Affiliation, Attraction and Close Relationships

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Affiliation, Attraction and Close Relationships 9781405124003_4_010.qxd 10/31/07 3:07 PM Page 196 Affiliation, Attraction and Close 10 Relationships Abraham P. Buunk and Pieternel Dijkstra KEY CONCEPTS affiliation attachment theory attraction buffer effect of social support commitment emotional contagion epidemiological studies equity theory evolutionary theory intimacy investment model loneliness passionate love propinquity reciprocity social comparison theory social exchange theory social support 9781405124003_4_010.qxd 10/31/07 3:07 PM Page 197 CHAPTER OUTLINE Humans are a very social breed. They seek each other’s company in a variety of situations, they make friendships with other people, and they seem to find their ultimate happiness and despair in their intimate relationships. But what is it that drives us to interact socially with others? What determines the fact that we often quite rapidly find ourselves liking some people more than others? Such issues are dealt with in the present chapter. We begin with a discussion of affiliation, followed by a section on attraction and friendships, focusing on the factors that make individuals like other people and become friends with them. Next, we deal with the nature of romantic attraction and, finally, we deal with the development and dissolution of close relationships. Introduction Carl is in general a happy man. He enjoys having fun and spending time with his friends. Recently, he fell in love with Carin, a beautiful woman whom he had known for some time; however, Carin does not reciprocate his feelings. Since then, Carl has felt quite unhappy and at times lonely. Although he needs company because of his unhappiness, even being with his friends hardly improves his mood. Carin likes Carl, but just does not have romantic feelings for him. She feels that Carl lacks ambition, and is not the type of man she is looking for. Her closest girlfriend, with whom she discusses everything, agrees that Carl is not right for her. Why does Carl feel so lonely? Why does being with his friends not improve his mood? And why does Carin find ambition such an important attribute in a mate? The social psychology of personal relationships helps us to answer all these questions, and explains why our close relationships with others can be such sources of personal happiness and fulfilment, but also of great sadness, even despair. This chapter will provide you with the tools to answer these questions, beginning with a consideration of why people need others in the first place. 9781405124003_4_010.qxd 10/31/07 3:07 PM Page 198 198 CHAPTER 10 AFFILIATION, ATTRACTION AND CLOSE RELATIONSHIPS same experiment, rather than someone who was in a quite differ- AFFILIATION: THE NEED ent situation, such as waiting for a professor. As Schachter con- cluded: ‘Misery doesn’t love just any kind of company, it loves only FOR SOCIAL CONTACT miserable company’ (p. 24). Knowledge about social comparison and affiliation has proven very useful in the area of psychology and health. Gump and Kulik When do we like to be in the company of others? (1997) found that participants who were faced with the prospect Does social support for other people help to reduce stress? of undergoing experimentally induced pain spent more time What are the health consequences of a lack of affiliation? looking at how other individuals responded who were to undergo the same threat than at individuals who were to participate in a very different experiment – their responses would not be relevant Situations fostering affiliation: to compare with one’s own responses. Social comparison with others may also provide individuals with valuable information, When do people affiliate? for example about how to deal with their own situation. As a result of this motive, individuals faced with a threat may prefer Humans have a general need to affiliate with others, and they contact with someone knowledgeable, who may provide infor- spend a considerable part of their life in the company of other mation about the potential threat. In their study Stanton, Danoff- people. By affiliation we mean Burg, Cameron, Snider and Kirk (1999) assigned breast cancer affiliation the tendency to seek out the the tendency to seek out the patients randomly to listen to an audiotaped interview in which company of others, irrespective of the company of others, even if we another breast cancer patient’s psychological adjustment and feelings towards such others do not feel particularly close disease prognosis were manipulated to reflect good, poor or un- to them. According to the specified psychological and physical health status. Participants homeostatic model (O’Connor & Rosenblood, 1996), affiliation demonstrated a greater desire for information and emotional can be seen as a drive, in that people look for an optimal range of support from the patient with good rather than poor health social contact. This model states that, when individuals experi- status, but not more than from the patient with unspecified ence too much solitude, they seek out social contact until their health status. Thus they did not profit from the opportunity affiliative drive is satiated; and when they experience excess social to learn more about the patient with poor health status, which contact, they seek out solitude to restore the optimum level of might in fact have been useful in putting their own situation affiliation. It is generally assumed that the human desire for affili- into perspective and, indeed, making them feel comparatively ation stems in large part from our evolutionary past, when joining better off. others in the face of threat, such as predators and aggressors, enhanced our chances of survival. It has indeed been found that Anxiety reduction A host of evidence indicates that indi- individuals are particularly likely to affiliate under conditions viduals in threatening and stressful circumstances do often turn of stress, in which