PSYC236 Semester Study Notes Introduction

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

PSYC236 Semester Study Notes Introduction PSYC236 Semester Study Notes Introduction Definitions - Sensation = when your sensory organs transform physical properties (e.g. light energy) from the environment into electrical signals which are sent to the brain - Perception = turning the sensory information into meaningful experience/information • PERCEPTION IS CONSCIOUS - Cognition = processes by which we elaborate/store/organise/recall sensory information • Involves things like memory, recall, storing, relating • Face perception = perception, Face recognition = cognition - Pre-conscious and conscious visual processing involved Senses - Our senses turn physical properties in the world into psychological information (perceptions) Visual perception - Light reflected off objects into eye onto back of retina - Rod + cone receptor cells in retina stimulated > convert light into electrical/neural energy > sent to visual cortex in brain (V1) > V2, V3… Visual perception and parallel processing - Parallel processing = the brain has separate systems that process information independently/separately before integration • Different areas specialised for specific tasks • Info about an image > each element (motion, colour, size) individually extracted by parallel/separate pathways Visual perception - Visual perception is very automatic and usually very accurate - Visual perception OVERCOMES these problems so we get complete image • We have 2 eyes but see 1 world/complete image • Retina is 2D but we correctly see the 3D world • When shake head the world doesn’t “shake” • When get partial view of object can recognise the objects is still whole • Still continuous vision when blink 1 Our perception can get things wrong – shown by optical illusions - Muller-Lyer Illusion - The Poggendorff Illusion - The Thatcher Illusion = shows facial configuration is crucial for facial recognition but only when the face is upright - Illusion of spinning dancer = “accommodation/adaption” to one direction causes it to change - Brain interprets things but can be wrong (e.g. subjective contours) Why visual illusions are important - They show us our systems aren’t always accurate - They show us perception is subjective (reality is what we perceive) - They show us we are always “guessing” based on visual cues - “Change blindness” good example that we pay attention to what we attend to • “The grand illusion” = thinking that we perceive everything around us Two important questions - What visual info is available in the environment? - What mechanisms does the brain have for utilizing this info? Ways we study visual perception - Physiological approaches = directly observe which areas of the brain are “lighting up” in response to a stimulus • PET, fMRI, lesion studies, single cell recordings, etc - Cons of physiological approaches: • A lot more is happening in brain not just responding to the selected stimulus • Lesions are coarse not fine • Restricted to studying simple visual stimuli - Psychophysical approaches = examine the relationship between the visual stimulus and perception - Examples of psychophysical approaches: • Thresholds = minimum amount of stimulus energy to detect a stimulus (e.g. dimmest light can see) • Sensitivity = 1/threshold Useful measures of visual perception - Visual acuity = clarity of image • Measure of central vision only • The minimum resolvable separation between two lines • Example of visual acuity test is an eye test 2 The Visual System Main areas of the visual system - The eye (retina) - The Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN) in the thalamus - The visual/striate cortex in the occipital lobe - The extra striate cortex (areas in the temporal/parietal/frontal lobes) Top-down and bottom-up processing - Bottom up = begin with simple sensory information first > then perception - Top down = perception driven by cognition first Vision - Light is reflected off 3d objects in the environment and create 2d image on our retina - First stages of the visual system (bottom up) break up image into small components (lines, colours, etc) 2 main visual pathways for parallel processing - The “what” pathway = responsible for shape, motion, colour, brightness, depth (VENTRAL) - The “where” pathway = responsible for perceiving location of object in space (DORSAL) Accommodation - Focusing vision by changing shape of the lens - Use accommodation to get a SINGLE CLEAR IMAGE - 70% of the focusing is done by the Cornea - the lens only does fine tuning - Closer focus = fat lens, further focus = narrow lens The retina - Where the photoreceptors are - Light onto eye > onto retina (where image falls) - Focus light to put a clear image on the back of the retina - The retina is a network of neurons which cover the back of the eye - Light must pass a lot of neurons before it reaches the receptors (rods & cones) Rod and cone receptor cells - Neurons that turn the light on the retina into electrical energy - This is achieved with light sensitive chemicals called visual