Interactions of Prejudice with Race and Social Desirability

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Interactions of Prejudice with Race and Social Desirability University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology Psychology, Department of May 1970 Positive and negative prejudice: Interactions of prejudice with race and social desirability Richard A. Dienstbier University of Nebraska-Lincoln, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/psychfacpub Part of the Psychiatry and Psychology Commons Dienstbier, Richard A., "Positive and negative prejudice: Interactions of prejudice with race and social desirability" (1970). Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology. 121. https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/psychfacpub/121 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Psychology, Department of at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. It has been accepted for inclusion in Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. Published in Journal of Personality, 38 (1970), pp. 198-215. Copyright © 1970 Blackwell Pub- POSITIVE AND NEGATIVE PREJUDICE 199 lishing. Used by permission. pretation of specifi c studies and in a lack of closure on some of the is- sues which have received attention in the recent literature. The various Positive and negative prejudice: studies designed to investigate the belief theory of prejudice (Rokeach, Interactions of prejudice with race and Smith, & Evans, 1960), for ex ample, have employed an array of differ- 1 ent scales and behavioral measures between them as indices of preju- social desirability dice. This variety of dependent measures has caused problems in com- paring the re sults of the various studies. Triandis (1964) has indicat- Richard A. Dienstbier2, University of Rochester ed, for example, not only that there is often a great deal of difference in social distance indicated toward a stimulus individual on different While dictionary defi nitions of the term prejudice often indicate ei- measures but that across different subject populations different factors ther positive or negative bias, the use of the term in the social scienc- emerge on such scales. Triandis’s work has strongly indi cated that it is es has been largely restricted to indicating negative biases, the infor- potentially more useful to operationalize prejudice as a pattern of dis- mation available on positive racial bias, or posi tive prejudice, is mini- crimination on a variety of scales Following these suggestions from mal. Apparent examples of the phenomenon of positive racial bias are the research of Triandis, for the purpose of the studies reported in this becoming relatively common, however. For example, when a universi- paper, prejudice was viewed in terms of discrimination patterns on a ty begins actively to recruit black students whose academic credentials number of scales would not normally be ac ceptable, then a specifi c instance of positive The purpose of the studies of this paper was to investigate positive racial discrimination is occurring, possibly indicating a form of posi- prejudice, with an eye toward underlying dynamics. Positive Negro prej- tive racial prej udice. udice was defi ned (for a specifi c issue) as existing when a Negro stimu- Those studies which bear on positive prejudice were designed to lus individual received less negative discrimination on a specifi c social examine the infl uence of ethnic attitude on reasoning. Prentice (1957) distance dimension than a comparable white stimulus individual. found that subjects scoring in the lower quartile of ethno centrism and Although the fi rst of the two studies reported below was not begun fascism scales demonstrated bias in favor of certain ethnic groups in a in order to investigate positive prejudice, the results were particularly syllogistic reasoning task by accepting syllo gisms favorable to certain interesting insofar as they pertained to positive prej udice. Only those ethnic groups more than comparable but racially neutral syllogisms. aspects of the results which directly relate to positive prejudice are em- In a similar study, Schuman and Harding (1964) found a tendency for phasized in this paper. The second study reported herein was designed highly antiprejudiced sub jects to accept a larger number of irrational to investigate positive prejudice directly, and those results are present- but positive state ments than of irrational but negative statements (when ed in total. each type was paired with a rational statement of opposite sign), lead- Since the hypotheses of the second study were partially based on the ing the authors to conclude that there were individuals who demon- results of the fi rst study, a preview of the results of that fi rst study, as strated “love prejudice.” they pertain to the second, is necessary. It was noted that the factorial The problem of various defi nitions of prejudice goes beyond the design of race by personality valence of Study I allowed the direct com- simple issue of positive or negative bias, the great variability of def- parison of social distance type ratings of the same stimulus personality inition at the operational level has resulted in problems in the inter- with different racial labels, and that for the socially desirable stimulus personality, ap preciable and signifi cant positive prejudice toward Ne- 1 Special thanks are due Vincent Nowlis and Edward E. Ware for advice and en- groes ex isted on several scales. The second study was performed with couragement in the planning and writing of these studies. 