survival issues may become salient (e.g., to sympathetic others who may offer them reassurance, comfort Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Buunk & Schaufeli, 1993). Why would individuals affiliate when confronted with a stressful situation? Two theoretical answers to this question are particularly relevant, one stemming from social comparison theory and the other from attachment theory. Social comparison According to social comparison theory, par- ticularly in novel and stressful situations, individuals tend to seek out others to compare them- social comparison theory assumes that selves with, to learn more individuals seek out others to compare about their own feelings and themselves with, to assess the to obtain information about appropriateness of their feelings and to the most effective way of be- obtain information about the most effective having. They may not know way of behaving how to feel and respond: ‘Am I too worried?’, ‘Should I be really concerned’, ‘Am I the only one who is so upset?’ Affiliation with others facing the same situation gives individuals the oppor- tunity to compare their responses with those of others, and thus to assess the appropriateness of their feelings (Festinger, 1954). In line with this theory, Schachter (1959) found that research participants Plate 10.1 Social comparison with others may provide individuals under threat of receiving an electric shock preferred to be in the with valuable information, for example about how to deal with company of someone else who was also waiting to take part in the their own illness. 9781405124003_4_010.qxd 10/31/07 3:07 PM Page 199 AFFILIATION: THE NEED FOR SOCIAL CONTACT 199 present is nervous rather than emotional contagion the unconscious PIONEER calm. In such cases emotional mimicking of the facial expressions and contagion may occur: indi- feelings of another person viduals unconsciously mimic John Bowlby (1907–1990) started his intellectual career others’ facial expressions and at the University of Cambridge where he studied develop- feelings (e.g., Gump & Kulik, 1997, Study 2). mental psychology and, later, child psychiatry and psycho- analysis. Upon returning from army service in 1945, Bowlby Social support and stress reduction Whereas seeking out became head of the Children’s Department at the Tavistock the company of others is an active strategy for reducing anxiety, Clinic in London. Studying maladapted children, Bowlby felt that psychoanalysis was putting far too much emphasis on having a supportive network of friends or relatives is something the child’s fantasy world and far too little on actual events. that is reliably associated with As a result, Bowlby developed attachment theory, which stress reduction. Social sup- social support the feeling of being states that a child’s actual experiences within the family have port refers to the feeling of supported by others, usually divided into far-reaching effects on his or her personality development. being supported by others, four components: emotional support, and is usually divided into appraisal support, informational support According to Bowlby, starting during the first and instrumental support months in their relationships with both par- four components (House, ents, children build up so-called ‘working 1981), i.e., emotional support models’ of how attachment figures are likely (feeling cared for, loved and appreciated); appraisal support to behave, and for the rest of their lives (feedback and social comparison on how to evaluate things); children’s expectations are based on these informational support (such as information about how to handle models. situations); and instrumental support (receiving concrete aid and help). The first three of these components correspond to the two functions of affiliation under stress that were mentioned above. In addition, social support often takes place ‘invisibly’, without indi- viduals noticing that others are supporting them. A person may, for instance, not explicitly notice that his or her partner had done and emotional support chores around the house, but nevertheless feel happy as a result attachment theory proposes that the development of secure infant–caregiver (e.g., Stroebe & Stroebe, that things at home seem to be going so well (Bolger, Zuckerman attachment in childhood is the basis for the 1996; Wills, 1991).
Recommended publications
  • 22. the Immortal Bhaktas
    22. The Immortal Bhaktas AMONG all forms of Sadhana, Bhakti (devotion to the Lord) is the easiest and holiest. Bhakti is derived from the root "Bhaj", with the suffix "thi." It means Seva (Service). It denotes a feeling of friendship coupled with awe. For one who is a creature of the gunas (Satwa, Rajas, Tamas), to understand what transcends the gunas, an attitude of humility and reverence is required. "Bhaja Sevaayaam" (worship the Divine through Seva). Bhakti calls for utilising the mind, speech and body to worship the Lord. It represents total love. Devotion and love are inseparable and interdependent. Bhakti is the means to salvation. Love is the expression of Bhakti. Narada declared that worshipping the Lord with boundless love is Bhakti. Vyasa held that performing worship with love and adoration is Bhakti. Garga Rishi declared that serving the Lord with purity of mind, speech and body is Bhakti. Yajnavalkya held that true Bhakti consists in controlling the mind, turning it inwards and enjoying the bliss of communion with the Divine. Another view of Bhakti is concentration of the mind on God and experiencing oneness with the Divine. Win love through love Although many sages have expressed different views about the nature of Bhakti, the basic characteristic of devotion is Love. Love is present in every human being in however small a measure. The riva (individual) is an aspect of the Divine, who is the supreme embodiment of Love. Man also is an embodiment of Love, but because his love is directed towards worldly objects, it gets tainted and he is unable to get a vision of God in all His beauty.
