SHORT COMMUNICATION Brain Size, Body Size and Longevity

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

SHORT COMMUNICATION Brain Size, Body Size and Longevity International Journal of Obesity (2010) 34, 1349–1352 & 2010 Macmillan Publishers Limited All rights reserved 0307-0565/10 www.nature.com/ijo SHORT COMMUNICATION Brain size, body size and longevity A Peters1, B Hitze1, D Langemann2, A Bosy-Westphal3 and MJ Mu¨ller3 1Medical Clinic 1, University of Luebeck, Luebeck, Germany; 2Institute of Computational Mathematics, Technical University Braunschweig, Braunschweig, Germany and 3Institute of Human Nutrition and Food Science, Christian-Albrechts-University Kiel, Kiel, Germany In this analysis, we bring together two research fields that have never been associated before: the clinical issue ‘Que´telet’s body-mass index and longevity’ and the comparative biological issue ‘body–brain allometry’. Comparison of medical and biological data supports the view that body mass index is just a one-to-one mapping of the body–brain–energy balanceFa biological variable indicating that an individual maintains its systemic energy homeostasis and therefore is likely to perform well in the coming years. International Journal of Obesity (2010) 34, 1349–1352; doi:10.1038/ijo.2010.65; published online 30 March 2010 Keywords: brain size; body size; body mass index; longevity One currently prevailing myth is that a large cerebral As body metabolism increases, brain metabolism increases capacity should guarantee a long life. Here we suggest that in a so-called ‘isometric’ manner, that is, it follows a ‘linear’ there might indeed be a hidden link between brain size function. ‘Isometric’ functions number among a larger class and longevity. of so-called ‘allometric’ functions that are often used in Focussing on humans alone, clinicians ascertained that comparative biology, and all of which follow the power body mass is related to life span.1–3 As long ago as 1835, the function Y ¼ kXa. Such allometric functions are used, for mathematician Adolphe Que´telet analyzed human data sets example, to describe the nonlinear relationship between with the intention of finding a statistical variable that would brain mass and body mass, or the relationship between body allow him to identify ‘healthy’ humans whom he considered metabolism and body mass among different species where to be of ‘normal’ stature.4 He achieved a little success the allometric exponents have been reported to be around in this task in finding that a man or woman displaying a 0.7.9,10 Allometric functions have also been reported for the mass/height2 ratio of B24 kg mÀ2 matched his image of nonlinear relationship between brain mass and life span.11,12 being ‘normal’. Later on, insurance companies used his Remarkably, the relationship between brain energy metabo- concept to calculate the health risks for humans. Que´telet’s lism (Y) and body energy metabolism (X) is particularly ratio is now referred to as the ‘body mass index’ (BMI). simple, as the exponent a has been found to be very close to Recent studies confirm that a BMI of between 22.5 and 1.0.8 As already mentioned, such special linear relationships 25.0 kg mÀ2 is a strong predictor for maximal longevity are referred to as ‘isometric’ by biologists. In vertebrates (Figure 1c).1,2 It is known that in mice, less insulin-like ranging in size and metabolism from goldfishes to elephants, signalling throughout the body or just in the brain consi- brains become proportionally larger and hence utilize a derably extends the life span.5,6 It is unknown, however, larger amount of energy when body metabolism increases. whether brain energy metabolism specifically can predict In the research field of neuroenergetics, there is interest in longevity. knowing how the control system (brain) and the executor Taking the broader perspective of comparative biology, system (periphery) of a given vertebrate species compete vertebrate brain metabolism has been found to be strictly for the available energy resources within the organism.13 The proportional to body energy metabolism (Figure 1b).7,8 brains of Homo sapiens not only use a larger proportion of the organism’s energy supply (20%) than do those of rats, cats and dogs (4–6%), but also are the largest compared with their Correspondence: Dr A Peters, Clinical Research Group SELFISH BRAIN, Brain body size, with humans exhibiting a relatively gracile metabolism, Neuroenergetics, Obesity and Diabetes, Medical Clinic I, physique.7,8,14 We therefore consider the ‘body–brain– University of Luebeck, Ratzeburger Allee 160, Luebeck 23538, Germany. energy balance’ (X/Y) to be a variable indicating the E-mail: [email protected] Received 17 December 2009; revised 10 February 2010; accepted 19 February proportion of energy used to cover the individual cerebral 2010; published online 30 March 2010 need in an individual subject. In this body–brain–energy Body–brain–energy balance A Peters et al 1350 8 80 Y= 1/5 ·X 7 70 6 in] (Y) 60 X/Y =5 Y= 0. 