2 a group of subjects who were known to be relatively liberal in their so- Presently at the University of Nebraska. cial-political viewpoint. For the second study, it was hypothesized that 198 200 R. DIENSTBIER IN JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY 38 (1970) POSITIVE AND NEGATIVE PREJUDICE 201 individuals demonstrating positive prejudice, as defi ned above, would analysis of variance design was thus effected, with two levels each of be on the highly liberal end of attitude scales pertaining to war, crime, order, race, and social desirability, and with each subject seeing two of and rioting and that such individ uals would be low in dogmatism and the four personality-race combinations. The effects of race, personali- rigidity as measured by standard personality tests of those variables. ty, and order, and the triple interactions were therefore within-subject Those hypotheses were derived from the extensive literature on author- differences in the analyses of variance of the scales; the three two-way itarianism starting with Adorno, Frenkel-Brunswik, Levinson, and San- interactions were all between-subject effects. ford (1950). That research has indicated that conservative or puni tive The personality profi les. The personality profi les were introduced social attitudes and tendencies to be rigid and dogmatic are often asso- by instructions which indicated that the evaluations were part of a ciated with negative racial prejudice. The hypotheses of Study II were study in interpersonal liking and that the persons who were to be eval- based on the assumption that the personality correlates which are nor- uated were real people. Emphasis was placed on the desirability of mally associated with negative racial prejudice will be negatively relat- answers refl ecting real honesty, subjects were assured that no identi- ed to positive prejudice. That hypothesis is also related to hypotheses fi cation with their own answers would be required, and none were. suggested by Keniston (1967) that student liberalism, related to person- The profi les were each approximately 200 words in length. That of ality character istics of fl exibility and open-mindedness, is characterized the individual with positive values (John) depicted a likable and so- by identifi cation with the oppressed. cially successful high school junior who was college bound upon his graduation, and whose ideas were “a lot like those of most people his STUDY I age.” The sketch of the negative individual (Jack) described a 17- year- Subjects old high school dropout who was a somewhat greedy, shy, and rebel- Subjects were 80 male juniors from a Jesuit high school in a sub- lious social misfi t whose ideas were generally “too far out for most of urb of Rochester, New York; 78 of the subjects were white, and 2 were the other people his age.” Negro. The scaling of the parents’ occupations on the Warner, Meeker, Race was indicated by the profi le title and by the phrase “is a Negro and Eels (1949) index indicated that generally the subjects were from who” inserted in the fi rst line of the profi le when it pertained to a Ne- the middle range of the middle class. gro stimulus person. Race was also indicated on each of the pages of The subjects were not volunteers, three entire classes which would scales pertaining to a given profi le by a title line identical to that used normally have received regular instruction during that time were used. with the personality sketch (e.g., “Jack B , Age 17, Negro, High School Dropout”). Each sketch, with its name, race, age, and status designa- Procedure tion, appeared on an individual page. Design. Each subject read and evaluated two personality profi les, Prejudice measures. The prejudice measures followed (on subse- one of which was Negro, one white. One of the profi les pertained to quent test booklet pages) the profi le to which they pertained. The mea- a stimulus person who had beliefs and values of high social desirabil- sures of prejudice or social distance toward the stimulus persons were ity (the John profi le), while the other profi le described a stimulus per- scales taken from or related to those used in the belief theory of prej- son with characteristics generally low in social desirability (the Jack udice studies of the 1960’s. The purpose of this rather wide range of pro fi le). Subjects who were presented the desirable profi le as that of a scales was simply to allow comparisons between the data from a num- white person evaluated the undesirable profi le as that of a Negro, the ber of studies pertaining to the belief theory of prejudice.