pigments 3 - When these pigments absorb light they trigger a chemical reaction, which generates an electrical signal in the receptors - They have differences in: • Shape • Retinal distribution • Number • Dark adaptation • Spectral sensitivity • Spatial summation Rod/cones retinal distribution - The fovea is a small region on the centre of the retina which has the highest visual acuity - The fovea has only cones - Peripheral regions of the retina have both rods and cones - No rod or cone cells in blind spot • Blind spot = optic nerve Rod/cones total number - More rods than cones = 20:1 - 120 million rods and 6 million cones Rod/cones dark adaption - Our sensitivity (dimmest light can detect) increases when illumination decreases - Rod vision has much higher sensitivity than cone vision (referred to as the rod-cone break) • At rod cone break the cones reach max sensitivity at 10 mins in dark but rods continue to maximise sensitivity longer • Rods increase to maximise sensitivity for up to 20 minutes • Rod cone break = point where only rods continue to become more sensitive Rod/cones spectral sensitivity - Rods more sensitive to short wavelengths of light (BLUE) - Cones responsible for “colour vision” - Different colours for wavelengths: • Short = blue • Medium = yellow/green • Long = red Rod/cones spatial summation - Rods more sensitive than cones - Rods high summation - Many rods add up responses by connecting to the one ganglion cell = high sensitivity 4 Other retinal neuron (A, B, H) - Between the receptors and ganglion cells - Bipolar cells = transmit info from receptors to ganglion cells - Horizontal + amacrine cells = transmit signals from one receptor to another and from one bipolar cell to another (INHIBITORY SIGNALS) • INHIBITORY SIGNALS WHICH CAUSE LATERAL INHIBITION Ganglion cells - Ganglion cells make up the optic nerve fibre - Ganglion cells don’t respond to overall changes in light but to differences in light (DETECT EDGES) Ganglion cell receptive fields - Receptive field = part which receives signals - Ganglion cells have circular receptive fields - On and off surround centres = THEY ARE ANTAGONISTIC - If no light on receptive fields it will have a baseline firing rate - If falls on centre of receptive field firing will increase/decrease - If light falls on both the centre and surround of the ganglion cells receptive field, there will be no increase in firing rate (excitation cancelled by inhibition) Different types of ganglion cells - Parvocellular • Sustained response • Centre-surround antagonism • Most near the fovea • Important for detail/image/pattern perception - Magnocellular • Transient response • 4% of all ganglion cells • Spread across the whole retina • Important for motion perception - Koniocellular Eyes to the brain - Electrical signals leave the eye through the optic nerve and then through several structures before they reach the brain - Retina to the LGN in the thalamus • BUT 10% also goes to superior colliculis (controls eye movements) 5 Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN) - There are 2 LGNs in each hemisphere - 2 right halves of visual field = go to left hemisphere - 2 left halves of visual field = go to right hemisphere Retinotopical mapping in the LGN - The LGN is a Retinotopic map = each location in the LGN corresponds to a SAME location on the retina - Cells in the LGN have adjacent receptive fields on the retina - Parts of the LGN are specialised for motion (magno layers) and other parts are specialised for colour, form, and depth (parvo layers) Occipital lobe – visual cortex – striate cortex - Many axons go from the LGN to the occipital lobe of the cortex - Striate Cortex = visual receiving area of the occipital lobe - The LGN AND the striate cortex are organised into layers and retinotopically 3 types of feature detectors cells in the striate cortex (Hubel & Wiesel) - Simple cells: • Responds to bars of a particular orientation - Complex cells: • Respond best to movement of a correctly oriented bar across the receptive field - Hypercomplex cells: • Respond to corners, angles, or bars of a particular length moving in a particular direction Parallel pathways - All info in a visual scene extracted from parallel and independent pathways (the colour, shape, etc) then integrated together Organisation of the visual cortex - Location columns = neurons responding to the same retinal location are found in columns - Orientation columns = neurons responding to same orientation are found in columns, so that all possible orientations are included within 1mm of the cortex - Ocular dominance columns = alternating columns which respond
Recommended publications
  • A Review and Selective Analysis of 3D Display Technologies for Anatomical Education
