Cultural Variation in Affect Valuation

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Cultural Variation in Affect Valuation PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES Cultural Variation in Affect Valuation Jeanne L. Tsai and Brian Knutson Helene H. Fung Stanford University Chinese University of Hong Kong The authors propose that how people want to feel (“ideal affect”) differs from how they actually feel (“actual affect”) and that cultural factors influence ideal more than actual affect. In 2 studies, controlling for actual affect, the authors found that European American (EA) and Asian American (AA) individuals value high-arousal positive affect (e.g., excitement) more than do Hong Kong Chinese (CH). On the other hand, CH and AA individuals value low-arousal positive affect (e.g., calm) more than do EA individuals. For all groups, the discrepancy between ideal and actual affect correlates with depression. These findings illustrate the distinctiveness of ideal and actual affect, show that culture influences ideal affect more than actual affect, and indicate that both play a role in mental health. Keywords: culture, affect, emotion, values, temperament How does culture shape emotion? The answer has important 1994). These reports have led many scholars to conclude that implications for mental health interventions, because many psy- emotional experience is largely culturally determined (e.g., Klein- chiatric disorders feature various types of emotional distress as a man & Good, 1985; Shweder, 1994; Wierzbicka, 1994). core symptom. Ethnographic and empirical studies diverge in their Empirical findings, however, indicate that cultural differences in answers. Ethnographic accounts report significant variation in emotion may not be as profound as suggested by ethnographic emotional experience in China (Ots, 1990; Potter, 1988), India reports. For instance, Scherer and colleagues (Scherer, 1997; (Shweder & Haidt, 2002), Indonesia (Heider, 1991; Lutz, 1988), Scherer & Wallbott, 1994) found more similarities than differences the Mediterranean (Gaines & Farmer, 1986), Poland (Wierzbicka, in reported emotional responses to prototypical emotion-eliciting 1994), Tahiti (Levy, 1983), and the United States (Wierzbicka, events among people from 37 different nations. Oishi (2002) found no significant differences between Asian American (AA) and European American (EA) individuals in reports of online emo- tional experience over the course of a week. Breugelmans et al. Jeanne L. Tsai and Brian Knutson, Department of Psychology, Stanford (2005) observed that Raramuri Indians, rural Javanese, and college University; Helene H. Fung, Department of Psychology, Chinese Univer- sity of Hong Kong. students from Belgium, Indonesia, and Mexico were more similar This research was funded by National Institute of Mental Health Grant than different in the bodily sensations they associated with specific R01MH068879, National Institute on Aging Grant R03AG023302, Na- emotions. Finally, we have observed more similarities than differ- tional Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression Grant ences in online reports of emotional experience, facial expressions, UAYFY, and a Stanford Office of Technology and Licensing Grant and measures of autonomic nervous system activity (e.g., heart awarded to Jeanne L. Tsai; National Institute on Aging Grant Seed Grant rate, skin conductance) when EA and AA participants conversed AG024957-02 awarded to Brian Knutson; and Hong Kong Research with romantic partners (Tsai & Levenson, 1997), watched emo- Grants Council Earmarked Research Grant CUHK4256/03H and a Chinese tional films (Tsai, Levenson, & Carstensen, 2000), and relived University of Hong Kong Direct Grant awarded to Helene H. Fung. emotional events (Tsai, Chentsova-Dutton, Friere-Bebeau, & We thank Richard Ryan and Martin Lynch for their comments and suggestions as well as Yulia Chentsova-Dutton, Stephanie Fryberg, May- Przymus, 2002). Together, these findings suggest that cultural ling Halim, Anna Kaiser, Iris Lee, Veronica Marin, Felicity Miao, Chelsey similarities outweigh cultural differences in emotion. Morrell, Trang Pham, Julie Sandler, Emma Seppala, Alicia Sheen, Susanna Moreover, when group differences in emotional experience are Tan, Lilian Tang, Ying Wong, and Saiwing Yeung for their assistance with observed (e.g., Mesquita & Karasawa, 2002; Scollon, Diener, the project. We would also like to express our gratitude to Mike Cheung for Oishi, & Biswas-Diener, 2004), it is often unclear whether they his assistance in using structural equation modeling (SEM) to assess arise from cultural or other factors. Research has demonstrated a measurement equivalence in Study 2 and for referring us to articles related moderate to high (r ϭ .5 to .8) correlation between personality to testing nested models in SEM and to Gregory A. Miller for his consul- traits related to