20 X0.99 / m 5 2 50 4 40 3 30 Y= 0.07 X0.99 body metabolism (X) / metabolism body (X) brain metabolism (Y) 2 20 -- vertebrates (Mink, 1981) — humans (Mink, 1981) 1 10 • normal-weight subjects; brain metabolism [cm³ O this MRI study (n=89) 0 0 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 0 200 400 600 800 1000 BMI [kg/m²] body metabolism [cm³ O2 / min] (X) 1700 1600 1500 BMI = 24 kg/m² 1400 1300 35 30 1200 25 brain mass [g] 1100 20 15 1000 10 900 5 0 800 yearlydeath per 1000 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 BMI [kg/m²] BMI [kg/m²] Figure 1 (a) Scatter plot of the body–brain–energy balance (X/Y) and body mass index (BMI) in our human magnetic-resonance imaging/calorimetric study (n ¼ 208).17 At a BMI of 24 kg/m2 we measured a body–brain–energy balance of 5.0 (black bold graph). (b) Allometric brain-to-body functions in vertebrates (dashed gray graph) and humans (solid gray graph) according to Mink.8 Note: the dashed gray graph actually covers a larger scope of values than depicted here, 3 À1 that is, bodies ranging in size from that of a goldfish to an elephant (0.04–6000.00 cm O2 min ). In (a), we considered the function X/Y ¼ 5 (black bold), which applies for humans exhibiting a BMI of 24 kg mÀ2, and integrated it (black bold graph) into (b), the large-scaled body–brain plot. The human-specific function (black bold) assessed here is a good approximation to the function (solid gray) based on the general power law. (c) Annual death rate related to body-mass index in females (solid line) and males (dashed line) according to Whitlock et al.1 Optimal survival is observed in humans with a BMI of B24 kg mÀ2.(d) Plot of brain mass and BMI in our human magnetic-resonance imaging/indirect calorimetry study (n ¼ 208). Brain mass is found to be independent from the BMI (women: r ¼ 0.131, NS; men: r ¼ 0.079, NS), as is consistent with the observation that the brain mass is maintained even when body mass changes dramatically.15 balance, the denominator, ‘brain metabolism’ (Y), ade- already well established over a wider range of body sizes quately fulfils the criteria for reference variables, as cerebral ranging in size from the goldfish to the elephant? morphological and energetic changes are known to be very In order to address the first question, we used magnetic- small even under conditions of long-term or short-term resonance imaging to measure brain size and indirect 15,16 3 À1 energy depletion. Every individual subject appears to calorimetry to measure body metabolism (cm O2 min )as strive for an optimal allocation of energy resources between described earlier.17 In all, 104 women and 104 men of widely the brain and the periphery in order to realize its best varying weight (underweight to obesity) participated in the adaptive capacity. Considering this background, the follow- study. We took the product of the subject’s individual brain 3 À1 À1 ing two questions are posed here: (1) is the relationship size and 0.0349 cm O2 min gbrain (that is, cerebral oxygen between body and brain energy metabolism somehow linked consumption according to Sokoloff18) in order to estimate to longevity in humans?, and (2) if so, can we integrate such their brain energy metabolism. We found that the body– a finding into the laws of body–brain allometry that are brain–energy balances (X/Y) were correlated with the International Journal of Obesity Body–brain–energy balance A Peters et al 1351 BMIs (r ¼ 0.553; Po0.001) (see Figure 1a). The regression able to find a simple ‘statistically derived’ variable, that is, equation read as follows: the BMI, which allowed him to identify humans who were X expecting maximal longevity. However, although Que´telet’s ¼ 0:085 Á BMI þ 2:961 ð1Þ Y variable is predictive, it is also only a descriptive measure. Does it in fact make any biological sense? Here, we made If a BMI of 24 kg mÀ2 is inserted in Equation (1), a body– an attempt to replace the descriptive ratio BMI (mass (kg) brain–energy balance (X/Y) of 5.0 is obtained. As such, the divided by area (m2)) with a variable representing a relevant optimal survival of humans with a BMI of 24 kg mÀ2 as biological feature. reported by Whitlock et al.1 is associated with a body–brain– In conclusion, we brought two research fields together energy balance of 5, or put otherwise, with a relative that have never been associated before, that is, the clinical brain energy consumption of 20%.