    [Show full text]
  • Compassionate Love for Close Others and Humanity
    03_sprecher_056439 (jk-t) 29/9/05 1:42 pm Page 629 Compassionate love for close others and humanity Susan Sprecher Illinois State University Beverley Fehr University of Winnipeg ABSTRACT A compassionate love scale was developed that can be used, in alternative forms, to assess compassionate or altruistic love for different targets (e.g., close others and all of humankind). Using three samples (total N = 529), the Compas- sionate Love scale was developed and piloted. Three studies (total N = 700) were then conducted to provide validation of the scale and to examine correlates of compassionate love. In support of our predictions, compassionate love was found to be associated positively with prosocial behavior, as directed both to close others and to all of humanity. Those who were more religious or spiritual experienced more compassionate love than those who were less religious or spiritual. Evidence was found that compassionate love is distinct from empathy. In the final study, we introduced a relationship-specific version of the Compassionate Love scale, and found that compassionate love for a specific close other was associated with the provision of social support for that person. KEY WORDS: altruism • compassionate love • love • social support • spirituality Considerable research has been conducted on love in the past two decades. This research has focused primarily on defining and operational- izing love and the examination of its predictors, correlates, and outcomes within romantic relationships (for a review, see S. S. Hendrick & Hendrick, 2000). Love for close others such as family and friends, for peripheral ties, and for all of humanity has generally not been a topic of investigation.
    [Show full text]
  • Interpersonal Attraction
    Dr K Williams 11/19/07 Interpersonal Attraction Psych 240; Fall 2007 Purdue University Prof. Kipling Williams Initial Attraction: What matters at first? Propinquity (we like those who live/work near us; mere exposure) Physical Attractiveness (we like those who are physically attractive; halo, rewards, kernel of truth; evolutionary signs of health and reproductive advantage) Similarity/Complementarity (we like those who are similar to us—it’s rewarding; complementarity of needs) Responsiveness (we like those who are responsive to us; signals belonging, worth, and control) Reciprocal liking (we like those who like us—it’s rewarding) Interpersonal Attraction 1 Dr K Williams 11/19/07 Propinquity Festinger, Schachter & Back (1959)’s “Social pressures in informal groups: A study of human factors in housing” Zajonc’s (1968) “Attitudinal effects of mere exposure” (JPSP) “Mirror exposure” - we like our reflection view better than the view that others see of us; and vice versa. Moreland & Beach’s (1992) “Exposure effects in the classroom…” (JESP) Physical Attractiveness Walster, Aronson, Abrahams & Rottman, 1966: Computer Dating paradigm Take a battery of measures of personality SES interests physical attractiveness Randomly pair college students with person of opposite sex. Asked them to rate their date Only one factor predicted liking and intention to ask out again…physical attractiveness (for males & females!) How about after the fifth date? (Mathes, 1975) Interpersonal Attraction 2 Dr K Williams 11/19/07 Physical Attractiveness How ubiquitous? In the courtroom less likely to be found guilty; if guilty, lighter sentence (except if used to commit crime) In job applications More likely to be hired even for jobs in which appearance could have no conceivable relationship to job performance Class project (High, Med, Low Phys Attractiveness X High, Med, Low Qualifications) • Which matters most? Physical Attractiveness How ubiquitous? With children They are more popular, better liked by parents, teachers, and peers.
