject marking. The strength of the case bearing arguments will be shown to be of utmost importance for case marking as well as voice alternations. The strength of arguments can be viewed as a function of their discourse prominence. The analysis of the case marking patterns we find cross-linguistically is couched in a bidirectional OT analysis. 1. Assumptions In this section we wish to put forward our three basic assumptions: (1) In ergative-absolutive systems ergative case is assigned to the first argument x of a two-place relation R(x,y). (2) In nominative-accusative systems accusative case is assigned to the second argument y of a two-place relation R(x,y). (3) Morphologically unmarked case can be the absence of case. The first two assumptions deal with the linking between the first (highest) and second (lowest) argument in a transitive sentence and the type of case marking. For reasons of convenience, we will refer to these arguments quite sloppily as the subject and the object respectively, although we are aware of the fact that the labels subject and object may not be appropriate in all contexts, dependent on how they are actually defined. In many languages, ergative and accusative case are assigned only or mainly in transitive sentences, while in intransitive sentences ergative and accusative case are usually not assigned.
    [Show full text]
  • A Crosslinguistic Approach to Double Nominative and Biabsolutive Constructions
    A Crosslinguistic Approach to Double Nominative and Biabsolutive Constructions: Evidence from Korean and Daghestanian∗ Andrei Antonenko1 and Jisung Sun2 Stony Brook University1,2 1. Introduction Distribution of case among distinct grammatical relations is one of the most frequently studied topics in the syntactic theory. Canonical cases are, in accusative languages, subjects of both intransitive and transitive verbs being nominative, while direct objects of transitive verbs are usually marked accusative. In ergative languages, subjects of intransitive verbs share properties with direct objects of transitive verbs, and are marked absolutive. Subjects of transitive verbs are usually ergative. When you look into world languages, however, there are ‘non-canonical’ case patterns too. Probably the most extreme kind of non-canonical case system would be so-called Quirky Subject constructions in Icelandic (see Sigurðsson 2002). This paper concerns constructions, in which two nominals are identically case-marked in a clause, as observed in Korean and Daghestanian languages. Daghestanian languages belong to Nakh-Daghestanian branch of North Caucasian family. Nakh-Daghestanian languages are informally divided into Nakh languages, such as Chechen and Ingush, spoken in Chechnya and the Republic of Ingushetia, respectively; and Daghestanian languages, spoken in the Republic of Daghestan. Those regions are located in the Caucasian part of Russian Federation. Some Daghestanian languages are also spoken in Azerbaijan and Georgia. This study focuses on Daghestanian languages, such as Archi, Avar, Dargwa, Hinuq, Khwarshi, Lak and Tsez, due to similar behaviors of them with respect to the described phenomenon. 2. Ergativity in Daghestanian Aldridge (2004) proposes that there are two types of syntactically ergative languages, based on which argument is performing functions typical for subjects.
    [Show full text]
  • Dative (First) Complements in Basque
    Dative (first) complements in Basque BEATRIZ FERNÁNDEZ JON ORTIZ DE URBINA Abstract This article examines dative complements of unergative verbs in Basque, i.e., dative arguments of morphologically “transitive” verbs, which, unlike ditransitives, do not co-occur with a canonical object complement. We will claim that such arguments fall under two different types, each of which involves a different type of non-structural licensing of the dative case. The presence of two different types of dative case in these constructions is correlated with the two different types of complement case alternations which many of these predicates exhibit, so that alternation patterns will provide us with clues to identify different sources for the dative marking. In particular, we will examine datives alternating with absolutives (i.e., with the regular object structural case in an ergative language) and datives alternating with postpositional phrases. We will first examine an approach to the former which relies on current proposals that identify a low applicative head as case licenser. Such approach, while accounting for the dative case, raises a number of issues with respect to the absolutive variant. As for datives alternating with postpositional phrases, we claim that they are lexically licensed by the lower verbal head V. Keywords Dative, conflation, lexical case, inherent case, case alternations 1. Preliminaries: bivalent unergatives Bivalent unergatives, i.e., unergatives with a dative complement, have remained largely ignored in traditional Basque studies, perhaps due to the Journal of Portuguese Linguistics, 11-1 (2012), 83-98 ISSN 1645-4537 84 Beatriz Fernández & Jon Ortiz de Urbina identity of their morphological patterns of case marking and agreement with those of ditransitive configurations.
    [Show full text]
  • How Do Young Children Acquire Case Marking?