temperament (i.e., extraversion and neuroticism: tation regarding the conceptual and statistical assumptions of analysis of covariance. also called “affective traits”) and self-reported emotional experi- Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jeanne L. ence in people from a variety of different cultures (e.g., Costa & Tsai, Department of Psychology, Building 420, Jordan Hall, Stanford, CA McCrae, 1980; David, Green, Martin, & Suls, 1997; Emmons & 94305. E-mail: [email protected] Diener, 1985; Gomez, Cooper, & Gomez, 2000; Gross, Sutton, & Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2006, Vol. 90, No. 2, 288–307 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0022-3514/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.2.288 288 CULTURE AND AFFECT VALUATION 289 Ketelaar, 1998; McCrae, Costa, & Yik, 1996; Rusting & Larsen, empirically distinct constructs. Whereas ideal affect refers to a 1997; Schimmack, Radhakrishnan, Oishi, & Dzokoto, 2002). Be- goal, actual affect refers to a response. Whereas ideal affect cause these traits also show moderate heritability (e.g., Bouchard, requires some understanding of different affective states and their 1994; Finkel, Wille, & Matheny, 1998; Goldsmith & Campos, contingencies, actual affect does not. Finally, because most people 1986; Goldsmith & Lemery, 2000; Hettema, Neale, & Kendler, want to feel good, ideal affect should primarily involve different 2001; Krueger, 2000; Lemery & Goldsmith, 2002; Plomin & positive states, whereas actual affect should instead involve the Caspi, 1999; Tellegen, Lykken, Bouchard, & Wilcox, 1988), the- entire spectrum of affective states. In support of the prediction that orists have argued that genetic factors account for a greater pro- ideal and actual affect are distinct constructs, two previous studies portion of the variance in affective experience than do environ- have demonstrated that participants’ ratings of their own affective mental factors (Diener & Lucas, 1999; Lykken & Tellegen, 1996). experience differ significantly from their ratings of how desirable Because these traits vary across different national samples (Allik they perceive those states to be (Feldman Barrett, 1996; Rusting & & McCrae, 2004), it is possible that national differences in self- Larsen, 1995). reported affective experience result in part from temperamental Most scientists, however, do not distinguish between actual and differences. However, most cross-cultural studies of emotion fail ideal affect and, therefore, implicitly treat actual affect and ideal to measure these affective traits, and therefore, they cannot rule out affect as a single construct. In part, this conflation of actual and this possibility. Thus, the influence of cultural variables versus ideal affect may reflect genuine overlap between the two (i.e., their affective traits on emotion remains largely unexplored. shared variance). If ideal affect and actual affect are distinct entities, then this overlap may reflect the various ways in which Affect Valuation Theory actual and ideal affect interact. For example, actual and ideal affect could reinforce each other; that is, the more a person feels excited, How can the small cultural differences in emotional experience the more she or he may seek further excitement. Similarly, people reported by scientists and the large cultural differences reported by who value excitement may engage in exciting activities, which ethnographers be reconciled? To address this question, we intro- may in turn increase their actual feelings of excitement. However, duce affect valuation theory (AVT), which predicts that (a) “ideal AVT predicts that although actual and ideal affect may interact, affect” (i.e., the affective states that people value and would ideally they are also distinct. For the rest of the article, we focus on this like to feel) differs from “actual affect” (i.e., the affective states distinctiveness and refer to the nonoverlapping variance as the that people actually feel); and (b) cultural factors shape ideal affect “pure” components of ideal and actual affect and to their shared more than actual affect, whereas temperament shapes actual affect variance as the “interactive” component. A raw measure of actual more than ideal affect. Thus, AVT suggests that the discrepancy and ideal affect should include both pure and interactive compo- between ethnographic accounts and empirical research may be due nents, and therefore, unless otherwise specified, we use the terms to their different ways of measuring emotional experience. By “ideal affect” and “actual affect” when referring to both. administering measures that ask people to rate how intensely or frequently they experience various states, scientists have primarily Cultural Factors
Recommended publications
  • Cultural Distance, Perception of Emotional Display Rules, And