Recommended publications
  • Gender Bias in Sexual Assault Response And
    End Violence Against Women International (EVAWI) Gender Bias in Sexual Assault Response and Investigation Part 1: Implicit Gender Bias Heather Huhtanen Contributions by Kimberly A. Lonsway, PhD Sergeant Joanne Archambault (Ret.) November 2017 Updated October 2020 . This project is supported by Grant No. 2016-TA-AX-K010 awarded by the Office on Violence Against Women, US Department of Justice. The opinions, findings, conclusions, and recommendations expressed in this publication are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the views of the Department of Justice, Office on Violence Against Women. Gender Bias in Sexual Assault Response and Investigation October Part 1: Implicit Gender Bias Huhtanen 2020 Public Domain Notice Unless something is excerpted directly from a copyrighted source, all the material in this document is in the public domain and may be reproduced or copied without specifically requesting permission from End Violence Against Women International (EVAWI) or the authors. Any direct quotes or excerpts should be properly cited, however. No one may reproduce or distribute this material for a fee without the specific, written authorization of End Violence Against Women International (EVAWI). Electronic Access The publication may be downloaded from End Violence Against Women International’s Resource Library. Recommended Citation Huhtanen, H. (2020). Gender Bias in Sexual Assault Response and Investigation. Part 1: Implicit Gender Bias. End Violence Against Women International. End Violence Against Women International 2 www.evawintl.org Gender Bias in Sexual Assault Response and Investigation October Part 1: Implicit Gender Bias Huhtanen 2020 Authors Heather Huhtanen is currently based in Geneva, Switzerland where she works to promote gender equality in the context of international development, security and justice reform and peace and transition processes.
    [Show full text]
  • •Understanding Bias: a Resource Guide
    Community Relations Services Toolkit for Policing Understanding Bias: A Resource Guide Bias Policing Overview and Resource Guide The Community Relations Service (CRS) is the U.S. Justice Department’s “peacemaker” agency, whose mission is to help resolve tensions in communities across the nation, arising from differences of race, color, national origin, gender, gender identity, sexual orientation, religion, and disability. CRS may be called to help a city or town resolve tensions that stem from community perceptions of bias or a lack of cultural competency among police officers. Bias and a lack of cultural competency are often cited interchangeably as challenges in police- community relationships. While bias and a lack of cultural competency may both be present in a given situation, these challenges and the strategies for addressing them differ appreciably. This resource guide will assist readers in understanding and addressing both of these issues. What is bias, and how is it different from cultural competency? The Science of Bias Bias is a human trait resulting from our tendency and need to classify individuals into categories as we strive to quickly process information and make sense of the world.1 To a large extent, these processes occur below the level of consciousness. This “unconscious” classification of people occurs through schemas, or “mental maps,” developed from life experiences to aid in “automatic processing.”2 Automatic processing occurs with tasks that are very well practiced; very few mental resources and little conscious thought are involved during automatic processing, allowing numerous tasks to be carried out simultaneously.3 These schemas become templates that we use when we are faced with 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Sexism at Work: How Can We Stop It? Handbook for the EU Institutions and Agencies Acknowledgements
    Sexism at work: how can we stop it? Handbook for the EU institutions and agencies Acknowledgements This handbook was developed by the European The personal stories were collected in person Institute for Gender Equality (EIGE). It was or in writing during the drafting of the hand- adapted from JUMP’s handbook Libérez votre book. They include experiences from the EU entreprise du sexisme* by Dorothy Dalton, who institutions and agencies, as well as from bod- was contracted as an External Expert. EIGE co- ies that work closely with these organisations. ordinated the work and provided quality assur- Stories were collected by the External Expert ance (Veronica Collins, Valentina Canepa, Vasiliki during workshops and coaching sessions, as Saini). well as through conversations. All stories are On 20 September 2019, EIGE held an expert real, though some names have been changed meeting in Vilnius, Lithuania, to receive com- to protect identities. EIGE would like to thank ments on a draft of the handbook. The meeting the Directorate-General (DG) for Justice and was attended by EU institutions and agencies, Consumers and DG Human Resources and non-governmental organisations and the pri- Security of the European Commission for their vate sector. The final version of this handbook valuable input, as well as Dr Sonja Robnik, mem- reflects the input of these organisations. ber of EIGE’s Expert Forum. The European Institute for Gender Equality The European Institute for Gender Equality Email: [email protected] (EIGE) is an autonomous body of the European Union established to strengthen gender Tel. +370 52157444 equality across the EU.
