An Overview of the Vocal Repertoire of Indri Indri

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Load more

JASs Reports

Journal of Anthropological Sciences
Vol. 88 (2010), pp. 151-165

Not just a pretty song: an overview of the vocal repertoire of Indri indri

Giovanna Maretti1, Viviana Sorrentino1, Andriamasitoly Finomana2, Marco Gamba1 & Cristina Giacoma1

1 ) D epartment of Animal and Human Biology, University of T o rino, Via Accademia Albertina 13, 10123 T o rino, Italy

e-mail: [email protected]

2) Faculté des Sciences, University of Mahajanga, Campus Ambondrona, BP 652, Mahajanga (401), Madagascar

Summary - e vocal behaviour of wild indris inhabiting the area near Andasibe was studied by means of all occurrence sampling. We provide a quantitative overview of the vocal repertoire of Indri indri, describing qualitative contextual information and quantitative acoustic analysis for all the utterances we recorded from adult individuals. Other than the song, the repertoire of Indri indri comprises 8 vocal types uttered by the adults. Future studies are necessary to explore whether vocalisations uttered in different contexts have different functions and how these functions relate to acoustic structure.

Keywords - Strepsirhine primates, V o cal behaviour, Acoustic structure, Ambient noise.

Introduction

The indri (Indri indri) is a particularly interesting species for the study of vocal communica-
To investigate the relationship between vocal tion for at least three reasons. It lives in small and non-vocal behaviour, it is crucial to analyse family groups in lowland to mid-altitude primary vocal signals making up the vocal repertoire of a and secondary rainforest of eastern Madagascar species, to describe vocal types and, where possi- (Garbutt, 1999; Pollock, 1979), spending most ble, provide information regarding the variation of its time in the dense canopy. Each group has of acoustic parameters between and within vocal a relatively stable home range and shows exclutypes. The study of vocal repertoires can provide sive territoriality. In order to regulate spacing information about the factors that affect signal- between neighbouring groups in the forest, all ling and how vocal signals influence the behav- group members emit a complex howling call, iour of receivers, thereby indicating the function which can be broadcast over several kilometres.

  • of the signals. Information concerning the acous-
  • In the present study, we aimed to provide a

tic structure and the context, or contexts, of comprehensive overview of the vocal repertoire vocal production can lay the foundation for fur- of the indri (Indri indri) with special attention ther studies on a species, and provide researchers to those utterances that do not occur during the with the basic knowledge necessary for design- indri song. Previous studies of the indri’s vocal ing detailed studies on the relationship between communication focussed their attention on the vocal communication and social behaviour, or to song (Pollock, 1975, 1986; Oliver & O’Connor, perform cross-specific comparisons (Owings & 1980; Haimoff, 1986; Thalmann et al., 1993;

  • Morton,1998; Owren & Rendall, 2001).
  • Geissmann & Mutschler, 2006; Sorrentino et al.,

the JASs is published by the Istituto Italiano di Antropologia

www.isita-org.com

Vocal repertoire of Indri indri

152

in press).The indri song is a vocal display in which capital Antananarivo and the east coast, ca. 30 one or more individuals emit a sequence of notes km east of Moramanga. Previously covered with separated by short pauses (Pollock, 1986). Songs continuous forest, the region now has only fraglast 40-250 s and are usually introduced by three ments of forest (Dolch, 2003). We collected data or four “roars” uttered simultaneously by several in three different sites: the 810 ha Analamazaotra members of a group (Pollock, 1986); then, one at Reserve (18° 56’ S, 48° 25’ E); Mitsinjo Forest a time, or sometimes in pairs, the members of the Station (18° 56’ S, 48° 24’ E), a 250 ha area group produce a long “modulated howl” (Petter 200 m from the border of the Analamazaotra & Charles-Dominique, 1979), or “song proper” Reserve; and Mantadia National Park, 10,000 ha (Pollock, 1986). Previous investigators agreed situated 20 km from the other two sites (Powzyk that the main functions of indri songs are related & Mowry, 2003). The climate is humid with to territorial announcement and defence (Petter a mean annual rainfall of 1700 mm, a mean et al., 1977; Petter & Charles-Dominique, 1979; annual temperature of 18°C, and an atmospheric Pollock, 1986; Geissmann & Mutschler, 2006). humidity >70%; elevation ranges between 850- We aimed at improving knowledge of the indri 1220 metres and mean canopy cover is 87.6 repertoire, providing a robust classification of the 4.3% (Stephenson, 1994; Powzyk & Mowry, utterances based on quantitative acoustic descrip- 2003; Biebouw, 2009). tions as well as contextual information for each of the vocal types. Our hypothesis is that, as in Data collection

  • other non-human primates, certain vocal types
  • We recorded the vocalisations of ten

occur in several behavioural contexts, while oth- groups of indris. Six groups were recorded in ers are strictly associated with particular contexts, Analamazaotra Special Reserve, one group in