    University of Central Florida STARS Electronic Theses and Dissertations, 2004-2019 2018 A Review and Selective Analysis of 3D Display Technologies for Anatomical Education Matthew Hackett University of Central Florida Part of the Anatomy Commons Find similar works at: https://stars.library.ucf.edu/etd University of Central Florida Libraries http://library.ucf.edu This Doctoral Dissertation (Open Access) is brought to you for free and open access by STARS. It has been accepted for inclusion in Electronic Theses and Dissertations, 2004-2019 by an authorized administrator of STARS. For more information, please contact [email protected]. STARS Citation Hackett, Matthew, "A Review and Selective Analysis of 3D Display Technologies for Anatomical Education" (2018). Electronic Theses and Dissertations, 2004-2019. 6408. https://stars.library.ucf.edu/etd/6408 A REVIEW AND SELECTIVE ANALYSIS OF 3D DISPLAY TECHNOLOGIES FOR ANATOMICAL EDUCATION by: MATTHEW G. HACKETT BSE University of Central Florida 2007, MSE University of Florida 2009, MS University of Central Florida 2012 A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Modeling and Simulation program in the College of Engineering and Computer Science at the University of Central Florida Orlando, Florida Summer Term 2018 Major Professor: Michael Proctor ©2018 Matthew Hackett ii ABSTRACT The study of anatomy is complex and difficult for students in both graduate and undergraduate education. Researchers have attempted to improve anatomical education with the inclusion of three-dimensional visualization, with the prevailing finding that 3D is beneficial to students. However, there is limited research on the relative efficacy of different 3D modalities, including monoscopic, stereoscopic, and autostereoscopic displays.
    [Show full text]
  • The Role of Motion Streaks in the Perception of the Kinetic Zollner Illusion
    Journal of Vision (2012) 12(6):19, 1–14 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/6/19 1 The role of motion streaks in the perception of the kinetic Zollner illusion The School of Optometry and Vision Science, The University of New South Wales, Sieu K. Khuu Sydney, New South Wales $ In classic geometric illusions such as the Zollner illusion, vertical lines superimposed on oriented background lines appear tilted in the direction opposite to the background. In kinetic forms of this illusion, an object moving over oriented background lines appears to follow a titled path, again in the direction opposite to the background. Existing literature does not proffer a complete explanation of the effect. Here, it is suggested that motion streaks underpin the illusion; that the effect is a consequence of interactions between detectors tuned to the orientation of background lines and those sensing the motion streaks that arise from fast object motion. This account was examined in the present study by measuring motion-tilt induction under different conditions in which the strength or salience of motion streaks was attenuated: by varying object speed (Experiment 1), contrast (Experiment 2), and trajectory/length by changing the element life-time within the stimulus (Experiment 3). It was predicted that, as motion streaks become less available, background lines would less affect the perceived direction of motion. Consistent with this prediction, the results indicated that, with a reduction in object speed below that required to generate motion streaks (, 1.128/s), Weber contrast (, 0.125) and motion streak length (two frames) reduced or extinguished the motion-tilt-induction effect.
    [Show full text]
  • Geometrical Optical Illusion Via Sub-Riemannian Geodesics in the Roto-Translation Group
    Geometrical Optical Illusion via Sub-Riemannian Geodesics in the Roto-Translation Group B. Franceschiello1, A. Mashtakov2, G. Citti3, and A. Sarti4 1 Fondation Asile des Aveugles and Laboratory for Investigative Neurophisiology, Department of Radiology, CHUV - UNIL, Lausanne 2 Program Systems Institute of RAS, Russia, CPRC, 3 Department of Mathematics, University of Bologna, Italy 4 CAMS, Center of Mathematics, CNRS - EHESS,Paris, France [email protected], [email protected], [email protected], [email protected] Abstract. We present a neuro-mathematical model for geometrical op- tical illusions (GOIs), a class of illusory phenomena that consists in a mismatch of geometrical properties of the visual stimulus and its associ- ated percept. They take place in the visual areas V1/V2 whose functional architecture have been modelled in previous works by Citti and Sarti as a Lie group equipped with a sub-Riemannian (SR) metric. Here we ex- tend their model proposing that the metric responsible for the cortical connectivity is modulated by the modelled neuro-physiological response of simple cells to the visual stimulus, hence providing a more biologically plausible model that takes into account a presence of visual stimulus. Il- lusory contours in our model are described as geodesics in the new metric. The model is confirmed by numerical simulations, where we compute the geodesics via SR-Fast Marching. 1 Introduction Geometrical-optical illusions (GOIs) have been discovered in the XIX century by German psychologists (Oppel 1854 [50], Hering, 1878,[33]) and have been defined as situations in which there is an awareness of a mismatch of geometrical prop- erties between an item in the object space and its associated percept [68].