    CULTURAL DISTANCE, PERCEPTION OF EMOTIONAL DISPLAY RULES, AND THEIR INFLUENCE ON SOJOURNER ADJUSTMENT A thesis presented to the faculty of the College of Arts and Sciences of Ohio University In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree Master of Science Nicole L. Gullekson August 2007 2 This thesis titled CULTURAL DISTANCE, PERCEPTION OF EMOTIONAL DISPLAY RULES, AND THEIR INFLUENCE ON SOJOURNER ADJUSTMENT by NICOLE L. GULLEKSON has been approved for the Department of Psychology and the College of Arts and Sciences by ________________________________________________ Jeffrey B. Vancouver Associate Professor of Psychology ________________________________________________ Benjamin M. Ogles Dean, College of Arts and Sciences 3 Abstract GULLEKSON, NICOLE L., M.S., August 2007, Experimental Psychology CULTURAL DISTANCE, PERCEPTION OF EMOTIONAL DISPLAY RULES, AND THEIR INFLUENCE ON SOJOURNER ADJUSTMENT (109 pp.) With increased globalization, more individuals temporarily leave home to work and study in foreign countries. These sojourners are confronted with societal norms different from their home cultures. The present study investigated the extent to which international student sojourners perceive differences in emotional display norms between their home and host cultures, as well as the influence of such perception on adjustment. Although accurate perception of the host culture’s emotional display rules was not related to adjustment, a “guest” effect existed. Specifically, international student participants reported that one should
    [Show full text]
  • Buddhism and Holistic Versus Analytic Thought
    CULTURE, RELIGION AND COGNITION: BUDDHISM AND HOLISTIC VERSUS ANALYTIC THOUGHT by Alain Samson A thesis submitted for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy (PhD) 2007 University of London The London School of Economics and Political Science Institute of Social Psychology UMI Number: U615882 All rights reserved INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. Dissertation Publishing UMI U615882 Published by ProQuest LLC 2014. Copyright in the Dissertation held by the Author. Microform Edition © ProQuest LLC. All rights reserved. This work is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code. ProQuest LLC 789 East Eisenhower Parkway P.O. Box 1346 Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346 Declaration I certify that the thesis I have presented for examination for the MPhil/PhD degree of the London School of Economics and Political Science is solely my own work other than where I have clearly indicated that it is the work of others (in which case the extent of any work carried out jointly by me and any other person is clearly identified in it). The copyright of this thesis rests with the author. Quotation from it is permitted, provided that full acknowledgement is made. This thesis may not be reproduced without the prior written consent of the author. I warrant that this authorization does not, to the best of my belief, infringe the rights of 2 British ity<;(.
    [Show full text]
  • Cultural Neuroscience: a Historical Introduction and Overview Nicholas O
    Unit 9 Biological Psychology, Neuropsychology and Culture Article 1 Subunit 2 Neuropsychology and Culture 8-1-2014 Cultural Neuroscience: A Historical Introduction and Overview Nicholas O. Rule University of Toronto, [email protected] Please address correspondence to: Nicholas O. Rule, Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, [email protected]; this work was supported in part by NSERC 491593. Recommended Citation Rule, N. O. (2014). Cultural Neuroscience: A Historical Introduction and Overview. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture, 9(2). https://doi.org/10.9707/2307-0919.1128 This Online Readings in Psychology and Culture Article is brought to you for free and open access (provided uses are educational in nature)by IACCP and ScholarWorks@GVSU. Copyright © 2014 International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology. All Rights Reserved. ISBN 978-0-9845627-0-1 Cultural Neuroscience: A Historical Introduction and Overview Abstract The integration of cognitive neuroscience with the study of culture emerged from independent ascensions among both fields in the early 1990s. This marriage of the two previously unconnected areas of inquiry has generated a variety of empirical and theoretical works that have provided unique insights to both partners that might have otherwise gone overlooked. Here, I provide a brief historical introduction to the emergence of cultural neuroscience from its roots in cultural psychology and cognitive neuroscience to its present stature as one of the most challenging but rewarding sub-disciplines to have come from the burgeoning growth of the study of the brain and behavior. In doing so, I overview some of the more studied areas within cultural neuroscience: language, music, mathematics, visual perception, and social cognition.
