Self-Conscious Asides

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Self-Conscious Asides _N_ATU__ ~__ v_o_t_ .. ~_~ __ 3t_MA __ R_t_·H __ t%_3 _____________________ E3()()~~E:\/If:\A/~-----------------------------------------45_7 between intelligence and the brain/weight ratio. Self-conscious asides He is equally scathing on the doctrine of encephalization of function, that is that in Stuart Sutherland vertebrate evolution the locus of behavioural functions moves progressively Animal Thought. By Stephen Walker. forward in the brain, particularly into the Routledge & Kegan Paul: 1983. Pp.437. £17.50, $35. forebrain, and neocortex. He points out that the hypothalamus is present in teleost FIFTEEN years ago, my interest in lated in consciousness) and that far from fishes and can be regarded in all vertebrates comparative psychology made it necessary inventing the association of ideas as a as the head-ganglion of the autonomic to keep a number of different animals. A general explanatory principle, he thought it system: its functions do not seem to have distinguished worker in artificial was a thoroughly bad thing and was the shifted. Moreover, although they only intelligence happened to visit me and explanation of the "madness, found in have a rudimentary cortex, birds display asked: "Haven't you got anything better to most men". Darwin was curiously soft on learning abilities in no way inferior to those do than keep a zoo?''. Although at the time animals, ascribing to dogs a sense of of many mammals. his remark seemed to lack a proper humour and a belief in the supernatural. As After an interesting but speculative appreciation of the scientific spirit, the to the behaviourists, they denied thought chapter on the survival value of subsequent history of comparative to both man and animals thus placing them intelligence, Walker concludes with a psychology suggests that he may have had on the same level. survey of perception and memory in more sense than I credited him with. Walker concludes that little reliance can animals and an account of monkeys' use of At that time comparative psychologists be placed on the authorities and that one knowledge and their ability to learn to use were painstakingly measuring the can only decide whether animals think like signs. He is somewhat preoccupied with the performance of different groups of man by examining how far their behaviour notion of conscious thought, and takes as animals on a large number of learning and neuroanatomy are similar to our own. evidence for it the finding that many tasks. They were trying to construct a two­ The idea that similarities in neuroanatomy animals can be trained to wait for a minute by-two table on which the axes represented are relevant to consciousness is not new. or more after having received a signal to tasks and groups, and the cell entries Walker quotes Charles Kingsley satirising respond, and can still make the correct represented performance. Apart from Thomas Huxley who is made to declare reaction. But this surely does not imply that keeping themselves busy, they had two that apes have "hippopotamus majors" in the animal has a conscious memory of the aims. First, by discovering sets of tasks in their brains just like men: signal. Again, recent work on animal which any species that could perform one learning proves that animals do not learn task could perform all the others, they You may think that there are more important merely to make responses on the basis of hoped to infer the nature of the skill needed differences between you and an ape, such as rewards and punishments - they learn for all the tasks in the set. Second, by being able to speak, and make machines, and what sequences of events occur, but this know right from wrong, and say your prayers, examining the neuroanatomical dif­ and other little matters of that kind; but that is a does not demonstrate that they have ferences between the species that could and child's fancy, my dear. Nothing is to be conscious expectancies, even though they could not perform a given task, they hoped depended on but the great hippopotamus test. act as though they do. Indeed one of the to learn about the brain mechanisms main problems with consciousness is that underlying successful performance. For Walker gives a clear account of nobody has been able to suggest any several reasons this laborious approach to comparative neuroanatomy, in which he evolutionary advantage for it. Men can the problem of comparative intelligence displays a pleasing cynicism about the learn and carry out highly skilled and proved a failure. It turned out that in many specious generalizations often drawn in complex motor patterns without being cases whether or not an animal could be that field. Much effort has been wasted conscious of how the movements are trained to perform a task reflected not its measuring brain size and wholly useless executed and it is not apparent why taking own native wit, but the ingenuity of the