Recommended publications
  • The Neuroscience of Human Intelligence Differences
    Edinburgh Research Explorer The neuroscience of human intelligence differences Citation for published version: Deary, IJ, Penke, L & Johnson, W 2010, 'The neuroscience of human intelligence differences', Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 11, pp. 201-211. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2793 Digital Object Identifier (DOI): 10.1038/nrn2793 Link: Link to publication record in Edinburgh Research Explorer Document Version: Peer reviewed version Published In: Nature Reviews Neuroscience Publisher Rights Statement: This is an author's accepted manuscript of the following article: Deary, I. J., Penke, L. & Johnson, W. (2010), "The neuroscience of human intelligence differences", in Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11, p. 201-211. The final publication is available at http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2793 General rights Copyright for the publications made accessible via the Edinburgh Research Explorer is retained by the author(s) and / or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing these publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. Take down policy The University of Edinburgh has made every reasonable effort to ensure that Edinburgh Research Explorer content complies with UK legislation. If you believe that the public display of this file breaches copyright please contact [email protected] providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Download date: 02. Oct. 2021 Nature Reviews Neuroscience in press The neuroscience of human intelligence differences Ian J. Deary*, Lars Penke* and Wendy Johnson* *Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh EH4 2EE, Scotland, UK. All authors contributed equally to the work.
    [Show full text]
  • Seasonality and Brain Size Are Negatively Associated in Frogs
    www.nature.com/scientificreports OPEN Seasonality and brain size are negatively associated in frogs: evidence for the expensive brain Received: 7 July 2017 Accepted: 20 November 2017 framework Published: xx xx xxxx Yi Luo1,2, Mao Jun Zhong1,2, Yan Huang1,2, Feng Li1,2, Wen Bo Liao1,2 & Alexander Kotrschal3 The challenges of seasonal environments are thought to contribute to brain evolution, but in which way is debated. According to the Cognitive Bufer Hypothesis (CBH) brain size should increase with seasonality, as the cognitive benefts of a larger brain should help overcoming periods of food scarcity via, for instance, increased behavioral fexibility. However, in line with the Expensive Brain Framework (EBF) brain size should decrease with seasonality because a smaller brain confers energetic benefts in periods of food scarcity. Empirical evidence is inconclusive and mostly limited to homoeothermic animals. Here we used phylogenetic comparative analyses to test the impact of seasonality on brain evolution across 30 species of anurans (frogs) experiencing a wide range of temperature and precipitation. Our results support the EBF because relative brain size and the size of the optic tectum were negatively correlated with variability in temperature. In contrast, we found no association between the variability in precipitation and the length of the dry season with either brain size or the sizes of other major brain regions. We suggest that seasonality-induced food scarcity resulting from higher variability in temperature constrains brain size evolution in anurans. Less seasonal environments may therefore facilitate the evolution of larger brains in poikilothermic animals. Brain size varies dramatically between species1 and the recent decades have uncovered a variety of factors that drive this evolution2–10.