    [Show full text]
  • Migration, Acculturation, and the Maintenance of Between-Group Cultural Variation’
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/234807; this version posted June 13, 2018. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license. 1 Migration, acculturation, and the maintenance of 2 between-group cultural variation ∗ 3 Alex Mesoudi 4 12 June, 2018 5 Abstract 6 How do migration and acculturation (i.e. psychological or behavioral change resulting from migration) 7 affect within- and between-group cultural variation? Here I answer this question by drawing analogies 8 between genetic and cultural evolution. Population genetic models show that migration rapidly 9 breaks down between-group genetic structure. In cultural evolution, however, migrants or their 10 descendants can acculturate to local behaviors via social learning processes such as conformity, 11 potentially preventing migration from eliminating between-group cultural variation. An analysis 12 of the empirical literature on migration suggests that acculturation is common, with second and 13 subsequent migrant generations shifting, sometimes substantially, towards the cultural values of the 14 adopted society. Yet there is little understanding of the individual-level dynamics that underlie these 15 population-level shifts. To explore this formally, I present models quantifying the effect of migration 16 and acculturation on between-group cultural variation, for both neutral and costly cooperative 17 traits. In the models, between-group cultural variation, measured using F statistics, is eliminated by 18 migration and maintained by conformist acculturation. The extent of acculturation is determined 19 by the strength of conformist bias and the number of demonstrators from whom individuals learn.
    [Show full text]
  • An Introduction to Cultural Neuroscience Lyndac.Linandevah.Telzer
    An Introduction to Cultural Neuroscience LyndaC.LinandEvaH.Telzer This chapter’s goal is to serve as a brief introduction to the emerging feld of cultural neuroscience. It does not intend to provide an extensive review but rather a brief overview of the feld. We start by defning cultural neu- roscience and follow by providing important reasons for doing research in this feld. Next we explain some key terms and methodologies. We then highlight important studies in the feld and provide recommendations for designing a study. Finally, we make suggestions for future directions. Throughout this chapter, we choose a few specifcexamplestoillustrate ways in which cultural neuroscience researchers have utilized the concepts and tools described here. What Is Cultural Neuroscience? Cultural neuroscience is an emerging interdisciplinary feld that combines theories and methods from cultural and social psychology, anthropology, and social and cognitive neuroscience to investigate the interactions between culture, psychological processes, brain, and genes at diferent timescales (for reviews, see Chiao, Cheon, Pornpattananangkul, Mrazek, & Blizinsky, 2013; Han et al., 2013; Kim & Sasaki, 2014). Because of the broad interdisciplinary nature of the feld, describing all of the diferent ways in which cultural neuroscientists do research in this area is beyond the scope of this chapter. Here we will be focusing on the most common method, which is representations in the brain through neuroimaging techniques. Because this feld lies at the intersection of many areas of study, we begin by describing how cultural neuroscience derives from each of these The Handbook of Culture and Biology,FirstEdition.EditedbyJose´ M. Causadias, Eva H. Telzer and Nancy A.
    [Show full text]
  • Society and Culture
    03 CHAPTER Society and Culture America is the only culture that went from barbarism to decadence without civilization in between. —Oscar Wilde LEARNING OUTCOMES The request “May I take your order, please?” is familiar to everyone in the world today. Obviously you are in a McDonald’s, Burger King, 01 Define society and culture and Starbucks, or some other fast-food restaurant, where global corpo- give examples of different types of rations have programmed worker greetings, worker routines, and societies. even worker attitudes to a standardized formula. You can be assured that the menu, the food, and the service will be the same no matter 02 List and give examples of the major where the particular restaurant is located, but things are not what they components of culture. seem. Many people believe that McDonald’s, for instance, is such a potent source of American fast-food culture and American culture in 03 Define and give examples of general that when it transplants itself overseas, “American values” ethnocentrism and cultural are widely disseminated. In effect, this thesis holds that no matter relativism. the cultural setting, the arrival of the Golden Arches inevitably leads 04 Explain globalization and cultural to cultural homogenization on the American model, a trend that is diversity. supposedly sweeping the globe (Ritzer, 2018). Once again, however, things may not be what they seem. Although McDonald’ s relies on 05 Discuss society and culture from standardization and emphasizes that a person always knows what each of the major theoretical to expect from McDonald’s anywhere in the world, local franchises perspectives in sociology.