  • or referential.
  • Mantadia National Park, and three groups

In the indri, as in most primates, phona- in Mitsinjo Forest Station. Of these groups tion is usually the result of an expiratory airflow, (Tab. 1), three were usually exposed to tourwhich causes oscillation of the vocal folds to pro- ist groups (contributing for 38% of the study duce sounds. The acoustic correlate of the rate of sample), and three more were seldom visited vocal fold vibration is the fundamental frequency by tourists (contributing for 23%). The other (F0) of the vocal signal. By changing the stiffness four groups were habituated by V. S. before the and length of the vocal folds, mammals (includ- beginning of the recording sessions (contributing humans) can modify the periodicity of vocal ing for 39%). The total number of individuals fold vibration and vary the F0 characteristics of a recorded was 28: 14 males, 13 females, and one

  • vocal sound over time.
  • infant of unknown sex (Tab. 1). We observed

Our second goal was to quantify the vocal and recorded only one group per day, for a total flexibility of this species, analysing F0 variation of 160 days. We usually followed the same group quantitatively across the vocal types. We expected for a minimum of four consecutive days, during that, as for many non-human primates, F0 vari- September-December of four consecutive years ation would be marked between different vocal (2005-2008). We carried out survey walks daily

  • types and limited within vocal type.
  • from 06.00 h to 13.00 h.

For the purpose of this research we obtained research permits from Direction des Eaux et Forêts in 2005 (N° 197 /MINENV.EF/SG/ DGEF/DPB/SCBLF/RECH), 2006 (N° 172/06 /MINENV.EF/SG/ DGEF/DPB/SCBLF), 2007

Materials and Methods

Study areas

We conducted the study in a mid-altitude (N° 0220/07 /MINENV.EF/SG/ DGEF/DPSAP/ montane rain forest near Andasibe, Madagascar. SSE) and 2008 (N° 258/08 /MEFT/SG/ DGEF/ The small village of Andasibe lies between the DSAP/SSE).

G. Maretti et al.

153

Tab. 1 - Composition of the study groups (2005-2008).

  • Group
  • Fieldsite
  • Number of

recorded individuals

  • Composition
  • Number of

recording sessions
Number of vocalizations per group

1R§ 2R§ 3R#
5R
Analamazaotra SR Analamazaotra SR Analamazaotra SR Analamazaotra SR Analamazaotra SR Analamazaotra SR Station Forestière Station Forestière Station Forestière Mantadia NP

  • 4
  • 1AF; 2AM; [1SAF]*

2AM; 1JM

  • 25
  • 512

  • 3
  • 7
  • 80

  • 4
  • 1AF; 2AM; [1AM]**

1AM; [1AF]**
1AF; 1SAF; 1AM
1AF

  • 7
  • 63

  • 2
  • 2
  • 7

  • 6R
  • 3
  • 11
  • 596

XR# ASF§ YSF# WSF
1M

  • 1
  • 1
  • 1

  • 48
  • 3
  • 1AF; 1AM; 1JM

2AF; 1SAF; 1AM; 1I
1AF; 1AM
8

  • 5
  • 14
  • 313

21
53
40

  • 1SAF
  • 10

  • TOT. 28
  • TOT. 82
  • TOT. 1670

§ usually visited by tourists, # seldom visited by tourists. Analamazaotra SR: Analamazaotra Special Reserve; Station Forestière: Station Forestière Mitsinjo; Mantadia NP: Mantadia National Park. * departed since 2008, ** departed since 2007. M = male, F = female, ? = sex unknown. Age classes according to Pollock 1986: A = adult (>6 years), SA = subadult (>3 years), J = juvenile (<3 years), I = infant (<1 year).

Recording methods

occurring concomitant with vocalisations were
We recorded calls using Marantz PMD671 recorded and coded into broad categories as folsolid-state recorders on compact flash memory lows: rest (remaining still or quietly monitoring cards. Recorders were equipped using Sennheiser the environment), foraging (searching, obtaining MKH-60 (with windscreen) and ME66 (with and eating food), travel (moving for a distance Sennheiser MZW66 windscreen) shotgun greater than 10 m), vigilance (looking into vegmicrophones. Sounds were digitised at 44.1 kHz etation, either quickly moving the head or with sampling rate, 16 bits amplitude resolution. No the head stationary), aggression (chasing or fightrecording was taken when tourists were near the ing between group members), agonistic (negative focal group, nor were vocalisations elicited by interaction without physical contact), territorial means of playbacks. We only recorded spontane- advertisement (sequential singing of the indri ously occurring vocalisations. Recording sessions song), anti-predatory [in presence of terrestrial or took place when researchers could directly see aerial predators or potential predators (e.g. dogs, the animals and follow them in the forest. The mongooses), or human observers]. Recording distance between the researcher and the focal sessions usually lasted 10 to 45 minutes, dependindri could range from 3 m to approximately ing on the animal movements. We analysed a 10 m. A recording session ended when the dis- total of 82 recording sessions (Tab. 1).