equations have been produced, such as the certain decisions or using language should experimenter in devising training appro­ formula governing the (approximate) ratio involve consciousness. priate to the animal's natural way of life. of brain weight to body weight, which for Walker is in fact concerned that Moreover, the assumption that the same mammals and birds is E= 0.07°·67 P (where consciousness may be bound up with anatomical part of the brain, as defined by E is the expected ratio and Pis the body language, in which case one would have to its evolutionary origin, has the same role in weight). Some intelligent species such as deny animals conscious thought. But we do all species is false. Removing the man, the crow and the porpoise have a not deny consciousness to infants and on hippocampus in a rat has very different brain weight considerably in excess of that her own account Helen Keller, who was effects on behaviour from those produced predicted by this formula, but measuring deaf, dumb and blind, was conscious by removing it in man. Finally, there is brain size is no more likely to tell us before she learned to communicate. often as wide a range of ability on a given anything significant about how the brain Regardless of language, the similarity task within a group as between groups. works than measuring the size of between human and animal behaviour and Although Stephen Walker sets out to computers is to reveal the programs they neuroanatomy surely justifies Walker in answer the question how far animal and can run. Moreover, the appearance of ascribing conscious thought to primates human thought are similar, he is body weight in the ratio is curious. Birds, and other higher animals, but neither he thoroughly aware of the difficulties that for obvious reasons, are very light, and so nor anyone else is ever likely to penetrate beset comparative psychology. He starts are sharks which have a cartilaginous the thoughts of a lamprey. with a historical review of the opinions of skeleton and therefore turn in a high Animal Thought is a well-written and earlier authorities, which is more brain/body-weight ratio, although they are stimulating book, containing occasional interesting in its asides than in its main regarded as a primitive order of fish. flashes of wit. But it is interesting more for theme. Thus, it is of little consequence that Neanderthal man probably had a higher its obiter dicta and for its way of dealing Descartes thought animals (and machines) ratio than modern man, though he may of with hallowed nonsense than for its main could never have knowledge or language, course have been more intelligent. As message which will surprise few readers. ~ I but it is of interest that Locke formulated Walker remarks, since nobody has the recently rediscovered concept of succeeded in assessing the intelligence of Stuart Sutherland is Director of the Centre for "working memory" (only a small number animals (or man come to that) objectively, Research on Perception and Cognition at the of ideas can be simultaneously manipu- there is no good evidence for an association University of Sussex. © 1983 Nature Publishing Group.
Recommended publications
  • The Neuroscience of Human Intelligence Differences
    Edinburgh Research Explorer The neuroscience of human intelligence differences Citation for published version: Deary, IJ, Penke, L & Johnson, W 2010, 'The neuroscience of human intelligence differences', Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 11, pp. 201-211. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2793 Digital Object Identifier (DOI): 10.1038/nrn2793 Link: Link to publication record in Edinburgh Research Explorer Document Version: Peer reviewed version Published In: Nature Reviews Neuroscience Publisher Rights Statement: This is an author's accepted manuscript of the following article: Deary, I. J., Penke, L. & Johnson, W. (2010), "The neuroscience of human intelligence differences", in Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11, p. 201-211. The final publication is available at http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2793 General rights Copyright for the publications made accessible via the Edinburgh Research Explorer is retained by the author(s) and / or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing these publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. Take down policy The University of Edinburgh has made every reasonable effort to ensure that Edinburgh Research Explorer content complies with UK legislation. If you believe that the public display of this file breaches copyright please contact [email protected] providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Download date: 02. Oct. 2021 Nature Reviews Neuroscience in press The neuroscience of human intelligence differences Ian J. Deary*, Lars Penke* and Wendy Johnson* *Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh EH4 2EE, Scotland, UK. All authors contributed equally to the work.
