Cultural Variation in Affect Valuation

Cultural Variation in Affect Valuation

PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES Cultural Variation in Affect Valuation Jeanne L. Tsai and Brian Knutson Helene H. Fung Stanford University Chinese University of Hong Kong The authors propose that how people want to feel (“ideal affect”) differs from how they actually feel (“actual affect”) and that cultural factors influence ideal more than actual affect. In 2 studies, controlling for actual affect, the authors found that European American (EA) and Asian American (AA) individuals value high-arousal positive affect (e.g., excitement) more than do Hong Kong Chinese (CH). On the other hand, CH and AA individuals value low-arousal positive affect (e.g., calm) more than do EA individuals. For all groups, the discrepancy between ideal and actual affect correlates with depression. These findings illustrate the distinctiveness of ideal and actual affect, show that culture influences ideal affect more than actual affect, and indicate that both play a role in mental health. Keywords: culture, affect, emotion, values, temperament How does culture shape emotion? The answer has important 1994). These reports have led many scholars to conclude that implications for mental health interventions, because many psy- emotional experience is largely culturally determined (e.g., Klein- chiatric disorders feature various types of emotional distress as a man & Good, 1985; Shweder, 1994; Wierzbicka, 1994). core symptom. Ethnographic and empirical studies diverge in their Empirical findings, however, indicate that cultural differences in answers. Ethnographic accounts report significant variation in emotion may not be as profound as suggested by ethnographic emotional experience in China (Ots, 1990; Potter, 1988), India reports. For instance, Scherer and colleagues (Scherer, 1997; (Shweder & Haidt, 2002), Indonesia (Heider, 1991; Lutz, 1988), Scherer & Wallbott, 1994) found more similarities than differences the Mediterranean (Gaines & Farmer, 1986), Poland (Wierzbicka, in reported emotional responses to prototypical emotion-eliciting 1994), Tahiti (Levy, 1983), and the United States (Wierzbicka, events among people from 37 different nations. Oishi (2002) found no significant differences between Asian American (AA) and European American (EA) individuals in reports of online emo- tional experience over the course of a week. Breugelmans et al. Jeanne L. Tsai and Brian Knutson, Department of Psychology, Stanford (2005) observed that Raramuri Indians, rural Javanese, and college University; Helene H. Fung, Department of Psychology, Chinese Univer- sity of Hong Kong. students from Belgium, Indonesia, and Mexico were more similar This research was funded by National Institute of Mental Health Grant than different in the bodily sensations they associated with specific R01MH068879, National Institute on Aging Grant R03AG023302, Na- emotions. Finally, we have observed more similarities than differ- tional Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression Grant ences in online reports of emotional experience, facial expressions, UAYFY, and a Stanford Office of Technology and Licensing Grant and measures of autonomic nervous system activity (e.g., heart awarded to Jeanne L. Tsai; National Institute on Aging Grant Seed Grant rate, skin conductance) when EA and AA participants conversed AG024957-02 awarded to Brian Knutson; and Hong Kong Research with romantic partners (Tsai & Levenson, 1997), watched emo- Grants Council Earmarked Research Grant CUHK4256/03H and a Chinese tional films (Tsai, Levenson, & Carstensen, 2000), and relived University of Hong Kong Direct Grant awarded to Helene H. Fung. emotional events (Tsai, Chentsova-Dutton, Friere-Bebeau, & We thank Richard Ryan and Martin Lynch for their comments and suggestions as well as Yulia Chentsova-Dutton, Stephanie Fryberg, May- Przymus, 2002). Together, these findings suggest that cultural ling Halim, Anna Kaiser, Iris Lee, Veronica Marin, Felicity Miao, Chelsey similarities outweigh cultural differences in emotion. Morrell, Trang Pham, Julie Sandler, Emma Seppala, Alicia Sheen, Susanna Moreover, when group differences in emotional experience are Tan, Lilian Tang, Ying Wong, and Saiwing Yeung for their assistance with observed (e.g., Mesquita & Karasawa, 2002; Scollon, Diener, the project. We would also like to express our gratitude to Mike Cheung for Oishi, & Biswas-Diener, 2004), it is often unclear whether they his assistance in using structural equation modeling (SEM) to assess arise from cultural or other factors. Research has demonstrated a measurement equivalence in Study 2 and for referring us to articles related moderate to high (r ϭ .5 to .8) correlation between personality to testing nested models in SEM and to Gregory A. Miller for his consul- traits related to temperament (i.e., extraversion and neuroticism: tation regarding the conceptual and statistical assumptions of analysis of covariance. also called “affective traits”) and self-reported emotional experi- Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jeanne L. ence in people from a variety of different cultures (e.g., Costa & Tsai, Department of Psychology, Building 420, Jordan Hall, Stanford, CA McCrae, 1980; David, Green, Martin, & Suls, 1997; Emmons & 94305. E-mail: [email protected] Diener, 1985; Gomez, Cooper, & Gomez, 2000; Gross, Sutton, & Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2006, Vol. 90, No. 2, 288–307 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0022-3514/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.2.288 288 CULTURE AND AFFECT VALUATION 289 Ketelaar, 1998; McCrae, Costa, & Yik, 1996; Rusting & Larsen, empirically distinct constructs. Whereas ideal affect refers to a 1997; Schimmack, Radhakrishnan, Oishi, & Dzokoto, 2002). Be- goal, actual affect refers to a response. Whereas ideal affect cause these traits also show moderate heritability (e.g., Bouchard, requires some understanding of different affective states and their 1994; Finkel, Wille, & Matheny, 1998; Goldsmith & Campos, contingencies, actual affect does not. Finally, because most people 1986; Goldsmith & Lemery, 2000; Hettema, Neale, & Kendler, want to feel good, ideal affect should primarily involve different 2001; Krueger, 2000; Lemery & Goldsmith, 2002; Plomin & positive states, whereas actual affect should instead involve the Caspi, 1999; Tellegen, Lykken, Bouchard, & Wilcox, 1988), the- entire spectrum of affective states. In support of the prediction that orists have argued that genetic factors account for a greater pro- ideal and actual affect are distinct constructs, two previous studies portion of the variance in affective experience than do environ- have demonstrated that participants’ ratings of their own affective mental factors (Diener & Lucas, 1999; Lykken & Tellegen, 1996). experience differ significantly from their ratings of how desirable Because these traits vary across different national samples (Allik they perceive those states to be (Feldman Barrett, 1996; Rusting & & McCrae, 2004), it is possible that national differences in self- Larsen, 1995). reported affective experience result in part from temperamental Most scientists, however, do not distinguish between actual and differences. However, most cross-cultural studies of emotion fail ideal affect and, therefore, implicitly treat actual affect and ideal to measure these affective traits, and therefore, they cannot rule out affect as a single construct. In part, this conflation of actual and this possibility. Thus, the influence of cultural variables versus ideal affect may reflect genuine overlap between the two (i.e., their affective traits on emotion remains largely unexplored. shared variance). If ideal affect and actual affect are distinct entities, then this overlap may reflect the various ways in which Affect Valuation Theory actual and ideal affect interact. For example, actual and ideal affect could reinforce each other; that is, the more a person feels excited, How can the small cultural differences in emotional experience the more she or he may seek further excitement. Similarly, people reported by scientists and the large cultural differences reported by who value excitement may engage in exciting activities, which ethnographers be reconciled? To address this question, we intro- may in turn increase their actual feelings of excitement. However, duce affect valuation theory (AVT), which predicts that (a) “ideal AVT predicts that although actual and ideal affect may interact, affect” (i.e., the affective states that people value and would ideally they are also distinct. For the rest of the article, we focus on this like to feel) differs from “actual affect” (i.e., the affective states distinctiveness and refer to the nonoverlapping variance as the that people actually feel); and (b) cultural factors shape ideal affect “pure” components of ideal and actual affect and to their shared more than actual affect, whereas temperament shapes actual affect variance as the “interactive” component. A raw measure of actual more than ideal affect. Thus, AVT suggests that the discrepancy and ideal affect should include both pure and interactive compo- between ethnographic accounts and empirical research may be due nents, and therefore, unless otherwise specified, we use the terms to their different ways of measuring emotional experience. By “ideal affect” and “actual affect” when referring to both. administering measures that ask people to rate how intensely or frequently they experience various states, scientists have primarily Cultural Factors

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    20 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us