  • tance between the animal and the microphone
  • The software Praat 5.1.15 (Boersma &

exceeded 10 m or the animal fled away. For each Weenink, 2009) was used for visual inspection utterance, we noted the identity of the vocal- and selection of high quality vocalisations. These iser and the behavioural context. Behaviours utterances were identified on the basis of their

www.isita-org.com

Vocal repertoire of Indri indri

154

intensity, the absence of overlapping vocalisa- emphasis: 6.0 dB/Oct; dynamic compression: tions, and the degree of background noise (e.g. 0.0). The actual variation of the fundamental insects, birds, rain). We analysed a total of 1670 frequency was measured by using the autocorrevocal signals, 1514 of which were used in the dis- lation method [“Sound: To pitch (ac)…”], after

  • criminant function analyses.
  • adjusting the analysis parameters according to

The recording equipment we used for noise the range of variation in each of the vocalisations recordings was the same as that used for vocalisa- (Gamba & Giacoma, 2005).

  • tions. We considered 455 sound files, the dura-
  • Formants (F1, F2, F3) were studied using

tion of which was a minimum of 2 s, recorded linear predictive coding (LPC, Markel & Gray, before and after indri vocalisations. All ambient 1976). Because the vocal signals usually vary noise recordings were made at heights rang- with time, this process was done all along the siging between 1.5 and 4 m, with the microphone nal frames and then average formants were calcuorientated at about 45° above the horizontal lated. Depending on the acoustic characteristics towards the canopy, in order to record at the of the vocalisation, we used a window length strata mainly used by the indris. Recordings were of 0.03-0.05. Because the number of formants

  • taken between 06.00 and 13.00 h.
  • varies with vocal tract length, we measured 3-6

formants depending on the vocal type. Two methods were used in order to check the form-

Acoustic analyses

Taking the perspective of the source-filter ants predicted by LPC. First, formant analyses theory (Fant, 1960), we analysed the vocalisations were superimposed over the signal spectrogram. in the light of their being the combined product Second, autocorrelation-based LPC spectra were of two independent mechanisms: vibration of the overlaid on independently-derived FFT spectra vocal folds and filtering of the supra-laryngeal of the same frames to validate the LPC analysis. vocal cavities. Several studies have demonstrated The typical setting for the dynamic range was 25 how non-human primate acoustic signals are Hz. The formant pattern fitting was inferred durshaped by both these events (Fitch, 1997; Rendall ing a step-by-step monitored process, where the

  • et al., 2005; Gamba & Giacoma, 2006).
  • operator could interrupt the analysis and modify

We carried out all acoustic analyses using a the analysis parameters. The final output file window length of 0.05 s. The time range of the was assembled within PRAAT and exported to operating screen window was usually set to 3 s a SPSS spreadsheet. We only included variables during the acoustic analyses. We set the frequency that could be measured from all vocal types.

  • range to 0-10 kHz and dynamic range to 35 dB.
  • We generated a series of frequency spectra to

For each call, independent of the vocal cat- compare frequency distributions of the vocalisaegory to which it was assigned, we measured the tions emitted by the indris with those of the forest duration of the whole vocal emission (DUR); noise. We then visually inspected all the noise files three larynx-related features — average funda- by means of the spectrograms. Visual inspection mental frequency (MeF0), minimum fundamen- and the grouping techniques available in Praat tal frequency (MiF0), maximum fundamental (dynamic time warping) allowed us to select the frequency (MaF0); and four vocal tract-related typical frequency patterns, taking into account the acoustic features — average first formant (F1), most prominent bands and the background noise average second formant (F2) and average third (flies, birds, insects…). We then generated spectra formant (F3, see Fig. 2 in Gamba & Giacoma, from the vocalisation sound files for comparison. 2008 for details).
To detect source features (MeF0, MiF0, Statistical analysis

  • MaF0), Fast Fourier transforms were generated
  • We conducted stepwise cross-validation discri-

for all calls (frequency range: 0-12000 Hz; maxi- minant function analysis using SPSS 17.0 (SPSS mum: 50 dB/Hz; dynamic range: 30 dB; pre- Inc., USA) to classify vocalisations based on the

G. Maretti et al.

155

vocal type and to verify which parameters were most important in the classification. To reduce the possible effect of pseudo-replication (as in some Description of the vocal repertoire cases inter-dependence of the data could not be Besides the proper song notes, adult indris

Results

excluded, e.g. because of indris fleeing when they possess eight different vocal types, all of which emitted terrestrial alarm calls), we used two dif- are distinguishable by visual inspection of the ferent resampling methods: Jackknife cross-valida- spectrograms (Fig. 1) and by ear: roar (Fig. 1a), tion and a permutation resampling strategy. The honk (Fig. 1b), hum (Fig. 1c), short tonal call Jackknife was performed using the SPSS function (Fig. 1d), long tonal call (Fig. 1e), kiss (Fig. 1f), in the discriminant analysis. The permutation wheeze (Fig. 1g), grunt (Fig. 1h). To complete was performed by resampling the most numerous the description of the vocal repertoire, we provocal types, which were then subjected to both vide a spectrogram of infant calls (Fig. 1i) and of original and cross-validated discriminant analyses. the song (Fig. 1j).