    [Show full text]
  • Student Social and Emotional Development and Accountability: Perspective of Teachers
    Student Social and Emotional Development and Accountability: Perspective of Teachers National Network of State Teachers of the Year Elizabeth J. Glennie, Ph.D. Jeffrey A. Rosen, Ph.D. Rebecca Snyder, Ed.D. Maryann Woods-Murphy, Ed.D. Katherine Bassett, M.Ed. April 2017 We at the National Network of State Teachers of the Year (NNSTOY are most pleased to share with you the latest in our series of Research Reports. In this report, we focus on measures of social and emotional learning and their efficacy in educator evaluation. Using multiple approaches – literature review and three focus group panels – we examined what the field says about these measures and well as what expert teachers – State and National Teachers of the Year and Finalists for State Teacher of the Year think. I worked for twelve years at Educational Testing Service in the area of performance based assessment across a continuum of educator practice. One of the first maxims that I learned was: ‘…not everything that is important can be measured and not everything that can be measured is important.’ A second was ‘…we must not use assessment measures for pur- poses for which they have not been developed or validated.’ I also had the privilege of working with a team of expert research scientists, including Dr. Richard Roberts, to examine the efficacy of assessing social and emotional characteristics and their mallea- bility in educators. So, this study is of particular interest to me. Can we actually measure social and emotional characteristics, and, are they malleable? In other words, can we teach things like empathy, caring, persistence? Should we include measures of these characteristics in teachers as part of consequential evaluation? As expert educators, we deeply desire accountability measures.
    [Show full text]
  • Examples of Social Emotional Development in Preschool
    Examples Of Social Emotional Development In Preschool Serge skives perplexedly while reddish Yves disconnects abnormally or tees ambitiously. Wainwright forfeit his zibet perorated thoughtlessly or closest after Nichole depredate and authorises slightly, secund and hack. Is Alec sham or turbellarian when dapped some pitch-and-toss rupture audaciously? Different emotions and other adults to fit the groundwork for school day of the development in neuroscientific research links Examples of ways to support goals for am are where by. What are developing child social emotional skills of. By emotional development of social situation and develop socially and to integrate their skills developed in the examples of obtaining parent characteristics of other. Early childhood development of emotional skills social awareness. Early childhood social and emotional development Advancing the mistake of. Kids who are born prematurely have deep childhood trauma are most the autism. Foundation for healthy social and emotional development This process. They are willing to flex a reasonable risk, express and defend ideas, try new experiences, or engage in challenging tasks. Devereux foundation of emotion in your child develop an example. He got twisted and of? Early Childhood Inclusion; Focus on Change. Social and Emotional Development of Preschoolers. Early Childhood Services Ltd. Your preschooler has shown an example, while others when they continue to. You are using a browser that include not with Flash player enabled or installed. Set of social and in this example that out closeness with preschool curriculum. Causal talk about social emotional reactivity and develop socially appropriate ways worked closely tied to recognize individual experiences can also the examples of the next week were developed.
    [Show full text]
  • Lovesickness” in Late Chos Ǒn Literature
    UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA Los Angeles Reinterpreting “Lovesickness” in Late Chos ǒn Literature A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree Doctor of Philosophy in Asian Languages and Cultures by Janet Yoon-sun Lee 2014 © Copyright by Janet Yoon-sun Lee 2014 ABSTRACT OF THE DISSERTATION Reinterpreting “Lovesickness” in Late Chos ǒn Literature By Janet Yoon-sun Lee Doctor of Philosophy in Asian Languages and Cultures University of California, Los Angeles, 2014 Professor Peter H. Lee, Chair My dissertation concerns the development of the literary motif of “lovesickness” (sangsa py ǒng ) in late Chos ǒn narratives. More specifically, it examines the correlation between the expression of feelings and the corporeal symptoms of lovesickness as represented in Chos ǒn romance narratives and medical texts, respectively, of the seventeenth and eighteenth centuries. As the convergence of literary and medical discourse, lovesickness serves as a site to define both the psychological and physical experiences of love, implying the correlation between mind and body in the non-Western tradition. The analysis itself is re-categorized into the discussions of the feeling and the body. In the discussion of the feeling, it will be argued that the feeling of longing not only occupies an important position in literature, but also is gendered and structured in lyrics and narratives of the seventeenth century. In addressing the rubric of feelings of “longing,” this part seeks the ii theoretical grounds of how the intense experience of longing is converted to language of love and to bodily symptoms to constitute the knowledge of lovesickness. The second part concerns the representation of lovesick characters in Korean romance, particularly concerning the body politics of the Chos ŏn society.