    [Show full text]
  • Computational Role of Disinhibition in Brain Function
    COMPUTATIONAL ROLE OF DISINHIBITION IN BRAIN FUNCTION A Dissertation by YINGWEI YU Submitted to the Office of Graduate Studies of Texas A&M University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY August 2006 Major Subject: Computer Science COMPUTATIONAL ROLE OF DISINHIBITION IN BRAIN FUNCTION A Dissertation by YINGWEI YU Submitted to the Office of Graduate Studies of Texas A&M University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Approved by: Chair of Committee, Yoonsuck Choe Committee Members, Ricardo Gutierrez-Osuna Thomas Ioerger Takashi Yamauchi Head of Department, Valerie E. Taylor August 2006 Major Subject: Computer Science iii ABSTRACT Computational Role of Disinhibition in Brain Function. (August 2006) Yingwei Yu, B.E., Beihang University, China Chair of Advisory Committee: Dr. Yoonsuck Choe Neurons are connected to form functional networks in the brain. When neurons are combined in sequence, nontrivial effects arise. One example is disinhibition; that is, inhibition to another inhibitory factor. Disinhibition may be serving an important purpose because a large number of local circuits in the brain contain disinhibitory connections. However, their exact functional role is not well understood. The objective of this dissertation is to analyze the computational role of dis- inhibition in brain function, especially in visual perception and attentional control. My approach is to propose computational models of disinhibition and then map the model to the local circuits in the brain to explain psychological phenomena. Several computational models are proposed in this dissertation to account for disinhibition. (1) A static inverse difference of Gaussian filter (IDoG) is derived to account explic- itly for the spatial effects of disinhibition.
    [Show full text]
  • Symmetric Networks with Geometric Constraints As Models of Visual Illusions
    S S symmetry Article Symmetric Networks with Geometric Constraints as Models of Visual Illusions Ian Stewart 1,*,† and Martin Golubitsky 2,† 1 Mathematics Institute, University of Warwick, Coventry CV4 7AL, UK 2 Department of Mathematics, Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210, USA; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected] † These authors contributed equally to this work. Received: 17 May 2019; Accepted: 13 June 2019; Published: 16 June 2019 Abstract: Multistable illusions occur when the visual system interprets the same image in two different ways. We model illusions using dynamic systems based on Wilson networks, which detect combinations of levels of attributes of the image. In most examples presented here, the network has symmetry, which is vital to the analysis of the dynamics. We assume that the visual system has previously learned that certain combinations are geometrically consistent or inconsistent, and model this knowledge by adding suitable excitatory and inhibitory connections between attribute levels. We first discuss 4-node networks for the Necker cube and the rabbit/duck illusion. The main results analyze a more elaborate model for the Necker cube, a 16-node Wilson network whose nodes represent alternative orientations of specific segments of the image. Symmetric Hopf bifurcation is used to show that a small list of natural local geometric consistency conditions leads to alternation between two global percepts: cubes in two different orientations. The model also predicts brief transitional states in which the percept involves impossible rectangles analogous to the Penrose triangle. A tristable illusion generalizing the Necker cube is modelled in a similar manner.