  • This method reduced both the potential influence
  • A schematic overview of call usage is provided

of pseudo-replication and the influence of the in Table 2, including information concerning the different sample sizes across the vocal types. We nomenclature used for the same vocal types in randomly generated 70 data sets (total N ranging previous studies, the general context in which the between 511 and 591) in which the most numer- sound is usually uttered, a short description of ous vocal types, hums and grunts, were down- the overall acoustic structure, and a description

  • sampled to 10% of the original sets.
  • of the phonation mechanisms.

Fig. 1 - Sound spectrograms of Indri indri vocal types: Roar (a), honk (b, anticipated by voiced inha- lation), hum (c), short tonal call (d), long tonal call (e), kiss (f), wheeze (g), grunt (h), infant calls (hum, iI; contact-seeking call, iII; grunt, iIII) and the indri song (j, showing introductory roars). All spectrograms were generated in Praat 5.1.15 (www.praat.org) with the following parameters: window length: 0.02 sec, time range as shown (max. 0-62 sec); frequency range: 0-12000 Hz; maxi- mum: 100 dB/Hz; dynamic range: 30-55 dB; dynamic compression: 0.0.

www.isita-org.com

Vocal repertoire of Indri indri

156

Tab. 2 - Definition of the vocal types including synonyms used in previous works, context of emis-

sion, description of the call usage and phonation mechanics. References as follows: Petter, 1962 (1); Pollock, 1975 (2); Petter & Charles-Dominique, 1979 (3); Pollock, 1986 (4); Thalmann et al., 1991 (5); Macedonia & Stanger, 1994 (6); Sorrentino et al., (in press) (7); Giacoma et al., (in press) (8).

  • Call Type Synonyms
  • Context
  • Description
  • Phonation

  • Roar
  • Roar (4, 6),

bark (3),
Antipredatory
Loud and harsh vocalization used as a standalone vocal signal in the presence of aerial predators.
Mouth open waa note (5), aboiement (1)

  • Honk
  • Honk (3, 6),

klaxon (1), exhaled hoot (2), honk note (5)
Antipredatory
Alone, or together with other group individuals, indris produce rhythmic, rapid alerting calls given in series of short loud notes. These calls are given when terrestrial predators (e.g. fossa) or potential predators (e.g. mongoose, dog) are present. Nonhabituated indris emitted this also in presence of human observers. Inhalation before the uttering of honks is sometimes visible from the spectrograms.
Mouth open

Recommended publications
  • Fascinating Primates 3/4/13 8:09 AM Ancient Egyptians Used Traits of an Ibis Or a Hamadryas Used Traits Egyptians Ancient ) to Represent Their God Thoth

    Fascinating Primates 3/4/13 8:09 AM Ancient Egyptians Used Traits of an Ibis Or a Hamadryas Used Traits Egyptians Ancient ) to Represent Their God Thoth

    © Copyright, Princeton University Press. No part of this book may be distributed, posted, or reproduced in any form by digital or mechanical means without prior written permission of the publisher. Fascinating Primates Fascinating The Beginning of an Adventure Ever since the time of the fi rst civilizations, nonhuman primates and people have oc- cupied overlapping habitats, and it is easy to imagine how important these fi rst contacts were for our ancestors’ philosophical refl ections. Long ago, adopting a quasi- scientifi c view, some people accordingly regarded pri- mates as transformed humans. Others, by contrast, respected them as distinct be- ings, seen either as bearers of sacred properties or, conversely, as diabolical creatures. A Rapid Tour around the World In Egypt under the pharaohs, science and religion were still incompletely separated. Priests saw the Papio hamadryas living around them as “brother baboons” guarding their temples. In fact, the Egyptian god Thoth was a complex deity combining qualities of monkeys and those of other wild animal species living in rice paddies next to temples, all able to sound the alarm if thieves were skulking nearby. At fi rst, baboons represented a local god in the Nile delta who guarded sacred sites. The associated cult then spread through middle Egypt. Even- tually, this god was assimilated by the Greeks into Hermes Trismegistus, the deity measuring and interpreting time, the messenger of the gods. One conse- quence of this deifi cation was that many animals were mummifi ed after death to honor them. Ancient Egyptians used traits of an ibis or a Hamadryas Baboon (Papio hamadryas) to represent their god Thoth.
  • Fossil Lemur from Northern Madagascar (Palaeopropithecidae/Primate Evolution/Postcranium) WILLIAM L

    Fossil Lemur from Northern Madagascar (Palaeopropithecidae/Primate Evolution/Postcranium) WILLIAM L