    [Show full text]
  • About Emotions There Are 8 Primary Emotions. You Are Born with These
    About Emotions There are 8 primary emotions. You are born with these emotions wired into your brain. That wiring causes your body to react in certain ways and for you to have certain urges when the emotion arises. Here is a list of primary emotions: Eight Primary Emotions Anger: fury, outrage, wrath, irritability, hostility, resentment and violence. Sadness: grief, sorrow, gloom, melancholy, despair, loneliness, and depression. Fear: anxiety, apprehension, nervousness, dread, fright, and panic. Joy: enjoyment, happiness, relief, bliss, delight, pride, thrill, and ecstasy. Interest: acceptance, friendliness, trust, kindness, affection, love, and devotion. Surprise: shock, astonishment, amazement, astound, and wonder. Disgust: contempt, disdain, scorn, aversion, distaste, and revulsion. Shame: guilt, embarrassment, chagrin, remorse, regret, and contrition. All other emotions are made up by combining these basic 8 emotions. Sometimes we have secondary emotions, an emotional reaction to an emotion. We learn these. Some examples of these are: o Feeling shame when you get angry. o Feeling angry when you have a shame response (e.g., hurt feelings). o Feeling fear when you get angry (maybe you’ve been punished for anger). There are many more. These are NOT wired into our bodies and brains, but are learned from our families, our culture, and others. When you have a secondary emotion, the key is to figure out what the primary emotion, the feeling at the root of your reaction is, so that you can take an action that is most helpful. .
    [Show full text]
  • Aristotle on Love and Friendship
    ARISTOTLE ON LOVE AND FRIENDSHIP DAVID KONSTAN Philia is exceptional among ancient Greek value terms for the number of still unre- solved, or at least intensely debated, questions that go to the heart of its very nature.1 Does it mean “friendship”, as it is most commonly rendered in discussions of Aris- totle, or rather “love”, as seems more appropriate in some contexts? Whether it is love, friendship, or something else, is it an emotion, a virtue, or a disposition? The same penumbra of ambiguity surrounds the related term philos, often rendered as “friend” but held by some to include kin and other relations, and even to refer chiefly to them. Thus, Elizabeth Belfiore affirms that “the noun philos surely has the same range as philia, and both refer primarily, if not exclusively, to relationships among close blood kin” (2000: 20). In respect to the affective character of philia, Michael Peachin (2001: 135 n. 2) describes “the standard modern view of Roman friendship” as one “that tends to reduce significantly the emotional aspect of the relationship among the Ro- mans, and to make of it a rather pragmatic business”, and he holds the same to be true of Greek friendship or philia. Scholars at the other extreme maintain that ancient friendship was based essentially on affection. As Peachin remarks (ibid., p. 7), “D. Konstan [1997] has recently argued against the majority opinion and has tried to inject more (modern-style?) emotion into ancient amicitia”. Some critics, in turn, have sought a compromise between the two positions, according to which ancient friend- ship involved both an affective component and the expectation of practical services.
    [Show full text]
  • 5. Emotions: Taming the Destructive and Cultivating Balance
    5. EMOTIONS: TAMING THE DESTRUCTIVE AND CULTIVATING BALANCE Chapter Overview As the title states, this chapter is about emotions, how to reduce destructive emotions and cultivate helpful emotions. To begin with we will look at what we mean by emotions and explore what we know of emotions from a contemporary scientific perspective and a traditional Buddhist perspective. The chapter will outline how mindfulness can be used to balance our emotions and short circuit dysfunctional emotions. We will explore anger, fear, sadness and happiness in detail and how mindfulness can be used to understand and balance these emotions. The chapter will conclude by offering some strategies for working with painful emotions. What are emotions? The word emotion originates from the Latin "emovere", which means to disturb. The first part of the word, "e" means “out”, and “movere” translates as “to move”. When we need to function effectively in a demanding world, emotions move us to action. There is no direct Pali equivalent for the term “emotion”. Rather emotions are understood as a combination of physical sensations, feelings (vedana), states of heart-mind (citta) and mental and physical patterns. Emotions involve complex body-mind interactions. Emotions are necessary for our personal development, survival and thriving as a human being. They save lives, motivate behaviour and help us form relationships. Balanced emotions are essential for effective communication and the welfare and wellbeing of our families and communities. Unfortunately emotions can be unbalanced, dysfunctional, painful and destructive. At times, emotions can move us to act unskilfully with dire consequences. Emotions can arise and pass relatively quickly.