    [Show full text]
  • Seasonality and Brain Size Are Negatively Associated in Frogs
    www.nature.com/scientificreports OPEN Seasonality and brain size are negatively associated in frogs: evidence for the expensive brain Received: 7 July 2017 Accepted: 20 November 2017 framework Published: xx xx xxxx Yi Luo1,2, Mao Jun Zhong1,2, Yan Huang1,2, Feng Li1,2, Wen Bo Liao1,2 & Alexander Kotrschal3 The challenges of seasonal environments are thought to contribute to brain evolution, but in which way is debated. According to the Cognitive Bufer Hypothesis (CBH) brain size should increase with seasonality, as the cognitive benefts of a larger brain should help overcoming periods of food scarcity via, for instance, increased behavioral fexibility. However, in line with the Expensive Brain Framework (EBF) brain size should decrease with seasonality because a smaller brain confers energetic benefts in periods of food scarcity. Empirical evidence is inconclusive and mostly limited to homoeothermic animals. Here we used phylogenetic comparative analyses to test the impact of seasonality on brain evolution across 30 species of anurans (frogs) experiencing a wide range of temperature and precipitation. Our results support the EBF because relative brain size and the size of the optic tectum were negatively correlated with variability in temperature. In contrast, we found no association between the variability in precipitation and the length of the dry season with either brain size or the sizes of other major brain regions. We suggest that seasonality-induced food scarcity resulting from higher variability in temperature constrains brain size evolution in anurans. Less seasonal environments may therefore facilitate the evolution of larger brains in poikilothermic animals. Brain size varies dramatically between species1 and the recent decades have uncovered a variety of factors that drive this evolution2–10.
    [Show full text]
  • A Review of Effects of Environment on Brain Size in Insects
    insects Review A Review of Effects of Environment on Brain Size in Insects Thomas Carle Faculty of Biology, Kyushu University, Fukuoka 819-0395, Japan; [email protected] Simple Summary: What makes a big brain is fascinating since it is considered as a measure of intelligence. Above all, brain size is associated with body size. If species that have evolved with complex social behaviours possess relatively bigger brains than those deprived of such behaviours, this does not constitute the only factor affecting brain size. Other factors such as individual experience or surrounding environment also play roles in the size of the brain. In this review, I summarize the recent findings about the effects of environment on brain size in insects. I also discuss evidence about how the environment has an impact on sensory systems and influences brain size. Abstract: Brain size fascinates society as well as researchers since it is a measure often associated with intelligence and was used to define species with high “intellectual capabilities”. In general, brain size is correlated with body size. However, there are disparities in terms of relative brain size between species that may be explained by several factors such as the complexity of social behaviour, the ‘social brain hypothesis’, or learning and memory capabilities. These disparities are used to classify species according to an ‘encephalization quotient’. However, environment also has an important role on the development and evolution of brain size. In this review, I summarise the recent studies looking at the effects of environment on brain size in insects, and introduce the idea that the role of environment might be mediated through the relationship between olfaction and vision.
    [Show full text]
  • Advances in the Neuroscience of Intelligence: from Brain Connectivity to Brain Perturbation
    The Spanish Journal of Psychology (2016), 19, e94, 1–7. © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid doi:10.1017/sjp.2016.89 Advances in the Neuroscience of Intelligence: from Brain Connectivity to Brain Perturbation Emiliano Santarnecchi1 and Simone Rossi2,3 1 Berenson-Allen Center for Non-Invasive Brain Stimulation, Harvard Medical School, Boston (USA) 2 Department of Medicine, Surgery and Neuroscience, Brain Investigation and Neuromodulation (SiBIN) Lab, University of Siena 3 Deptartment of Medicine, Surgery and Neuroscience, Human Physiology section, University of Siena Abstract. Our view is that intelligence, as expression of the complexity of the human brain and of its evolutionary path, represents an intriguing example of “system level brain plasticity”: tangible proofs of this assertion lie in the strong links intelligence has with vital brain capacities as information processing (i.e., pure, rough capacity to transfer information in an efficient way), resilience (i.e., the ability to cope with loss of efficiency and/or loss of physical elements in a network) and adaptability (i.e., being able to efficiently rearrange its dynamics in response to environmental demands). Current evidence supporting this view move from theoretical models correlating intelligence and individual response to systematic “lesions” of brain connectivity, as well as from the field of Noninvasive Brain Stimulation (NiBS). Perturbation-based approaches based on techniques as transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and transcranial alter- nating current stimulation (tACS), are opening new in vivo scenarios which could allow to disclose more causal relation- ship between intelligence and brain plasticity, overcoming the limitations of brain-behavior correlational evidence Received 10 June 2016; Revised 21 October 2016; Accepted 24 October 2016 Keywords: intelligence, Gf, non-invasive brain stimulation, plasticity, perturbation.