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 88, pp. 9082-9086, October 1991 Evolution Phylogenetic and functional affinities of Babakotia (Primates), a fossil lemur from northern Madagascar (Palaeopropithecidae/primate evolution/postcranium) WILLIAM L. JUNGERSt, LAURIE R. GODFREYt, ELWYN L. SIMONS§, PRITHUJIT S. CHATRATH§, AND BERTHE RAKOTOSAMIMANANA$ tDepartment of Anatomical Sciences, State University of New York, Stony Brook, NY 117948081; tDepartment of Anthropology, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA 01003; §Department of Biological Anatomy and Anthropology and Primate Center, Duke University, Durham, NC 27705; and IService de Paldontologie, Universit6 d'Antananarivo, Antananarivo, Madagascar Contributed by Elwyn L. Simons, July 2, 1991 ABSTRACT Recent paleontological expeditions to the An- Craniodental Anatomy and Tooth Shape karana range of northern Madagascar have recovered the partial remains offour individuals ofa newly recognized extinct With an estimated body mass ofjust over 15 kg, Babakotia lemur, Babakoda radofia. Craniodental and postcranial ma- is a medium-sized indroid somewhat larger than the largest terial serve to identify Babakota as a member of the palae- living indrid (Indri) but similar in size to several of the opropithecids (also including the extinct genera Palaeopropith- smallest extinct lemurs, Mesopropithecus and Pachylemur ecus, Archaeoindris, and Mesopropithecus). Living indrids (4). A detailed description of the maxillary dentition of form the sister group to this fossil lade. The postcranial Babakotia exists
  • IMPACT REPORT Center for Conservation in Madagascar

    IMPACT REPORT Center for Conservation in Madagascar

    IMPACT REPORT 2018 Center for Conservation in Madagascar IMPACT REPORT Center for Conservation in Madagascar In-Country Location The primary geographic scope of the MFG’s conservation fieldwork and research consists of 1) Parc Ivoloina and 2) Betampona Natural Reserve. 1. Parc Ivoloina is a former forestry station that has been transformed into a 282-hectare conservation education, research and training center. Located just 30 minutes north of Tamatave (Figure 1), Parc Ivoloina is also home to a four-hectare zoo that only exhibits endemic wildlife. 2. Betampona Natural Reserve is a 2,228-hectare rainforest fragment that contains high levels of plant and animal diversity (Figure 2). Recent research has Photo 1: Diademed sifaka in Betampona Natural Reserve shown Betampona to be a small center of endemism Background Summary for both amphibian and reptile species. There is little doubt that the MFG’s continual research presence Founded in 2004, the Center for Conservation in has protected Betampona from large-scale habitat Madagascar is one of the original centers of the loss and poaching. Saint Louis Zoo WildCare Institute. The basis of the Center is the Madagascar Fauna and Flora Group Figure 1. Primary locations of MFG presence (MFG), which is an international non-governmental in Madagascar. organization that collaborates with and supports in-country staff to achieve its conservation mission. The Zoo has invested expertise and funding in this organization since its inception in 1988. The Saint Louis Zoo has assumed chairmanship of the MFG twice; first with Dr. Jeffrey Bonner, President & CEO of the Saint Louis Zoo, from 2003 to 2006, and second under Dr.
  • REVIEW ARTICLE Agroecosystems and Primate Conservation in the Tropics: a Review

    REVIEW ARTICLE Agroecosystems and Primate Conservation in the Tropics: a Review

    American Journal of Primatology 74:696–711 (2012) REVIEW ARTICLE Agroecosystems and Primate Conservation in The Tropics: A Review ∗ ALEJANDRO ESTRADA1 , BECKY E. RABOY2,3, AND LEONARDO C. OLIVEIRA3-6 1Estaci´on de Biolog´ıa Tropical Los Tuxtlas Instituto de Biolog´ıa, Universidad Nacional Aut´onoma de M´exico, Mexico City, Mexico 2Conservation Ecology Center, Smithsonian Conservation Biology Institute, National Zoological Park, Washington, DC 3Instituto de Estudos S´ocioambientais do Sul da Bahia (IESB), Ilh´eus-BA, Brazil 4Programa de P´os-Gradua¸c˜ao em Ecologia, Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil 5Programa de P´os-Gradua¸c˜ao em Ecologia e Conserva¸c˜ao da Biodiversidade, Universidade Estadual de Santa Cruz, Ilh´eus-BA, Brazil 6Bicho do Mato Instituto de Pesquisa, Belo Horizonte-MG, Brazil Agroecosystems cover more than one quarter of the global land area (ca. 50 million km2) as highly simplified (e.g. pasturelands) or more complex systems (e.g. polycultures and agroforestry systems) with the capacity to support higher biodiversity. Increasingly more information has been published about primates in agroecosystems but a general synthesis of the diversity of agroecosystems that primates use or which primate taxa are able to persist in these anthropogenic components of the landscapes is still lacking. Because of the continued extensive transformation of primate habitat into human-modified landscapes, it is important to explore the extent to which agroecosystems are used by primates. In this article, we reviewed published information on the use of agroecosystems by primates in habitat countries and also discuss the potential costs and benefits to human and nonhuman primates of primate use of agroecosystems.
  • Coexistence of Confamilial, Folivorous Indriids, Propithecus Diadema And