    [Show full text]
  • A Review of Effects of Environment on Brain Size in Insects
    insects Review A Review of Effects of Environment on Brain Size in Insects Thomas Carle Faculty of Biology, Kyushu University, Fukuoka 819-0395, Japan; [email protected] Simple Summary: What makes a big brain is fascinating since it is considered as a measure of intelligence. Above all, brain size is associated with body size. If species that have evolved with complex social behaviours possess relatively bigger brains than those deprived of such behaviours, this does not constitute the only factor affecting brain size. Other factors such as individual experience or surrounding environment also play roles in the size of the brain. In this review, I summarize the recent findings about the effects of environment on brain size in insects. I also discuss evidence about how the environment has an impact on sensory systems and influences brain size. Abstract: Brain size fascinates society as well as researchers since it is a measure often associated with intelligence and was used to define species with high “intellectual capabilities”. In general, brain size is correlated with body size. However, there are disparities in terms of relative brain size between species that may be explained by several factors such as the complexity of social behaviour, the ‘social brain hypothesis’, or learning and memory capabilities. These disparities are used to classify species according to an ‘encephalization quotient’. However, environment also has an important role on the development and evolution of brain size. In this review, I summarise the recent studies looking at the effects of environment on brain size in insects, and introduce the idea that the role of environment might be mediated through the relationship between olfaction and vision.
    [Show full text]
  • Advances in the Neuroscience of Intelligence: from Brain Connectivity to Brain Perturbation
    The Spanish Journal of Psychology (2016), 19, e94, 1–7. © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid doi:10.1017/sjp.2016.89 Advances in the Neuroscience of Intelligence: from Brain Connectivity to Brain Perturbation Emiliano Santarnecchi1 and Simone Rossi2,3 1 Berenson-Allen Center for Non-Invasive Brain Stimulation, Harvard Medical School, Boston (USA) 2 Department of Medicine, Surgery and Neuroscience, Brain Investigation and Neuromodulation (SiBIN) Lab, University of Siena 3 Deptartment of Medicine, Surgery and Neuroscience, Human Physiology section, University of Siena Abstract. Our view is that intelligence, as expression of the complexity of the human brain and of its evolutionary path, represents an intriguing example of “system level brain plasticity”: tangible proofs of this assertion lie in the strong links intelligence has with vital brain capacities as information processing (i.e., pure, rough capacity to transfer information in an efficient way), resilience (i.e., the ability to cope with loss of efficiency and/or loss of physical elements in a network) and adaptability (i.e., being able to efficiently rearrange its dynamics in response to environmental demands). Current evidence supporting this view move from theoretical models correlating intelligence and individual response to systematic “lesions” of brain connectivity, as well as from the field of Noninvasive Brain Stimulation (NiBS). Perturbation-based approaches based on techniques as transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and transcranial alter- nating current stimulation (tACS), are opening new in vivo scenarios which could allow to disclose more causal relation- ship between intelligence and brain plasticity, overcoming the limitations of brain-behavior correlational evidence Received 10 June 2016; Revised 21 October 2016; Accepted 24 October 2016 Keywords: intelligence, Gf, non-invasive brain stimulation, plasticity, perturbation.