    [Show full text]
  • Modi Ed Formulas for Calculation of Encephalization Quotient in Dogs
    Modied Formulas for Calculation of Encephalization Quotient in Dogs Saganuwan Alhaji Saganuwan ( [email protected] ) Federal University of Agriculture Research note Keywords: German Shepherd, Allometry, Encephalization Quotient, Intelligence Posted Date: March 18th, 2021 DOI: https://doi.org/10.21203/rs.3.rs-322274/v1 License: This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. Read Full License Page 1/8 Abstract Objective Dogs are a breed of animals that play important roles, ranging from security passing through companionship to models of research for application in humans. Intelligence is the key factor to success in life, most especially for dogs that are used for security purposes at the airports, seaports, public places, houses, schools and farms. However, it has been reported that there is correlation between intelligence, body weight, height and craniometry in human. In view of this, literatures on body weight, height and body surface areas of ten dogs were assessed with a view to determining their comparative level of intelligence. Results Findings revealed that dogs share brain common allometric relationships with human as shown by Encephalization Quotient (EQ)= Brain Mass/0.14 x Body weight0.528 as compared with Brain Mass /0.12 x Body Weight0.66 and Brain Mass (E)=kpβ, where p is the body weight,k=0.14 and β=0.528 which yielded better results as compared with the other formulas. Dogs with BSA, weight and height similar to that of human are the most intelligent. Doberman Pinscher is the most intelligent followed by German Shepherd, Labrador Retriever, Golden Retriever, respectively.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolution of the Size and Functional Areas of the Human Brain
    ANRV287-AN35-20 ARI 9 September 2006 8:42 Evolution of the Size and Functional Areas of the Human Brain P. Thomas Schoenemann Department of Behavioral Sciences, University of Michigan–Dearborn, Dearborn, Michigan 48128; email: [email protected] Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006. 35:379–406 Key Words First published online as a Review in neuroanatomy, encephalization, behavior, adaptation, selection Advance on June 16, 2006 The Annual Review of Anthropology is online Abstract at anthro.annualreviews.org The human brain is one of the most intricate, complicated, and This article’s doi: impressive organs ever to have evolved. Understanding its evolu- by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org 10.1146/annurev.anthro.35.081705.123210 tion requires integrating knowledge from a variety of disciplines in Copyright c 2006 by Annual Reviews. the natural and social sciences. Four areas of research are particu- All rights reserved larly important to this endeavor. First, we need to understand basic 0084-6570/06/1021-0379$20.00 principles of brain evolution that appear to operate across broad classes of organisms. Second, we need to understand the ways in which human brains differ from the brains of our closest living rela- tives. Third, clues from the fossil record may allow us to outline the manner in which these differences evolved. Finally, studies of brain structure/function relationships are critical for us to make behav- ioral sense of the evolutionary changes that occurred. This review highlights important questions and work in each of these areas.
    [Show full text]
  • Animal Consciousness
    The Distribution and Evolution of Animal Consciousness By Joseph Vitti Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Bachelor of Arts with Honors in Philosophy Harvard University March 26, 2010 1 © Joseph Vitti 2010 Contents Chapter One Preliminaries of Animal Consciousness . 3 Chapter Two Towards a Function of Consciousness: Evolutionary Considerations and Global Workspace Theory . 13 Chapter Three From the Theoretical to the Empirical: The Broadcast Hypothesis, Modularity, and Associative Learning . 27 Chapter Four Evidence of Associative Capacities and Consciousness Across the Animal Kingdom . 35 Chapter Five Animal Consciousness and Ethics . 50 References . 59 2 © Joseph Vitti 2010 CHAPTER 1 Preliminaries of Animal Consciousness Which nonhuman animals (hereafter: animals) are conscious? Many people distinguish first between primary consciousness – which includes awareness of percepts, sensations, immediate thoughts and so on – and higher-order consciousness – which refers to awareness of one's primary awareness, or “thought about thought” (Edelman, 2003). A number of experimental paradigms have been devised in recent years to identify higher-order consciousness in animals (particularly primates, cetaceans, and birds), but the study of primary consciousness is somewhat more elusive. In what follows, I will use the term 'consciousness' to refer to primary consciousness. What does it mean to be a conscious animal? One oft-cited definition is the one Nagel invoked when he asked what it is like to be a bat (1974). An organism or a mental process can be said to be conscious if there is something that it is like to be that organism or to be undergoing that process; essential to consciousness is its phenomenal, experiential, or qualitative feel.