    Coexistence of Confamilial, Folivorous Indriids, Propithecus Diadema And

    Washington University in St. Louis Washington University Open Scholarship Arts & Sciences Electronic Theses and Dissertations Arts & Sciences Spring 5-15-2017 Coexistence of Confamilial, Folivorous Indriids, Propithecus diadema and Indri indri, at Betampona Strict Nature Reserve, Madagascar Lana Kerker Oliver Washington University in St. Louis Follow this and additional works at: https://openscholarship.wustl.edu/art_sci_etds Part of the Biodiversity Commons, Biological and Physical Anthropology Commons, Natural Resources and Conservation Commons, and the Natural Resources Management and Policy Commons Recommended Citation Oliver, Lana Kerker, "Coexistence of Confamilial, Folivorous Indriids, Propithecus diadema and Indri indri, at Betampona Strict Nature Reserve, Madagascar" (2017). Arts & Sciences Electronic Theses and Dissertations. 1134. https://openscholarship.wustl.edu/art_sci_etds/1134 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Arts & Sciences at Washington University Open Scholarship. It has been accepted for inclusion in Arts & Sciences Electronic Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Washington University Open Scholarship. For more information, please contact [email protected]. WASHINGTON UNIVERSITY IN ST. LOUIS Department of Anthropology Dissertation Examination Committee Crickette Sanz, Chair Kari Allen Benjamin Z. Freed Jane Phillips-Conroy David Strait Mrinalini Watsa Coexistence of Confamilial, Folivorous Indriids, Propithecus diadema and Indri indri, at Betampona Strict
  • World's Most Endangered Primates

    World's Most Endangered Primates

    Primates in Peril The World’s 25 Most Endangered Primates 2016–2018 Edited by Christoph Schwitzer, Russell A. Mittermeier, Anthony B. Rylands, Federica Chiozza, Elizabeth A. Williamson, Elizabeth J. Macfie, Janette Wallis and Alison Cotton Illustrations by Stephen D. Nash IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group (PSG) International Primatological Society (IPS) Conservation International (CI) Bristol Zoological Society (BZS) Published by: IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group (PSG), International Primatological Society (IPS), Conservation International (CI), Bristol Zoological Society (BZS) Copyright: ©2017 Conservation International All rights reserved. No part of this report may be reproduced in any form or by any means without permission in writing from the publisher. Inquiries to the publisher should be directed to the following address: Russell A. Mittermeier, Chair, IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group, Conservation International, 2011 Crystal Drive, Suite 500, Arlington, VA 22202, USA. Citation (report): Schwitzer, C., Mittermeier, R.A., Rylands, A.B., Chiozza, F., Williamson, E.A., Macfie, E.J., Wallis, J. and Cotton, A. (eds.). 2017. Primates in Peril: The World’s 25 Most Endangered Primates 2016–2018. IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group (PSG), International Primatological Society (IPS), Conservation International (CI), and Bristol Zoological Society, Arlington, VA. 99 pp. Citation (species): Salmona, J., Patel, E.R., Chikhi, L. and Banks, M.A. 2017. Propithecus perrieri (Lavauden, 1931). In: C. Schwitzer, R.A. Mittermeier, A.B. Rylands, F. Chiozza, E.A. Williamson, E.J. Macfie, J. Wallis and A. Cotton (eds.), Primates in Peril: The World’s 25 Most Endangered Primates 2016–2018, pp. 40-43. IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group (PSG), International Primatological Society (IPS), Conservation International (CI), and Bristol Zoological Society, Arlington, VA.
  • The Effects of Habitat Disturbance on the Populations of Geoffroy's Spider Monkeys in the Yucatan Peninsula

    The Effects of Habitat Disturbance on the Populations of Geoffroy's Spider Monkeys in the Yucatan Peninsula