    INVESTIGATING FINNISH-SPEAKING CHILDREN’S NOUN MORPHOLOGY: HOW DO YOUNG CHILDREN ACQUIRE CASE MARKING? Thesis submitted to the University of Manchester for the degree of Doctor in Philosophy in the Faculty of Medical and Human Sciences 2015 HENNA PAULIINA LEMETYINEN SCHOOL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCES 2 Table of Contents LIST OF TABLES ......................................................................................................................... 6 LIST OF FIGURES ....................................................................................................................... 7 ABSTRACT ................................................................................................................................. 8 DECLARATION .......................................................................................................................... 9 COPYRIGHT STATEMENT .......................................................................................................... 9 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS .......................................................................................................... 10 Chapter 1: General introduction to language acquisition research ...................................... 11 1.1. Generativist approaches to child language............................................................ 11 1.2. Usage-based approaches to child language........................................................... 14 1.3. The acquisition of morphology .............................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Morphological Causatives Are Voice Over Voice
    Morphological causatives are Voice over Voice Yining Nie New York University Abstract Causative morphology has been associated with either the introduction of an event of causation or the introduction of a causer argument. However, morphological causatives are mono-eventive, casting doubt on the notion that causatives fundamentally add a causing event. On the other hand, in some languages the causative morpheme is closer to the verb root than would be expected if the causative head is responsible for introducing the causer. Drawing on evidence primarily from Tagalog and Halkomelem, I argue that the syntactic configuration for morphological causatives involves Voice over Voice, and that languages differ in whether their ‘causative marker’ spells out the higher Voice, the lower Voice or both. Keywords: causative, Voice, argument structure, morpheme order, typology, Tagalog 1. Introduction Syntactic approaches to causatives generally fall into one of two camps. The first view builds on the discovery that causatives may semantically consist of multiple (sub)events (Jackendoff 1972, Dowty 1979, Parsons 1990, Levin & Rappaport Hovav 1994, a.o.). Consider the following English causative–anticausative pair. The anticausative in (1a) consists of an event of change of state, schematised in (1b). The causative in (2a) involves the same change of state plus an additional layer of semantics that conveys how that change of state is brought about (2b). (1) a. The stick broke. b. [ BECOME [ stick STATE(broken) ]] (2) a. Pat broke the stick. b. [ Pat CAUSE [ BECOME [ stick STATE(broken) ]]] Word Structure 13.1 (2020): 102–126 DOI: 10.3366/word.2020.0161 © Edinburgh University Press www.euppublishing.com/word MORPHOLOGICAL CAUSATIVES ARE VOICE OVER VOICE 103 Several linguists have proposed that the semantic CAUSE and BECOME components of the causative are encoded as independent lexical verbal heads in the syntax (Harley 1995, Cuervo 2003, Folli & Harley 2005, Pylkkänen 2008, a.o.).
    [Show full text]
  • Minimalist C/Case Sigurðsson, Halldor Armann
    Minimalist C/case Sigurðsson, Halldor Armann Published in: Linguistic Inquiry 2012 Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Sigurðsson, H. A. (2012). Minimalist C/case. Linguistic Inquiry, 43(2), 191-227. http://ling.auf.net/lingBuzz/000967 Total number of authors: 1 General rights Unless other specific re-use rights are stated the following general rights apply: Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. • Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research. • You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain • You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal Read more about Creative commons licenses: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/ Take down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. LUND UNIVERSITY PO Box 117 221 00 Lund +46 46-222 00 00 Access Provided by Lunds universitet at 04/30/12 3:55PM GMT Minimalist C/case Halldo´rA´ rmann SigurLsson This article discusses A-licensing and case from a minimalist perspec- tive, pursuing the idea that argument NPs cyclically enter a number of A-relations, rather than just a single one, resulting in event licensing, case licensing, and ␾-licensing.
    [Show full text]
  • Ergative As Accusative Case: Evidence from Adiyaman
    ERGATIVE AS ACCUSATIVE CASE: EVIDENCE FROM ADIYAMAN KURMANJI ÜMİT ATLAMAZ ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! BOĞAZİÇİ UNIVERSITY 2012 ERGATIVE AS ACCUSATIVE CASE: EVIDENCE FROM ADIYAMAN KURMANJI Thesis submitted to the Institute for Graduate Studies in the Social Sciences in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Masters of Arts in Linguistics by Ümit Atlamaz ! ! Boğaziçi University 2012 ! ! Thesis Abstract Ümit Atlamaz, “Ergative as Accusative Case: Evidence from Adıyaman Kurmanji” This study aims to investigate the nature of ergativity in Adıyaman Kurmanji within the premises of the Minimalist Program. Adıyaman Kurmanji displays two alignment patterns depending on the tense. In non-past structures nominative alignment is observed whereas the past tense requires an ergative alignment. Based on these two types of alignments many linguists like Haig (2004), Thackston (2006), and Gündoğdu (2011) argue that Kurmanji is a split ergative language. Accordingly, the major aim of this study is to investigate the structure of the ergative pattern in Adıyaman Kurmanji. In this study, the initial step was to compare the ergative and nominative subjects in terms of certain tests like binding, scope and EPP to determine the phrase structure and where the subjects reside on the structure. Additionally, voice properties of the language were inspected as a background to the major claim. Based on the results of the tests applied and the motivation obtained from the data, it was argued that what has been called ergative in Adıyaman Kurmanji is, indeed, a passive structure diachronically reanalyzed as the past tense. According to Trask’s (1979) typology of ergative languages, there are two types of ergative languages, which labels as Type A and Type B.