    [Show full text]
  • Brain Size at Birth Throughout Human Evolution: a New Method for Estimating Neonatal Brain Size in Hominins
    Journal of Human Evolution 55 (2008) 1064–1074 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Human Evolution journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jhevol Brain size at birth throughout human evolution: A new method for estimating neonatal brain size in hominins Jeremy M. DeSilva*, Julie J. Lesnik Department of Anthropology University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-1107, USA article info abstract Article history: An increase in brain size is a hallmark of human evolution. Questions regarding the evolution of brain Received 5 December 2007 development and obstetric constraints in the human lineage can be addressed with accurate estimates of Accepted 30 July 2008 the size of the brain at birth in hominins. Previous estimates of brain size at birth in fossil hominins have been calculated from regressions of neonatal body or brain mass to adult body mass, but this approach is Keywords: problematic for two reasons: modern humans are outliers for these regressions, and hominin adult body Allometry masses are difficult to estimate. To accurately estimate the brain size at birth in extinct human ancestors, Australopithecus an equation is needed for which modern humans fit the anthropoid regression and one in which the Neonatal brain size hominin variable entered into the regression equation has limited error. Using phylogenetically sensitive statistics, a resampling approach, and brain-mass data from the literature and from National Primate Research Centers on 362 neonates and 2802 adults from eight different anthropoid species, we found that the size of the adult brain can strongly predict the size of the neonatal brain (r2 ¼ 0.97). This regression predicts human brain size, indicating that humans have precisely the brain size expected as an adult given the size of the brain at birth.
    [Show full text]
  • Body Mass Measurements and Their Effects on Calculating Encephalization Quotients in Mammals
    BODY MASS MEASUREMENTS AND THEIR EFFECTS ON CALCULATING ENCEPHALIZATION QUOTIENTS IN MAMMALS A THESIS Presented to the University Honors Program California State University, Long Beach In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the University Honors Program Certificate Chika Okeke Fall 2017 I, THE UNDERSIGNED MEMBER OF THE COMMITTEE, HAVE APPROVED THIS THESIS BODY MASS MEASUREMENTS AND THEIR EFFECTS ON CALCULATING ENCEPHALIZATION QUOTIENTS IN MAMMALS BY Chika Okeke _____________________________________________________________ Theodore Stankowich, Ph.D. (Thesis Advisor) Department of Biology California State University, Long Beach Fall 2017 ABSTRACT BODY MASS MEASUREMENTS AND THEIR EFFECTS ON CALCULATING ENCEPHALIZATION QUOTIENTS IN MAMMALS By Chika J. Okeke Fall 2017 Multiple intelligence and comparative animal behavior analysis studies use encephalization quotient (EQ) for their analyses. Most of these studies use the actual brain mass of each specimen in addition to an average of the body mass to generate EQ. However, using the average instead of individual-specific data could lead to bias if there is a significant difference between an individual’s actual body mass and the average published body masses. Using Sciuridae as my model, I measured the skull length and volume as well as the body mass of several species. These data were used to calculate the EQ of each species and then compared to EQs generated by using average body masses from published literature. The result of this project may improve the accuracy of future studies on the relationships between encephalization quotients and performance cognitive tasks. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to thank my research advisor, Theodore Stankowich, for helping me formulate and write this thesis.