    [Show full text]
  • A Review and Selective Analysis of 3D Display Technologies for Anatomical Education
    A REVIEW AND SELECTIVE ANALYSIS OF 3D DISPLAY TECHNOLOGIES FOR ANATOMICAL EDUCATION by: MATTHEW G. HACKETT BSE University of Central Florida 2007, MSE University of Florida 2009, MS University of Central Florida 2012 A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Modeling and Simulation program in the College of Engineering and Computer Science at the University of Central Florida Orlando, Florida Summer Term 2018 Major Professor: Michael Proctor ©2018 Matthew Hackett ii ABSTRACT The study of anatomy is complex and difficult for students in both graduate and undergraduate education. Researchers have attempted to improve anatomical education with the inclusion of three-dimensional visualization, with the prevailing finding that 3D is beneficial to students. However, there is limited research on the relative efficacy of different 3D modalities, including monoscopic, stereoscopic, and autostereoscopic displays. This study analyzes educational performance, confidence, cognitive load, visual-spatial ability, and technology acceptance in participants using autostereoscopic 3D visualization (holograms), monoscopic 3D visualization (3DPDFs), and a control visualization (2D printed images). Participants were randomized into three treatment groups: holograms (n=60), 3DPDFs (n=60), and printed images (n=59). Participants completed a pre-test followed by a self-study period using the treatment visualization. Immediately following the study period, participants completed the NASA TLX cognitive load instrument, a technology acceptance instrument, visual-spatial ability instruments, a confidence instrument, and a post-test. Post-test results showed the hologram treatment group (Mdn=80.0) performed significantly better than both 3DPDF (Mdn=66.7, p=.008) and printed images (Mdn=66.7, p=.007).
    [Show full text]
  • Hemifield-Specific Rotational Biases During the Observation Of
    S S symmetry Article Hemifield-Specific Rotational Biases during the Observation of Ambiguous Human Silhouettes Chiara Lucafò *,† , Daniele Marzoli *,†, Caterina Padulo , Stefano Troiano, Lucia Pelosi Zazzerini, Gianluca Malatesta , Ilaria Amodeo and Luca Tommasi Department of Psychological Sciences, Health and Territory, University of Chieti, Via dei Vestini 29, I-66013 Chieti, Italy; [email protected] (C.P.); [email protected] (S.T.); [email protected] (L.P.Z.); [email protected] (G.M.); [email protected] (I.A.); [email protected] (L.T.) * Correspondence: [email protected] (C.L.); [email protected] (D.M.) † These authors equally contributed to this work. Abstract: Both static and dynamic ambiguous stimuli representing human bodies that perform unimanual or unipedal movements are usually interpreted as right-limbed rather than left-limbed, suggesting that human observers attend to the right side of others more than the left one. Moreover, such a bias is stronger when static human silhouettes are presented in the RVF (right visual field) than in the LVF (left visual field), which might represent a particular instance of embodiment. On the other hand, hemispheric-specific rotational biases, combined with the well-known bias to perceive forward-facing figures, could represent a confounding factor when accounting for such findings. Therefore, we investigated whether the lateralized presentation of an ambiguous rotating human body would affect its perceived handedness/footedness (implying a role of motor representations), Citation: Lucafò, C.; Marzoli, D.; its perceived spinning direction (implying a role of visual representations), or both. To this aim, we Padulo, C.; Troiano, S.; required participants to indicate the perceived spinning direction (which also unveils the perceived Pelosi Zazzerini, L.; Malatesta, G.; handedness/footedness) of ambiguous stimuli depicting humans with an arm or a leg outstretched.