    [Show full text]
  • Modi Ed Formulas for Calculation of Encephalization Quotient in Dogs
    Modied Formulas for Calculation of Encephalization Quotient in Dogs Saganuwan Alhaji Saganuwan ( [email protected] ) Federal University of Agriculture Research note Keywords: German Shepherd, Allometry, Encephalization Quotient, Intelligence Posted Date: March 18th, 2021 DOI: https://doi.org/10.21203/rs.3.rs-322274/v1 License: This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. Read Full License Page 1/8 Abstract Objective Dogs are a breed of animals that play important roles, ranging from security passing through companionship to models of research for application in humans. Intelligence is the key factor to success in life, most especially for dogs that are used for security purposes at the airports, seaports, public places, houses, schools and farms. However, it has been reported that there is correlation between intelligence, body weight, height and craniometry in human. In view of this, literatures on body weight, height and body surface areas of ten dogs were assessed with a view to determining their comparative level of intelligence. Results Findings revealed that dogs share brain common allometric relationships with human as shown by Encephalization Quotient (EQ)= Brain Mass/0.14 x Body weight0.528 as compared with Brain Mass /0.12 x Body Weight0.66 and Brain Mass (E)=kpβ, where p is the body weight,k=0.14 and β=0.528 which yielded better results as compared with the other formulas. Dogs with BSA, weight and height similar to that of human are the most intelligent. Doberman Pinscher is the most intelligent followed by German Shepherd, Labrador Retriever, Golden Retriever, respectively.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolution of the Size and Functional Areas of the Human Brain
    ANRV287-AN35-20 ARI 9 September 2006 8:42 Evolution of the Size and Functional Areas of the Human Brain P. Thomas Schoenemann Department of Behavioral Sciences, University of Michigan–Dearborn, Dearborn, Michigan 48128; email: [email protected] Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006. 35:379–406 Key Words First published online as a Review in neuroanatomy, encephalization, behavior, adaptation, selection Advance on June 16, 2006 The Annual Review of Anthropology is online Abstract at anthro.annualreviews.org The human brain is one of the most intricate, complicated, and This article’s doi: impressive organs ever to have evolved. Understanding its evolu- by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org 10.1146/annurev.anthro.35.081705.123210 tion requires integrating knowledge from a variety of disciplines in Copyright c 2006 by Annual Reviews. the natural and social sciences. Four areas of research are particu- All rights reserved larly important to this endeavor. First, we need to understand basic 0084-6570/06/1021-0379$20.00 principles of brain evolution that appear to operate across broad classes of organisms. Second, we need to understand the ways in which human brains differ from the brains of our closest living rela- tives. Third, clues from the fossil record may allow us to outline the manner in which these differences evolved. Finally, studies of brain structure/function relationships are critical for us to make behav- ioral sense of the evolutionary changes that occurred. This review highlights important questions and work in each of these areas.
    [Show full text]
  • Animal Consciousness
    The Distribution and Evolution of Animal Consciousness By Joseph Vitti Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Bachelor of Arts with Honors in Philosophy Harvard University March 26, 2010 1 © Joseph Vitti 2010 Contents Chapter One Preliminaries of Animal Consciousness . 3 Chapter Two Towards a Function of Consciousness: Evolutionary Considerations and Global Workspace Theory . 13 Chapter Three From the Theoretical to the Empirical: The Broadcast Hypothesis, Modularity, and Associative Learning . 27 Chapter Four Evidence of Associative Capacities and Consciousness Across the Animal Kingdom . 35 Chapter Five Animal Consciousness and Ethics . 50 References . 59 2 © Joseph Vitti 2010 CHAPTER 1 Preliminaries of Animal Consciousness Which nonhuman animals (hereafter: animals) are conscious? Many people distinguish first between primary consciousness – which includes awareness of percepts, sensations, immediate thoughts and so on – and higher-order consciousness – which refers to awareness of one's primary awareness, or “thought about thought” (Edelman, 2003). A number of experimental paradigms have been devised in recent years to identify higher-order consciousness in animals (particularly primates, cetaceans, and birds), but the study of primary consciousness is somewhat more elusive. In what follows, I will use the term 'consciousness' to refer to primary consciousness. What does it mean to be a conscious animal? One oft-cited definition is the one Nagel invoked when he asked what it is like to be a bat (1974). An organism or a mental process can be said to be conscious if there is something that it is like to be that organism or to be undergoing that process; essential to consciousness is its phenomenal, experiential, or qualitative feel.