    [Show full text]
  • Cultural Diversity and Conformity
    Cultural Diversity and Conformity Chapter 2: Cultural Diversity and Conformity Case Study: Body Ritual Among the Nacirema Section 1: The Meaning of Culture Section 2: Cultural Variation Section 3: The American Value System Lab: Applying What You’ve Learned Original Content Copyright © Holt McDougal. Additions and changes to the original content are the responsibility of the instructor. Cultural Diversity and Conformity Case Study: Body Ritual Among the Nacirema Ceremonies and rituals are an important part of any culture. In the 1950s, Horace Miner examined some of the rituals of the Nacirema culture. His description of the culture included a portrait of an average Nacirema and his or her daily body ritual. Although the language Miner used made the culture seem exotic and strange, the description was a truthful representation of the American (Nacirema spelled backwards) morning ritual. Original Content Copyright © Holt McDougal. Additions and changes to the original content are the responsibility of the instructor. Cultural Diversity and Conformity Section 1 at a Glance The Meaning of Culture • Culture is made up of the material and nonmaterial products of human groups. • A society is a group of interdependent people who share a common culture and feeling of unity. Society differs from culture in that societies are made up of people and cultures are made up of products. • All cultures share certain elements: technology, symbols, language, values, and norms. Original Content Copyright © Holt McDougal. Additions and changes to the original content are the responsibility of the instructor. Cultural Diversity and Conformity How did the car influence American culture? Original Content Copyright © Holt McDougal.
    [Show full text]
  • Universals and Cultural Variations in Emotional Expression by Daniel Thomas Cordaro a Dissertation Submitted in Partial Satisfac
    Universals and Cultural Variations in Emotional Expression By Daniel Thomas Cordaro A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Psychology in the Graduate Division of the University of California, Berkeley Committee in charge: Professor Dacher Keltner, Chair Professor Iris Mauss Professor Cameron Anderson Spring 2014 1 Abstract Universals and Cultural Variations in Emotional Expression by Daniel Thomas Cordaro Doctor of Philosophy in Psychology University of California, Berkeley Professor Dacher Keltner, Chair One of the most fascinating characteristics of emotions is that they have universal expressive patterns. These expressions, which are encoded through multiple bodily channels, allow us to identify distinct emotions in ourselves and others. After groundbreaking theorizing by Charles Darwin and early empirical work by Ekman and Izard, further research in emotion science revealed evidence in single cultures for more emotional expressions above and beyond the well- studied set comprising of anger, contempt, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness, and surprise. Some of these new emotions were displayed using nonfacial modalities, such as the voice, touch, and posture. Following the logic set forth by these methods and findings, we tested two hypotheses: 1) there are more than seven universal expressions of emotion; 2) expressive universality is not limited to facial expression. Findings from three cross-cultural experimental studies yielded support to the above hypotheses. Our first study collected 5500 expressions of twenty-one emotions from five different cultures. Spontaneous behavioral analysis revealed core facial and bodily patterns for each of the 21 states across all five cultures. Additionally, we uncovered over 100 cultural variations to the universal patterns and hundreds of individual variations.
    [Show full text]
  • Cultural Diversity, Geographical Isolation, and the Origin of The
    Cultural Diversity, Geographical Isolation, and the Origin of the Wealth of Nations Quamrul Ashrafy Oded Galorz November 2011 Abstract This research argues that variations in the interplay between cultural assimilation and cultural di¤usion have played a signi…cant role in giving rise to di¤erential patterns of economic devel- opment across the globe. Societies that were geographically less vulnerable to cultural di¤usion bene…ted from enhanced assimilation, lower cultural diversity, and more intense accumulation of society-speci…c human capital. Thus, they operated more e¢ ciently with respect to their production-possibility frontiers and ‡ourished in the technological paradigm that characterized the agricultural stage of development. The lack of cultural di¤usion and its manifestation in cultural rigidity, however, diminished the ability of these societies to adapt to a new technological paradigm, which delayed their industrialization and, hence, their take-o¤ to a state of sustained economic growth. The theory thus contributes to the understanding of the advent of divergence and overtaking in the process of development. Consistently with the theory, the empirical analysis establishes that (i) geographical isolation prevalent in pre-industrial times (i.e., prior to the advent of airborne transportation technology) has had a persistent negative impact on the extent of contemporary cultural diversity; (ii) pre-industrial geographical isolation had a positive impact on economic development in the agricultural stage but has had a negative impact on income per capita in the course of industrialization; and (iii) cultural diversity, as determined exogenously by pre-industrial geographical isolation, has had a positive impact on economic development in the process of industrialization.