    [Show full text]
  • 50 Optical Illusions Free
    FREE 50 OPTICAL ILLUSIONS PDF Sam Taplin | none | 30 Oct 2009 | Usborne Publishing Ltd | 9781409507796 | English | London, United Kingdom “50 optical illusions” in Usborne Quicklinks Optical illusions are presentations of objects 50 Optical Illusions well as situations in such a 50 Optical Illusions, that it confuses your vision, and projects double meaning of the same image to you simultaneously. Optical illusions are really fun to see, as they confuse 50 Optical Illusions about what the picture really is, and play with our minds. So, for you to enjoy seeing some optical illusions and have your mind really confused, here is a list of some superb and mind-blowing optical illusions that will surely leave you confused and wondering, figuring out the real situation in the images. This article is also categorized into 50 Optical Illusions sections — Photographic Illusions, consisting of 50 Optical Illusions illusions in a taken photo of any situation, and Graphical Illusions, containing computer generated or hand-drawn illusions. Very Interesting Object. Pyramid Block. Realistic Dice Illusion. Green Pouch. Chess Illusion. Intersecting Buildings. Courtyard or Terrace? Half here, Half there. Bathroom Mirror Illusion. Which one is Cut: Wood or the Metal. Pressed In or Out? Truck Painting Illusion. Oh No! The Rug is Sinking! That Sinking Feeling. Crazy Car Optical Illusion. Are the Blue Lines horizontal? Impossible Arch. Move for You. Wooden Box Illusion. Moving Circles Illusion. Colourful Object Illusion. See beyond the Skull Illusion. Corner House Illusion. Moving Waves. Moving Bicycle Wheels. Two Faces or Two People? Staring at the Dot makes Grey Disappear. Old Man Illusion.
    [Show full text]
  • Applying Emmert's Law to the Poggendorff Illusion
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Frontiers - Publisher Connector ORIGINAL RESEARCH published: 16 October 2015 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00531 Applying Emmert’s Law to the Poggendorff illusion Umur Talasli and Asli Bahar Inan* Department of Psychology, Atilim University, Ankara, Turkey The Poggendorff illusion was approached with a novel perspective, that of applying Emmert’s Law to the situation. The extensities between the verticals and the transversals happen to be absolutely equal in retinal image size, whereas the registered distance for the verticals must be smaller than that of the transversals due to the fact that the former is assumed to occlude the latter. This combination of facts calls for the operation of Emmert’s Law, which results in the shrinkage of the occluding space between the verticals. Since the retinal image shows the transversals to be in contact with the verticals, the shrinkage must drag the transversals inwards in the cortical representation in order to eliminate the gaps. Such dragging of the transversals produces the illusory misalignment, which is a dictation of geometry. Some of the consequences of this new explanation were tested in four different experiments. In Experiment 1, a new illusion, the tilting of an occluded continuation of an oblique line, was predicted and achieved. In Experiments 2 and 3, perceived nearness of the occluding entity was Edited by: manipulated via texture density variations and the predicted misalignment variations Baingio Pinna, were confirmed by using a between-subjects and within-subjects designs, respectively. University of Sassari, Italy In Experiment 4, tilting of the occluded segment of the transversal was found to vary in Reviewed by: Branka Spehar, the predicted direction as a result of being accompanied by the same texture cues used University of New South Wales, in Experiments 2 and 3.
    [Show full text]
  • Neuropsychologia the Poggendorff Illusion Affects Manual Pointing As
    Neuropsychologia 47 (2009) 3217–3224 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Neuropsychologia journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia The Poggendorff illusion affects manual pointing as well as perceptual judgements Dean R. Melmoth, Marc S. Tibber, Simon Grant, Michael J. Morgan ∗ Henry Wellcome Laboratories, Department of Optometry and Visual Science, City University, Northampton Square, London EC1 V 0HB, United Kingdom article info abstract Article history: Pointing movements made to a target defined by the imaginary intersection of a pointer with a distant Received 28 July 2009 landing line were examined in healthy human observers in order to determine whether such motor Accepted 30 July 2009 responses are susceptible to the Poggendorff effect. In this well-known geometric illusion observers Available online 7 August 2009 make systematic extrapolation errors when the pointer abuts a second line (the inducer). The kinemat- ics of extrapolation movements, in which no explicit target was present, where similar to those made Keywords: in response to a rapid-onset (explicit) dot target. The results unambiguously demonstrate that motor Action (pointing) responses are susceptible to the illusion. In fact, raw motor biases were greater than for per- Perception Dorsal ceptual responses: in the absence of an inducer (and hence also the acute angle of the Poggendorff Ventral stimulus) perceptual responses were near-veridical, whilst motor responses retained a bias. Therefore, Pointing the full Poggendorff stimulus contained two biases: one mediated by the acute angle formed between Illusions the oblique pointer and the inducing line (the classic Poggendorff effect), which affected both motor and perceptual responses equally, and another bias, which was independent of the inducer and primarily affected motor responses.