    The Effects of Habitat Disturbance on the Populations of Geoffroy’s Spider Monkeys in the Yucatan Peninsula PhD thesis Denise Spaan Supervisor: Filippo Aureli Co-supervisor: Gabriel Ramos-Fernández August 2017 Instituto de Neuroetología Universidad Veracruzana 1 For the spider monkeys of the Yucatan Peninsula, and all those dedicated to their conservation. 2 Acknowledgements This thesis turned into the biggest project I have ever attempted and it could not have been completed without the invaluable help and support of countless people and organizations. A huge thank you goes out to my supervisors Drs. Filippo Aureli and Gabriel Ramos- Fernández. Thank you for your guidance, friendship and encouragement, I have learnt so much and truly enjoyed this experience. This thesis would not have been possible without you and I am extremely proud of the results. Additionally, I would like to thank Filippo Aureli for all his help in organizing the logistics of field work. Your constant help and dedication to this project has been inspiring, and kept me pushing forward even when it was not always easy to do so, so thank you very much. I would like to thank Dr. Martha Bonilla for offering me an amazing estancia at the INECOL. Your kind words have encouraged and inspired me throughout the past three years, and have especially helped me to get through the last few months. Thank you! A big thank you to Drs. Colleen Schaffner and Jorge Morales Mavil for all your feedback and ideas over the past three years. Colleen, thank you for helping me to feel at home in Mexico and for all your support! I very much look forward to continue working with all of you in the future! I would like to thank the CONACYT for my PhD scholarship and the Instituto de Neuroetología for logistical, administrative and financial support.
  • Blood Biochemistry Reference Values of Javan Slow Loris (Nycticebus Javanicus) in Rehabilitation Center

    Blood Biochemistry Reference Values of Javan Slow Loris (Nycticebus Javanicus) in Rehabilitation Center

    ACTA VETERINARIA INDONESIANA Special Issues: 127-131, May 2021 P-ISSN 2337-3202, E-ISSN 2337-4373 Research Blood Biochemistry Reference Values of Javan Slow Loris (Nycticebus javanicus) in Rehabilitation Center Nur Purba Priambada1*, Indri Saptorini1, Imam Arifin1, Wendi Prameswari1, Ursula Halla1, Karmele Llano Sanchez1, Joost Philippa1 1Yayasan Inisiasi Alam Rehabilitasi Indonesia, Jl. Curug Nangka, Sinarwangi RT 003/00, Kelurahan Sukajadi, Kecamatan Tamansari, Bogor, Jawa Barat, Indonesia - 16610 *Corresponding author: [email protected] Submitted April 15, 2021 Accepted May 16, 2021 ABSTRACT The Javan slow loris (Nycticebus javanicus) is an endemic primate species to Java Island, Indonesia. Currently, their conservation status is critically endangered due to habitat loss and the illegal wildlife trade. As a consequence of the pet trade, wild-caught slow lorises are confiscated or handed over to centers like IAR Indonesia Rescue Center. Rescued lorises present multiple health issues following stress, trauma, and miss- treatment after being kept as pets. During the medical evaluation, besides physical examination, blood biochemistry provides valuable diagnostic information. However, data on physiological values are unavailable and therefore interpretation of results is difficult. The objective of this study was to establish blood biochemistry reference values for wild, rehabilitant healthy adult Javan slow lorises in captivity. We anesthetized 20 individuals of Javan slow loris (10 males and 10 females) for pre-release check-up procedures. Blood samples were collected for blood biochemistry analysis on an in-house Vetscan VS2 (Zoetis), after which the results were statistically analyzed for mean and standard deviation. Results showed different values between the male and female group, however, they were not significant (p>0,05).
  • Laboratory Primate Newsletter

    Laboratory Primate Newsletter

    LABORATORY PRIMATE NEWSLETTER Vol. 49, No. 4 October 2010 JUDITH E. SCHRIER, EDITOR JAMES S. HARPER, GORDON J. HANKINSON AND LARRY HULSEBOS, ASSOCIATE EDITORS MORRIS L. POVAR AND JASON MACHAN, CONSULTING EDITORS ELVA MATHIESEN, ASSISTANT EDITOR ALLAN M. SCHRIER, FOUNDING EDITOR, 1962-1987 Published Quarterly by the Schrier Research Laboratory Psychology Department, Brown University Providence, Rhode Island ISSN 0023-6861 POLICY STATEMENT The Laboratory Primate Newsletter provides a central source of information about nonhuman primates and related matters to scientists who use these animals in their research and those whose work supports such research. The Newsletter (1) provides information on care and breeding of nonhuman primates for laboratory research, (2) disseminates general information and news about the world of primate research (such as announcements of meetings, research projects, sources of information, nomenclature changes), (3) helps meet the special research needs of individual investigators by publishing requests for research material or for information related to specific research problems, and (4) serves the cause of conservation of nonhuman primates by publishing information on that topic. As a rule, research articles or summaries accepted for the Newsletter have some practical implications or provide general information likely to be of interest to investigators in a variety of areas of primate research. However, special consideration will be given to articles containing data on primates not conveniently publishable elsewhere. General descriptions of current research projects on primates will also be welcome. The Newsletter appears quarterly and is intended primarily for persons doing research with nonhuman primates. Back issues may be purchased for $10.00 each. We are no longer printing paper issues, except those we will send to subscribers who have paid in advance.
  • Revised 22/05/2009