    [Show full text]
  • Dative Constructions in Romance and Beyond
    Dative constructions in Romance and beyond Edited by Anna Pineda Jaume Mateu language Open Generative Syntax 7 science press Open Generative Syntax Editors: Elena Anagnostopoulou, Mark Baker, Roberta D’Alessandro, David Pesetsky, Susi Wurmbrand In this series: 1. Bailey, Laura R. & Michelle Sheehan (eds.). Order and structure in syntax I: Word order and syntactic structure. 2. Sheehan, Michelle & Laura R. Bailey (eds.). Order and structure in syntax II: Subjecthood and argument structure. 3. Bacskai­Atkari, Julia. Deletion phenomena in comparative constructions: English comparatives in a cross­linguistic perspective. 4. Franco, Ludovico, Mihaela Marchis Moreno & Matthew Reeve (eds.). Agreement, case and locality in the nominal and verbal domains. 5. Bross, Fabian. The clausal syntax of German Sign Language: A cartographic approach. 6. Smith, Peter W., Johannes Mursell & Katharina Hartmann (eds.). Agree to Agree: Agreement in the Minimalist Programme. 7. Pineda, Anna & Jaume Mateu (eds.). Dative constructions in Romance and beyond. ISSN: 2568­7336 Dative constructions in Romance and beyond Edited by Anna Pineda Jaume Mateu language science press Pineda, Anna & Jaume Mateu (eds.). 2020. Dative constructions in Romance and beyond (Open Generative Syntax 7). Berlin: Language Science Press. This title can be downloaded at: http://langsci-press.org/catalog/book/258 © 2020, the authors Published under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 Licence (CC BY 4.0): http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ ISBN: 978-3-96110-249-5 (Digital) 978-3-96110-250-1
    [Show full text]
  • THE SUN, the MOON and FIRMAMENT in CHUKCHI MYTHOLOGY and on the RELATIONS of CELESTIAL BODIES and SACRIFICE Ülo Siimets
    THE SUN, THE MOON AND FIRMAMENT IN CHUKCHI MYTHOLOGY AND ON THE RELATIONS OF CELESTIAL BODIES AND SACRIFICE Ülo Siimets Abstract This article gives a brief overview of the most common Chukchi myths, notions and beliefs related to celestial bodies at the end of the 19th and during the 20th century. The firmament of Chukchi world view is connected with their main source of subsistence – reindeer herding. Chukchis are one of the very few Siberian indigenous people who have preserved their religion. Similarly to many other nations, the peoples of the Far North as well as Chukchis personify the Sun, the Moon and stars. The article also points out the similarities between Chukchi notions and these of other peoples. Till now Chukchi reindeer herders seek the supposed help or influence of a constellation or planet when making important sacrifices (for example, offering sacrifices in a full moon). According to the Chukchi religion the most important celestial character is the Sun. It is spoken of as an individual being (vaúrgún). In addition to the Sun, the Creator, Dawn, Zenith, Midday and the North Star also belong to the ranks of special (superior) beings. The Moon in Chukchi mythology is a man and a being in one person. It is as the ketlja (evil spirit) of the Sun. Chukchi myths about several stars (such as the North Star and Betelgeuse) resemble to a great extent these of other peoples. Keywords: astral mythology, the Moon, sacrifices, reindeer herding, the Sun, celestial bodies, Chukchi religion, constellations. The interdependence of the Earth and celestial as well as weather phenomena has a special meaning for mankind for it is the co-exist- ence of the Sun and Moon, day and night, wind, rainfall and soil that creates life and warmth and provides the daily bread.