    [Show full text]
  • The Influence of Visual Perception on the Academic Performance of the Mentally Retarded
    Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU Master's Theses Graduate College 4-1971 The Influence of Visual erP ception on the Academic Performance of the Mentally Retarded Shirley A. Knapp Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.wmich.edu/masters_theses Part of the Special Education and Teaching Commons Recommended Citation Knapp, Shirley A., "The Influence of Visual Perception on the Academic Performance of the Mentally Retarded" (1971). Master's Theses. 2874. https://scholarworks.wmich.edu/masters_theses/2874 This Masters Thesis-Open Access is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate College at ScholarWorks at WMU. It has been accepted for inclusion in Master's Theses by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks at WMU. For more information, please contact [email protected]. THE INFLUENCE OF VISUAL PERCEPTION ON THE ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE OF THE MENTALLY RETARDED by / " * ■ Shirley A. Knapp A Project Report Submitted to the Faculty of the Graduate College in partial fulfillment of the Specialist in Education Degree Western Michigan University Kalamazoo, Michigan April 1971 Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. THE INFLUENCE OF VISUAL PERCEPTION ON THE ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE OF THE MENTALLY RETARDED Shirley A. Knapp, Ed.S. Western Michigan University, 1971 Visual perception refers to the process of receiving and analyzing sensory information through the medium of the eye. The report extensively surveys literature concerning the developmental processes and characteristics of visual perception. The research tended to show a relationship between visual perceptual maturity and academic achievement. The prominent remedial programs were discussed in conjunction with supporting or disputing research concerning the alleged benefits.
    [Show full text]
  • Sub-Riemannian Problems on 3D Lie Groups with Applications to Retinal
    Sub-Riemannian Geometry in Image Processing and Modelling of Human Visual System Alexey Mashtakov Program Systems Institute of RAS Based on joint works with R. Duits, Yu. Sachkov, G. Citti, A. Sarti, A. Popov E. Bekkers, G. Sanguinetti, A. Ardentov, B. Franceschiello I. Beschastnyi, A. Ghosh and T.C.J. Dela Haije Scientific Heritage of Sergei A. Chaplygin Nonholonomic Mechanics, Vortex Structures and Hydrodynamics Cheboksary, Russia, 06.06.2019 SR geodesics on Lie Groups in Image Processing Crossing structures are Reconstruction of corrupted contours disentangled based on model of human vision Applications in medical image analysis SE(2) SO(3) SE(3) 2 Left-invariant sub-Riemannian structures 3 Optimal Control Problem: ODE-based Approach Optimality of Extremal Trajectories 5 Sub-Riemannian Wave Front 6 Sub-Riemannian Wave Front 7 Sub-Riemannian Wave Front 8 Self intersection of Sub-Riemannian Wave Front General case: astroidal shape of conjugate locus Special case with rotational symmetry A.Agrachev, Exponential mappings for contact sub-Riemannian structures. JDCS, 1996. 9 H. Chakir, J.P. Gauthier and I. Kupka, Small Subriemannian Balls on R3. JDCS, 1996. Sub-Riemannian Sphere 10 Sub-Riemannian Length Minimizers 11 Brain Inspired Methods in Computer Vision 12 12 Perception of Visual Information by Human Brain LGN (lateral geniculate nucleus) LGN V1 V1 13 Primary visual cortex A Model of the Primary Visual Cortex V1 Replicated from R. Duits, U. Boscain, F. Rossi, Y. Sachkov, 14 Association Fields via Cuspless Sub-Riemannian Geodesics in SE(2), JMIV, 2013. Cortical Based Model of Perceptual Completion 15 Detection of salient curves in images 16 16 Analysis of Images of the Retina Diabetic retinopathy --- one of the main causes of blindness.
    [Show full text]