    Revised 22/05/2009

    2009R0407 — EN — 22.05.2009 — 000.001 — 1 This document is meant purely as a documentation tool and the institutions do not assume any liability for its contents ►B COMMISSION REGULATION (EC) No 407/2009 of 14 May 2009 amending Council Regulation (EC) No 338/97 on the protection of species of wild fauna and flora by regulating trade therein (OJ L 123, 19.5.2009, p. 3) Corrected by: ►C1 Corrigendum, OJ L 139, 5.6.2009, p. 35 (407/2009) 2009R0407 — EN — 22.05.2009 — 000.001 — 2 ▼B COMMISSION REGULATION (EC) No 407/2009 of 14 May 2009 amending Council Regulation (EC) No 338/97 on the protection of species of wild fauna and flora by regulating trade therein THE COMMISSION OF THE EUROPEAN COMMUNITIES, Having regard to the Treaty establishing the European Community, Having regard to Council Regulation (EC) No 338/97 of 9 December 1996 on the protection of species of wild fauna and flora by regulating trade therein (1), and in particular Article 19(3) thereof, Whereas: (1) Regulation (EC) No 338/97 lists animal and plant species in respect of which trade is restricted or controlled. Those lists incorporate the lists set out in the Appendices to the Convention on International Trade in Endangered Species of Wild Fauna and Flora, hereinafter ‘the CITES Convention’. (2) The following species have been added to Appendix III to the CITES Convention at the request of China: Corallium elatius, Corallium japonicum, Corallium konjoi and Corallium secundum. (3) The species Crax daubentoni, Crax globulosa, Crax rubra, Ortalis vetula, Pauxi pauxi, Penelopina
  • Primate Families-Short

    Primate Families-Short

    The Primates compiled by Dana Visalli A male Mandrill Primates arose from ancestors that lived in the trees of tropical forests; many primate characteristics represent adaptations to life in this challenging three-dimensional environment. Most primate species remain at least partly arboreal. There are a surprisingly large number of primates, or ‘monkeys and their kin.’ The total number is variable, but settles in the range of approximately 325 species. The word ‘primate’ comes from a Latin root meaning ‘first rank’ or ‘numeral uno,’ a reflection of ever-present an- thropocentrism (human self-centeredness) of our species, which tends to judge nearby Homo sapiens (which means ‘wise man’) as the pinnacle of the infinitely long evolutionary journey, while those Homo sapiens at a distance are often considered to be ‘the other’ and ‘the enemy’ and therefore not wise at all. Primates range in size from a tiny lemur in Madagascar called Madame Berthe’s Mouse Lemur, which weighs one ounce, to the lowland gorillas in Africa, which weigh in at well over 400 pounds. Genetic studies indicate that the primate line originated about 85 million years ago, in the mid-Cretaceous Peri- od. New primate species are still being discovered. More than 25 species were taxonomically described in the decade of the 2000s and eleven have been described since 2010. Primates are characterized by large brains relative to other mammals, as well as an increased reliance on stereoscopic vision at the expense of smell, the dominant sensory system in most mammals. These features are more developed in monkeys and apes and noticeably less so in lorises and lemurs.
  • Lemurs of Madagascar – a Strategy for Their

    Lemurs of Madagascar – a Strategy for Their

    Cover photo: Diademed sifaka (Propithecus diadema), Critically Endangered. (Photo: Russell A. Mittermeier) Back cover photo: Indri (Indri indri), Critically Endangered. (Photo: Russell A. Mittermeier) Lemurs of Madagascar A Strategy for Their Conservation 2013–2016 Edited by Christoph Schwitzer, Russell A. Mittermeier, Nicola Davies, Steig Johnson, Jonah Ratsimbazafy, Josia Razafindramanana, Edward E. Louis Jr., and Serge Rajaobelina Illustrations and layout by Stephen D. Nash IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group Bristol Conservation and Science Foundation Conservation International This publication was supported by the Conservation International/Margot Marsh Biodiversity Foundation Primate Action Fund, the Bristol, Clifton and West of England Zoological Society, Houston Zoo, the Institute for the Conservation of Tropical Environments, and Primate Conservation, Inc. Published by: IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group, Bristol Conservation and Science Foundation, and Conservation International Copyright: © 2013 IUCN Reproduction of this publication for educational or other non-commercial purposes is authorized without prior written permission from the copyright holder provided the source is fully acknowledged. Reproduction of this publication for resale or other commercial purposes is prohibited without prior written permission of the copyright holder. Inquiries to the publisher should be directed to the following address: Russell A. Mittermeier, Chair, IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group, Conservation International, 2011 Crystal Drive, Suite 500, Arlington, VA 22202, USA Citation: Schwitzer C, Mittermeier RA, Davies N, Johnson S, Ratsimbazafy J, Razafindramanana J, Louis Jr. EE, Rajaobelina S (eds). 2013. Lemurs of Madagascar: A Strategy for Their Conservation 2013–2016. Bristol, UK: IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group, Bristol Conservation and Science Foundation, and Conservation International. 185 pp. ISBN: 978-1-934151-62-4 Illustrations: © Stephen D.