    [Show full text]
  • Bias, Employment Discrimination, and Black Women's Hair: Another Way Forward
    BYU Law Review Volume 2018 Issue 4 Article 7 Winter 2-28-2019 Bias, Employment Discrimination, and Black Women's Hair: Another Way Forward Crystal Powell Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.law.byu.edu/lawreview Part of the Civil Rights and Discrimination Commons, and the Labor and Employment Law Commons Recommended Citation Crystal Powell, Bias, Employment Discrimination, and Black Women's Hair: Another Way Forward, 2018 BYU L. Rev. 933 (2019). Available at: https://digitalcommons.law.byu.edu/lawreview/vol2018/iss4/7 This Comment is brought to you for free and open access by the Brigham Young University Law Review at BYU Law Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in BYU Law Review by an authorized editor of BYU Law Digital Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. 004.POWELL_FIN2_NOHEADERS.DOCX (DO DELETE) 2/17/19 8:33 PM Bias, Employment Discrimination, and Black Women’s Hair: Another Way Forward CONTENTS I. INTRODUCTION .......................................................................................... 933 II. HISTORY OF BLACK HAIR, IMPLICIT BIAS, AND WORKPLACE GROOMING STANDARDS ..................................................................... 937 A. History of Black Hair Texture and Hairstyle: Centuries of Stereotyping ...................................................................................... 938 B. Clean, Neat, and Kept Versus Extreme, Eye-Catching, and Unprofessional: Workplace Grooming Policies Reflect Racial Stereotypes ............................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Implicit Racial Bias
    December 2018 Implicit Racial Bias Bailey Maryfield, M.S. Introduction sciously reject racism or other bias may unwittingly act in ways that result in discrimination because of implicit Implicit racial bias and its potential effects on criminal bias” (Police Executive Research Forum, 2016). justice decision making have become highly visible issues in recent years. Driven by a myriad of factors, The following gives a brief overview of implicit racial including the well-documented disparity in criminal bias as it relates to the various stages of the criminal justice involvement across racial groups and several justice system. Various mechanisms used to address high-profile events that have exposed rifts in the re- implicit racial bias are also briefly discussed along with lationships between local police and the communities what is known about their effectiveness. they serve, both discourse and research on the topic of implicit bias arguably has become more prominent What is Implicit Racial Bias than ever before. This fact sheet is intended to raise awareness about the concept of implicit racial bias, the It is important to distinguish implicit racial bias effect of implicit racial bias in the criminal justice sys- from racism or discrimination. Implicit biases are tem, and how the issue is being addressed to enhance associations made by individuals in the uncon- the fair and equitable administration of justice. scious state of mind. This means that the individ- ual is likely not aware of the biased association. Although implicit bias can manifest itself in many different forms, such as gender, racial or religious bias, Implicit racial bias can cause individuals to un- this fact sheet is focused specifically on implicit racial knowingly act in discriminatory ways.
    [Show full text]
  • Unconscious Bias
    Conscious and Unconscious Gender Bias Response to DACOWITS RFI 4 Prepared for the December 2018 DACOWITS Quarterly Business Meeting Authors Rachel Gaddes Zoe Jacobson Sidra Montgomery Courtney Moore Submitted to Submitted by DACOWITS Insight Policy Research, Inc. 4800 Mark Center Drive 1901 North Moore Street Suite 04J25-01 Suite 1100 Alexandria, VA 22350-9000 Arlington, VA 22209 Project Officer Project Director DACOWITS Executive Staff Rachel Gaddes Contents Introduction .................................................................................................................................................. 1 Chapter 1. Definitions of Bias ....................................................................................................................... 2 A. Defining Bias and Gender Bias ......................................................................................................... 3 B. Defining Conscious and Unconscious Bias ....................................................................................... 3 C. Conscious and Unconscious Gender Bias in the Workplace ............................................................ 4 D. Definitions of Gender-Neutral and Gender-Inclusive Wording ....................................................... 5 Chapter 2. Recent Media Coverage of Gender Bias ..................................................................................... 6 A. Recent News Articles ......................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Dehumanization: Understanding the Paradox of Human Interaction Debra Keenahan University of Wollongong