    [Show full text]
  • THE SEXUAL MIND Exploring the Origins of Arousal Publisher: VäestLiitto Ry., Population Research Institute (VäestNtutkimuslaitos)
    •vaestoliitto OSMO KONTULA THE SEXUAL MIND Exploring the Origins of Arousal Publisher: Väestliitto ry., Population Research Institute (Väestntutkimuslaitos) Kalevankatu 16 B, P.O. Box 849, 00100 Helsinki. www.vaestoliitto.fi Original in Finnish: Mielen seksuaalisuus – Matka kiihottumisen alkulähteille (Duodecim, 2017) © Author and Väestöliitto Cover image: Sirkka-Liisa Lonka ISBN: 978-952-226-214-1 (English online - Väestliitto) ISBN: 978-951-656-680-4 (Finnish print - Duodecim) ISBN: 978-951-656-681-1 (Finnish online - Duodecim) Helsinki 2021 TABLE OF CONTENTS For the Reader...........................................................................................................................................................4 The mind as the source and barrier to sexuality.................................................................6 The sexual mind......................................................................................................... 6 Journey of sexual self-exploration ........................................................................ 8 Becoming sexual ......................................................................................................12 Why is sex seen as dangerous or condemnable?............................................15 Activation or suppression of the sexual mind .................................................18 Recollecting past sexual experiences .................................................................22 The erotic and romantic mind .............................................................................................................24
    [Show full text]
  • Live, Laugh, Limerence
    Live, Laugh, Limerence An Opera Buffa in four acts Libretto by Marijke De Roover 2019 De Roover’s introductory notes: The performance needs the ​ naturalism of the text. All characters exist all at once all as one. The landscape in which the experience is set could be a metropolitan studio apartment, an AA meeting, a deep dream or a dead star (more chance of it being a karaoke booth tbh). The I in the text is collective, the time suggestive. The simultaneity of the four parts of the text can be portrayed any which way. Me to me: You have a responsibility towards your audience. ​ Please keep all the ducks in a row! The ducks: 1 CAST OF CHARACTERS When reading this story out loud, please use the following voices: ELETTRA: fast-paced, sweet, melodic. LOTTE (V.O.): distant but sensitive, straight forward. TURANDOT (V.O.): wild but judgemental (but their friendship is like this). → darlin’, this kind of love is not viable (I can just see it with this fat american country accent lol) Or (Marilyn is so sweet and Jane is much more rough/tough) NARRATOR (V.O.): voice of god (distant), ironic, calm ​. 2 SCENES PROLOGUE: IN WHICH THE NARRATOR EXPRESSES HER DOUBTS ACT ONE: THE DAY I STOPPED DRINKING I BECAME A PLAYWRIGHT THAT JUST SITS IN COFFEE BARS Which introduces us to the tragedy and its Scene 1:​ ​ protagonist A brief encounter Scene 2:​ ​ I took a deep breath and listened to the old bray Scene 3:​ ​ of my heart. I am. I am. I am A realistic portrayal of someone using love as an Scene 4:​ ​ escapist drug ACT TWO: A KISS.
    [Show full text]
  • Zest and Work
    Journal of Organizational Behavior J. Organiz. Behav. 30, 161–172 (2009) Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/job.584 Zest and work CHRISTOPHER PETERSON1*, NANSOOK PARK2, NICHOLAS HALL3 AND MARTIN E.P.SELIGMAN 3 1University of Michigan, Michigan, U.S.A. 2University of Rhode Island, Rhode Island, U.S.A. 3University of Pennsylvania, Pennsylvania, U.S.A. Summary Zest is a positive trait reflecting a person’s approach to life with anticipation, energy, and excitement. In the present study, 9803 currently employed adult respondents to an Internet site completed measures of dispositional zest, orientation to work as a calling, and satisfaction with work and life in general. Across all occupations, zest predicted the stance that work was a calling (r ¼.39), as well as work satisfaction (r ¼.46) and general life satisfaction (r ¼.53). Zest deserves further attention from organizational scholars, especially how it can be encouraged in the workplace. Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Your work is to discover your work, and then with all of your heart to give yourself to it.—the Buddha Introduction Recent years have seen a widespread call for the study of work organizations in which people can be well and do well (Cameron, Dutton, & Quinn, 2003; Gardner, Csikszentmihalyi, & Damon, 2001; Luthans, 2003; Wright, 2003). The emergence of the positive perspective within organizational psychology has brought new attention to the venerable topic of work satisfaction (Hoppock, 1935). Satisfaction with the work that one does is seen not just as a contributor to good performance and increased profitability but as a worthy end in its own right (Heslin, 2005).
    [Show full text]