    [Show full text]
  • Brain Size at Birth Throughout Human Evolution: a New Method for Estimating Neonatal Brain Size in Hominins
    Journal of Human Evolution 55 (2008) 1064–1074 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Human Evolution journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jhevol Brain size at birth throughout human evolution: A new method for estimating neonatal brain size in hominins Jeremy M. DeSilva*, Julie J. Lesnik Department of Anthropology University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-1107, USA article info abstract Article history: An increase in brain size is a hallmark of human evolution. Questions regarding the evolution of brain Received 5 December 2007 development and obstetric constraints in the human lineage can be addressed with accurate estimates of Accepted 30 July 2008 the size of the brain at birth in hominins. Previous estimates of brain size at birth in fossil hominins have been calculated from regressions of neonatal body or brain mass to adult body mass, but this approach is Keywords: problematic for two reasons: modern humans are outliers for these regressions, and hominin adult body Allometry masses are difficult to estimate. To accurately estimate the brain size at birth in extinct human ancestors, Australopithecus an equation is needed for which modern humans fit the anthropoid regression and one in which the Neonatal brain size hominin variable entered into the regression equation has limited error. Using phylogenetically sensitive statistics, a resampling approach, and brain-mass data from the literature and from National Primate Research Centers on 362 neonates and 2802 adults from eight different anthropoid species, we found that the size of the adult brain can strongly predict the size of the neonatal brain (r2 ¼ 0.97). This regression predicts human brain size, indicating that humans have precisely the brain size expected as an adult given the size of the brain at birth.
    [Show full text]
  • Self-Conscious Asides
    _N_ATU__ ~__ v_o_t_ .. ~_~ __ 3t_MA __ R_t_·H __ t%_3 _____________________ E3()()~~E:\/If:\A/~-----------------------------------------45_7 between intelligence and the brain/weight ratio. Self-conscious asides He is equally scathing on the doctrine of encephalization of function, that is that in Stuart Sutherland vertebrate evolution the locus of behavioural functions moves progressively Animal Thought. By Stephen Walker. forward in the brain, particularly into the Routledge & Kegan Paul: 1983. Pp.437. £17.50, $35. forebrain, and neocortex. He points out that the hypothalamus is present in teleost FIFTEEN years ago, my interest in lated in consciousness) and that far from fishes and can be regarded in all vertebrates comparative psychology made it necessary inventing the association of ideas as a as the head-ganglion of the autonomic to keep a number of different animals. A general explanatory principle, he thought it system: its functions do not seem to have distinguished worker in artificial was a thoroughly bad thing and was the shifted. Moreover, although they only intelligence happened to visit me and explanation of the "madness, found in have a rudimentary cortex, birds display asked: "Haven't you got anything better to most men". Darwin was curiously soft on learning abilities in no way inferior to those do than keep a zoo?''. Although at the time animals, ascribing to dogs a sense of of many mammals. his remark seemed to lack a proper humour and a belief in the supernatural. As After an interesting but speculative appreciation of the scientific spirit, the to the behaviourists, they denied thought chapter on the survival value of subsequent history of comparative to both man and animals thus placing them intelligence, Walker concludes with a psychology suggests that he may have had on the same level.
    [Show full text]
  • Body Mass Measurements and Their Effects on Calculating Encephalization Quotients in Mammals
    BODY MASS MEASUREMENTS AND THEIR EFFECTS ON CALCULATING ENCEPHALIZATION QUOTIENTS IN MAMMALS A THESIS Presented to the University Honors Program California State University, Long Beach In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the University Honors Program Certificate Chika Okeke Fall 2017 I, THE UNDERSIGNED MEMBER OF THE COMMITTEE, HAVE APPROVED THIS THESIS BODY MASS MEASUREMENTS AND THEIR EFFECTS ON CALCULATING ENCEPHALIZATION QUOTIENTS IN MAMMALS BY Chika Okeke _____________________________________________________________ Theodore Stankowich, Ph.D. (Thesis Advisor) Department of Biology California State University, Long Beach Fall 2017 ABSTRACT BODY MASS MEASUREMENTS AND THEIR EFFECTS ON CALCULATING ENCEPHALIZATION QUOTIENTS IN MAMMALS By Chika J. Okeke Fall 2017 Multiple intelligence and comparative animal behavior analysis studies use encephalization quotient (EQ) for their analyses. Most of these studies use the actual brain mass of each specimen in addition to an average of the body mass to generate EQ. However, using the average instead of individual-specific data could lead to bias if there is a significant difference between an individual’s actual body mass and the average published body masses. Using Sciuridae as my model, I measured the skull length and volume as well as the body mass of several species. These data were used to calculate the EQ of each species and then compared to EQs generated by using average body masses from published literature. The result of this project may improve the accuracy of future studies on the relationships between encephalization quotients and performance cognitive tasks. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to thank my research advisor, Theodore Stankowich, for helping me formulate and write this thesis.