    University of Wollongong Research Online University of Wollongong Thesis Collection University of Wollongong Thesis Collections 1990 Dehumanization: understanding the paradox of human interaction Debra Keenahan University of Wollongong Recommended Citation Keenahan, Debra, Dehumanization: understanding the paradox of human interaction, Doctor of Philosophy thesis, Department of Psychology, University of Wollongong, 1990. http://ro.uow.edu.au/theses/1656 Research Online is the open access institutional repository for the University of Wollongong. For further information contact the UOW Library: [email protected] DEHUMANIZATION UNDERSTANDING THE PARADOX OF HUMAN INTERACTION A thesis submitted in fulfillment of the requirements for the award of the degree Doctor of Philosophy from the University of Wollongong by Debra Keenahan, BA (Hons) Department of Psychology 1990 University of Wollongong Candidate's Certificate i certify that the thesis entitled Dehumanization: Understanding the Paradox of Human Interaction, and submitted for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy, is the result of my own research, except where otherwise acknowledged, and that this thesis (or any part of the same) has not been submitted for a higher degree to any other university or institution. Signed : Date : ./2:..<3.:..?9, TABLE OF CONTENTS Acknowledgements v i i i Dedication x Abstract x i INTRODUCTION 1 PART 1 : THE DYNAMICS OF DEHUMANIZATION CHAPTER ONE: DEFINING DEHUMANIZATION 8 What Does It Mean To Be A Human Being? 9 Limitations of Human Agency 1 0 People
    [Show full text]
  • Not Yet Human: Implicit Knowledge, Historical Dehumanization, and Contemporary Consequences
    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 2008, Vol. 94, No. 2, 292–306 0022-3514/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.94.2.292 Not Yet Human: Implicit Knowledge, Historical Dehumanization, and Contemporary Consequences Phillip Atiba Goff Jennifer L. Eberhardt The Pennsylvania State University Stanford University Melissa J. Williams Matthew Christian Jackson University of California, Berkeley The Pennsylvania State University Historical representations explicitly depicting Blacks as apelike have largely disappeared in the United States, yet a mental association between Blacks and apes remains. Here, the authors demonstrate that U.S. broadly. citizens implicitly associate Blacks and apes. In a series of laboratory studies, the authors reveal how this association influences study participants’ basic cognitive processes and significantly alters their judg- publishers. ments in criminal justice contexts. Specifically, this Black–ape association alters visual perception and attention, and it increases endorsement of violence against Black suspects. In an archival study of actual allied disseminated criminal cases, the authors show that news articles written about Blacks who are convicted of capital its be crimes are more likely to contain ape-relevant language than news articles written about White convicts. of to Moreover, those who are implicitly portrayed as more apelike in these articles are more likely to be one not executed by the state than those who are not. The authors argue that examining the subtle persistence of or is specific historical representations such as these may not only enhance contemporary research on dehumanization, stereotyping, and implicit processes but also highlight common forms of discrimination and that previously have gone unrecognized.
    [Show full text]
  • Hate & Bias-Motivated Crimes
    Hate & bias-motivated crimes Sexual violence & individuals who identify as lgbtq © National Sexual Violence Resource Center and Pennsylvania Coalition Against Rape 2012. All rights reserved. This document was supported by Cooperative Agreement # 5VF1CE001751-03 from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). Its contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of the CDC. This project is supported by Grant No. 2010-SW-AX-0019 awarded by the Office on Violence Against Women, U.S. Department of Justice. The opinions, findings, conclusions, and recommendations expressed in this publication are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the views of the Department of Justice, Office on Violence Against Women. The content of this publication may be reprinted with the following acknowledgement: This material was reprinted, with permission, from the National Sexual Violence Resource Center’s publication entitled Hate & bias-motivated crimes: Sexual violence & individuals who identify as LGBTQ. This guide is available by visiting www.nsvrc.org or use your smartphone to scan the QR Code (at left) for more information online. Hate & bias-motivated crimes Sexual violence & individuals who identify as lgbtq eople often commit violence against individuals they perceive to be challenging traditional sexual P and gender norms. In a study of 162 men who identified as gay, and 111 women who identified as lesbian, 52% of the participants reported at least one incident of sexual assault/coercion (Waldner-Haugrud & Gratch, 1997). In this same study, men who identified as gay reported 1.6 incidents per person on average and women who identify as lesbians reported 1.2 incidents per person.