    [Show full text]
  • Menopause As a Social and Cultural Construction Bre'on Kelly
    XULAneXUS Volume 8 | Issue 2 Article 4 4-1-2011 Menopause as a Social and Cultural Construction Bre'on Kelly Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.xula.edu/xulanexus Recommended Citation Kelly, Bre'on (2011) "Menopause as a Social and Cultural Construction," XULAneXUS: Vol. 8 : Iss. 2 , Article 4. Available at: https://digitalcommons.xula.edu/xulanexus/vol8/iss2/4 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by XULA Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in XULAneXUS by an authorized editor of XULA Digital Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Kelly: Menopause as a Social and Cultural Construction ! ! Volume 8, Issue 2, April 2011. Scholarly Note. 29-39. <http://xulanexus.xula.edu/textpattern/index.php?id=114> ! Menopause as a Social and Cultural Construction Bre’on Andrice Kelly, Sociology Faculty Mentor: Dr. Christopher Faircloth Abstract When the word “menopause” is mentioned in this modern age, too often a picture appears of an unstable, mature woman, fragmenting her family with her emotional instability. This essay explores this image as a social and cultural construction. Drawing initially from the medicalization model exposed in sociology, I discuss the social construction of menopause and the related medical versus feminist Bre’on Kelly is a Sociology debate. Lastly, using studies of menopause from various cultures, major with a minor in this literature review explores how women of different cultures Psychology from New Orleans, define, give meaning to, and experience menopause, as well as the LA. Upon graduating from personal, societal, and cultural implications of these.
    [Show full text]
  • Culture and Culture Change
    M02_EMBER1208_13_SE_C02.QXD 10/21/09 9:05 PM Page 14 Culture and Culture Change e all consider ourselves to be unique individuals with our own set of per- sonal opinions, preferences, habits, and quirks. Indeed, all of us are unique; and yet most of us also share many feelings, beliefs, and habits with most of the people who live in our society. If we live in North America, we are likely to have the feeling that eating dogs is wrong, have the belief that bacteria Wor viruses cause illness, and have the habit of sleeping on a bed. Most peo- ple hardly ever think about the ideas and customs they share with other people in their society, assuming them to be “natural.” These ideas and be- haviors are part of what we mean by culture. We only begin to become chapter 2 aware that our culture is different when we become aware that other peo- ples have different feelings, different beliefs, and different habits from ours. So most North Americans would never even think of the possibility of eat- CHAPTER OUTLINE ing dog meat if they did not know that people in some other societies com- Defining Culture monly do so. They would not realize that their belief in germs was cultural if they were not aware that people in some societies think that witchcraft or Cultural Constraints evil spirits causes illness. They might not become aware that it is their cus- Attitudes That Hinder tom to sleep on beds if they were not aware that people in many societies the Study of Cultures sleep on the floor or on the ground.
    [Show full text]
  • Dying the Good Death: Cultural Competence and Variance in Hospice Care
    Tapestries: Interwoven voices of local and global identities Volume 8 Issue 1 Resisting Borders: Rethinking the Limits Article 9 of American Studies 2019 Dying the Good Death: Cultural Competence and Variance in Hospice Care Lydia Koh-Krienke Macalester College, [email protected] Keywords: Death, Hospice, Cultural competence, Culturally relevant, Pluralism, Universalism Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.macalester.edu/tapestries Recommended Citation Koh-Krienke, Lydia (2019) "Dying the Good Death: Cultural Competence and Variance in Hospice Care," Tapestries: Interwoven voices of local and global identities: Vol. 8 : Iss. 1 , Article 9. Available at: https://digitalcommons.macalester.edu/tapestries/vol8/iss1/9 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the American Studies Department at DigitalCommons@Macalester College. It has been accepted for inclusion in Tapestries: Interwoven voices of local and global identities by an authorized editor of DigitalCommons@Macalester College. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Dying the Good Death: Cultural Competence and Variance in Hospice Care Lydia Koh-Krienke nun-chi. While I would still most likely be Introduction dying in my house, it may have a greater What do you want your death to significance to me as an act of “returning look like? home”. In both of these cases, I die a good I am at home, in a familiar bed with death, even though the setting and process may the nostalgic scent of simmering chicken broth differ. wafting up my nose. Four generations of my This paper will interrogate what it family surround me; the throng of means to “die well” by examining the grandchildren, too young to understand, play intersections between death and culture.
    [Show full text]