    [Show full text]
  • From Latin to Romance: Case Loss and Preservation in Pronominal Systems
    FLORE Repository istituzionale dell'Università degli Studi di Firenze From Latin to Romance: case loss and preservation in pronominal systems Questa è la Versione finale referata (Post print/Accepted manuscript) della seguente pubblicazione: Original Citation: From Latin to Romance: case loss and preservation in pronominal systems / Manzini, MARIA RITA; Savoia, LEONARDO MARIA. - In: PROBUS. - ISSN 1613-4079. - STAMPA. - 26, 2(2014), pp. 217-248. Availability: This version is available at: 2158/891750 since: 2016-01-20T16:23:29Z Terms of use: Open Access La pubblicazione è resa disponibile sotto le norme e i termini della licenza di deposito, secondo quanto stabilito dalla Policy per l'accesso aperto dell'Università degli Studi di Firenze (https://www.sba.unifi.it/upload/policy-oa-2016-1.pdf) Publisher copyright claim: (Article begins on next page) 27 September 2021 Probus 2014; 26(2): 217 – 248 M. Rita Manzini* and Leonardo M. Savoia From Latin to Romance: case loss and preservation in pronominal systems Abstract: The evolution from Latin into Romance is marked by the loss of case in nominal declensions. In most Romance varieties, however, pronouns, specifi- cally in the 1st/2nd person singular, keep case differentiations. In some varieties 1st/2nd singular pronouns present a three-way case split, essentially the same re- constructed for proto-Romance (De Dardel and Gaeng 1992, Zamboni 1998). We document and analyze the current situation of Romance in the first part of the article (section 1). In the second part of the article we argue that the Dative Shifted distribution of loro in modern Italian, accounted for by means of the category of weak pronoun in Cardinaletti and Starke (1999), is best construed as a survival of oblique case in the 3rd person system (section 2).
    [Show full text]
  • Typological Studies with Passive/Antipassive As an Example
    Typological studies with passive/antipassive as an example Irina Burukina [email protected] *** . Irina Burukina ([email protected])Typological studies with passive/antipassive as an example *** 1 / 53 This lecture is about 1. Typology What is linguistic typology 2. Examples of linguistic typologies Word order typology Morphological typology Morphosyntactic typology: Nominative vs. ergative languages 3. Voices from a typological perspective Voice Passive Antipassive . Irina Burukina ([email protected])Typological studies with passive/antipassive as an example *** 2 / 53 What is linguistic typology . Irina Burukina ([email protected])Typological studies with passive/antipassive as an example *** 3 / 53 References The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Typology. 2010. edited by Jae Jung Song Pereltsvaig, Asya. 2012. Languages of the World. An Introduction. Croft, William. 1990, 2003. Typology and Universals. Nichols, Johanna. 1992. Linguistic Diversity in Space and Time. World Atlas of Language Structures (WALS) – https://wals.info/ . Irina Burukina ([email protected])Typological studies with passive/antipassive as an example *** 4 / 53 Linguistic typology Comparative study of human languages: Comparing languages with each other with respect to a given linguistic phenomenon. Classifying observed crosslinguistic variation into types. Formulating generalizations over the distribution of linguistic patterns across the languages of the world and their relationship to other patterns. – universals and parameters . Irina Burukina ([email protected])Typological studies with passive/antipassive as an example *** 5 / 53 Greenberg’s universals Greenberg (1963): What is possible/impossible in human language? Why? Examples of Greenberg’s universals: All languages with dominant VSO order have SVO as an alternative or as the only alternative basic order.
    [Show full text]
  • German Free Datives and Knight Move Binding Daniel Hole/Universität Stuttgart
    German free datives and Knight Move Binding Daniel Hole/Universität Stuttgart 1. Introduction This paper is concerned with German free datives and their peculiar binding behavior. * I ar- gue that free datives are best described in terms of voice. The free dative voice turns out to be very similar to run-of-the-mill cases of reflexivity, which must likewise be modeled as a kind of voice under the theoretical assumptions of Kratzer’s (1996) agent severance. The free da- tive, just like a reflexive antecedent in German, binds a variable in the local tense domain. What is highly peculiar about the free dative voice is the tree-geometrical requirement that goes along with it. The variable that free datives bind must be at the left edge of a clause-mate coargumental possessum phrase or purpose phrase (‘Knight Move Binding’). Standard im- plementations of binding don’t include requirements of this kind. The argumentation strives to show that the requirement of Knight Move Binding really exists, and that this kind of bind- ing is a privileged configuration in the grammaticalization of reflexive pronouns crosslinguis- tically. The paper delimits the empirical domain of free datives in sects. 2 and 3. Sect. 4 estab- lishes the parallel locality restrictions of dative binding for “possessor” and “beneficiary” da- tives. Sect. 5 establishes the Knight Move Binding requirement of free datives. Sect. 6 devel- ops the semantic implementation of free dative binding with a large detour via semantic theo- ries of reflexivization. Competing proposals are briefly discussed in sect. 7. Sect. 8 concludes the paper.
    [Show full text]