    [Show full text]
  • Examination of the Role of Dehumanization As a Potential Mechanism Underlying the Racial Disparities in School Disciplinary Measures
    Virginia Commonwealth University VCU Scholars Compass Theses and Dissertations Graduate School 2018 Examination of the Role of Dehumanization as a Potential Mechanism Underlying the Racial Disparities in School Disciplinary Measures. Ebony A. Lambert Virginia Commonwealth University Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarscompass.vcu.edu/etd Part of the Other Education Commons, and the Social Psychology Commons © The Author Downloaded from https://scholarscompass.vcu.edu/etd/5656 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at VCU Scholars Compass. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of VCU Scholars Compass. For more information, please contact [email protected]. EXAMINATION OF THE ROLE OF DEHUMANIZATION AS A POTENTIAL MECHANISM UNDERLYING THE RACIAL DISPARITIES IN SCHOOL DISCIPLINARY MEASURES A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science at Virginia Commonwealth University By: Ebony A. Lambert B.A., College of William & Mary, May 2016 Director: Nao Hagiwara, Ph.D. Associate Professor of Psychology Department of Psychology Virginia Commonwealth University Richmond, Virginia November 2018 © Ebony A. Lambert 2018 All Rights Reserved Acknowledgments Thank you to my committee members, Drs. Zewelanji Serpell and Jacqueline McDonnough, for their insight and expertise. I would also like to extend a special note of gratitude to my advisor, Dr. Nao Hagiwara, for her support and guidance. I am grateful also to Kayla Sanon, Randl Dent, and Zoe Smith for their grace and encouragement during the course of my graduate career. My deepest gratitude to my parents, Kathy and Clifford Lambert Sr., for their unconditional support and care throughout my life and academic career.
    [Show full text]
  • Implicit Bias and Prejudice in Mediation
    SMU Law Review Volume 70 Issue 3 ADR Symposium, Part 1 of 2 Article 5 2017 Implicit Bias and Prejudice in Mediation Carol Izumi University of California Hastings College of Law, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://scholar.smu.edu/smulr Part of the Dispute Resolution and Arbitration Commons Recommended Citation Carol Izumi, Implicit Bias and Prejudice in Mediation, 70 SMU L. REV. 681 (2017) https://scholar.smu.edu/smulr/vol70/iss3/5 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Law Journals at SMU Scholar. It has been accepted for inclusion in SMU Law Review by an authorized administrator of SMU Scholar. For more information, please visit http://digitalrepository.smu.edu. IMPLICIT BIAS AND PREJUDICE IN MEDIATION Carol Izumi* ABSTRACT Mediators aspire and endeavor to meet their ethical duty of “neutrality” in mediation. Yet their ability to actually conduct mediations without bias, prejudice, or favoritism toward any party is extraordinarily difficult, if not impossible. Research shows that unconscious mental processes involving stereotypes and attitudes affect our judgments, perceptions, and behavior toward others. Implicit bias, the automatic association of stereotypes and attitudes with social groups, may produce discriminatory responses toward parties despite a mediator’s best efforts at creating an outwardly even- handed process. Even the most well-intentioned and egalitarian mediators must actively engage in bias reduction strategies to mitigate prejudice in mediation. I. INTRODUCTION ........................................ 681 II. MEDIATOR NEUTRALITY ............................. 683 III. IMPLICIT BIAS IN MEDIATION ....................... 685 IV. BIAS REDUCTION STRATEGIES ...................... 690 I. INTRODUCTION1 IRST, I want to thank Michael Green, Richard Delgado, the sym- posium organizers, the SMU Dedman School of Law community, Fand other panelists.
    [Show full text]
  • Unconscious Bias
    Unconscious bias Adapted by Professor Uta Frith DBE FBA FMedSci FRS from guidance issued to recruitment panels by the Scottish Government Introduction All panels and committees for selection and appointments The Royal Society provides research funding to at The Royal Society should be carried out objectively individuals to support high quality scientific research, and professionally. with the expectation that these individuals are able to reach their full potential. The Royal Society therefore The Society is committed to making funding or award expects all organisations hosting Royal Society Research decisions purely on the basis of the quality of the Fellows and Research Professorships to provide supportive proposed science and merit of the individual. No funding workplace structures to ensure equality and diversity applicant or nominee for awards, Fellowship, Foreign within the scientific workforce. Evidence of a commitment Membership, election to a post or appointment to a to improving the culture in this area, such as Athena Swan committee should receive less favourable treatment on accreditation, may be taken into account in making awards. the grounds of: gender, marital status, sexual orientation, gender re-assignment, race, colour, nationality, ethnicity This short briefing is meant to alert you to potential or national origins, religion or similar philosophical belief, difficulties around unconscious bias and prompt you spent criminal conviction, age or disability. to consciously revisit them before making a decision. Think of them as the safety instructions that you are Equally, all proposals or nominations must be assessed given every time you are on an airplane. You may think on equal terms, regardless of the sex, age and/or ethnicity you know them already, but it is good to rehearse them of the applicant.
    [Show full text]