    [Show full text]
  • Brain Size Predicts the Success of Mammal Species Introduced Into Novel Environments
    vol. 172, supplement the american naturalist july 2008 ൴ Brain Size Predicts the Success of Mammal Species Introduced into Novel Environments Daniel Sol,1,* Sven Bacher,2,† Simon M. Reader,3,4,‡ and Louis Lefebvre4,§ 1. Centre for Ecological Research and Applied Forestries (CREAF), Some mammals, such as primates and cetaceans, have Autonomous University of Barcelona, E-08193 Bellaterra, evolved large brains despite the substantial metabolic and Catalonia, Spain; developmental costs involved (van Schaik and Deaner 2. Community Ecology, Zoological Institute, University of Bern, Baltzerstrasse 6, 3012 Bern, Switzerland; 2003; Isler and van Schaik 2006). Why? Past research on 3. Behavioural Biology and Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht this question has centered on the brain’s role in sensory, University, P.O. Box 80086, 3508 TB Utrecht, The Netherlands; cognitive, and motor functions (Barton 2006). Increased 4. Department of Biology, 1205 Avenue Docteur Penfield, McGill brain size has been linked to sensory specializations (Joffe University, Montre´al, Que´bec H3A 1B1, Canada and Dunbar 1997; Barton 1998), cognitive skills (Reader and Laland 2002; Byrne and Corp 2004), and motor func- Online enhancement: appendix. tions (Changizi 2003). Several of these associations have also been documented in birds (e.g., Lefebvre et al. 1997, 2004; Garamszegi et al. 2002), suggesting convergent evo- lution of the brain between these classes (Emery and Clay- abstract: Large brains, relative to body size, can confer advantages ton 2004; Lefebvre et al. 2004). Although the underlying to individuals in the form of behavioral flexibility. Such enhanced neurophysiological mechanisms are not well understood behavioral flexibility is predicted to carry fitness benefits to individ- uals facing novel or altered environmental conditions, a theory (Roth and Dicke 2005; Dunbar and Shultz 2007), the ex- known as the brain size–environmental change hypothesis.
    [Show full text]
  • Considering Animals' Feelings
    Animal Sentience 2016.005: Broom on Animal Welfare Multiple Book Review: Animal Sentience publishes multiple reviews of selected books whose Précis is published in the journal along with the reviews and the author’s response(s). Most of the multiple book reviews are invited individually by the Editor but freely submitted reviews of the selected books whose Précis has appeared can also be considered for publication. Books can also be nominated and reviewers can be proposed by writing to the Editor. Instructions: http://animalstudiesrepository.org/animsent/guidelines.html Considering animals’ feelings Précis of Sentience and animal welfare (Broom 2014) Donald M. Broom Centre for Anthrozoology and Animal Welfare University of Cambridge Abstract: The concept of sentience concerns the capacity to have feelings. There is evidence for sophisticated cognitive concepts and for both positive and negative feelings in a wide range of nonhuman animals. All vertebrates, including fish, as well as some molluscs and decapod crustaceans have pain systems. Most people today consider that their moral obligations extend to many animal species. Moral decisions about abortion, euthanasia, and the various ways we protect animals should take into account the research findings about sentience. In addition, all animal life should be respected and studies of the welfare of even the simplest invertebrate animals should be taken into consideration when we interact with these animals. Keywords: sentience, animal welfare, anthropomorphism, cognition, feelings, species- differences Donald M. Broom [email protected] is Emeritus Professor of Animal Welfare at Cambridge University. He has developed concepts and methods of scientific assessment of animal welfare and studied: cognitive abilities of animals, the welfare of animals in relation to housing and transport, behaviour problems, attitudes to animals and ethics of animal usage.
    [Show full text]