    [Show full text]
  • Brain Size Predicts the Success of Mammal Species Introduced Into Novel Environments
    vol. 172, supplement the american naturalist july 2008 ൴ Brain Size Predicts the Success of Mammal Species Introduced into Novel Environments Daniel Sol,1,* Sven Bacher,2,† Simon M. Reader,3,4,‡ and Louis Lefebvre4,§ 1. Centre for Ecological Research and Applied Forestries (CREAF), Some mammals, such as primates and cetaceans, have Autonomous University of Barcelona, E-08193 Bellaterra, evolved large brains despite the substantial metabolic and Catalonia, Spain; developmental costs involved (van Schaik and Deaner 2. Community Ecology, Zoological Institute, University of Bern, Baltzerstrasse 6, 3012 Bern, Switzerland; 2003; Isler and van Schaik 2006). Why? Past research on 3. Behavioural Biology and Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht this question has centered on the brain’s role in sensory, University, P.O. Box 80086, 3508 TB Utrecht, The Netherlands; cognitive, and motor functions (Barton 2006). Increased 4. Department of Biology, 1205 Avenue Docteur Penfield, McGill brain size has been linked to sensory specializations (Joffe University, Montre´al, Que´bec H3A 1B1, Canada and Dunbar 1997; Barton 1998), cognitive skills (Reader and Laland 2002; Byrne and Corp 2004), and motor func- Online enhancement: appendix. tions (Changizi 2003). Several of these associations have also been documented in birds (e.g., Lefebvre et al. 1997, 2004; Garamszegi et al. 2002), suggesting convergent evo- lution of the brain between these classes (Emery and Clay- abstract: Large brains, relative to body size, can confer advantages ton 2004; Lefebvre et al. 2004). Although the underlying to individuals in the form of behavioral flexibility. Such enhanced neurophysiological mechanisms are not well understood behavioral flexibility is predicted to carry fitness benefits to individ- uals facing novel or altered environmental conditions, a theory (Roth and Dicke 2005; Dunbar and Shultz 2007), the ex- known as the brain size–environmental change hypothesis.
    [Show full text]
  • Considering Animals' Feelings
    Animal Sentience 2016.005: Broom on Animal Welfare Multiple Book Review: Animal Sentience publishes multiple reviews of selected books whose Précis is published in the journal along with the reviews and the author’s response(s). Most of the multiple book reviews are invited individually by the Editor but freely submitted reviews of the selected books whose Précis has appeared can also be considered for publication. Books can also be nominated and reviewers can be proposed by writing to the Editor. Instructions: http://animalstudiesrepository.org/animsent/guidelines.html Considering animals’ feelings Précis of Sentience and animal welfare (Broom 2014) Donald M. Broom Centre for Anthrozoology and Animal Welfare University of Cambridge Abstract: The concept of sentience concerns the capacity to have feelings. There is evidence for sophisticated cognitive concepts and for both positive and negative feelings in a wide range of nonhuman animals. All vertebrates, including fish, as well as some molluscs and decapod crustaceans have pain systems. Most people today consider that their moral obligations extend to many animal species. Moral decisions about abortion, euthanasia, and the various ways we protect animals should take into account the research findings about sentience. In addition, all animal life should be respected and studies of the welfare of even the simplest invertebrate animals should be taken into consideration when we interact with these animals. Keywords: sentience, animal welfare, anthropomorphism, cognition, feelings, species- differences Donald M. Broom [email protected] is Emeritus Professor of Animal Welfare at Cambridge University. He has developed concepts and methods of scientific assessment of animal welfare and studied: cognitive abilities of animals, the welfare of animals in relation to housing and transport, behaviour problems, attitudes to animals and ethics of animal usage.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolution of the Human Brain • Homo Sapiens • Hominin Ancestors • Chimpanzees • Primates Evolution of Homo Sapiens Origin of Anatomically Modern Humans
    Evolution of the Human Brain • Homo sapiens • Hominin ancestors • Chimpanzees • Primates Evolution of Homo sapiens Origin of anatomically modern humans • ~200,000 ya • Human population genetics • Archeology, Fossil record Origin of anatomically modern humans • Average brain size 1300 cc • Extended life history • Increased post-natal brain growth • Expanded prefrontal cortex relative to early ancestors • Lack of brow ridges, presence of chin • Behavioral flexibility Origins of behavioral modernity • ~70,000 ya • Art, jewelry, stone tool refinement What behaviors are uniquely human? • Language • Abstract thinking • Art • Music • Sociality • Disorders (Schizophrenia, Autism, Alzheimer’s) What neurological changes made us human? • No brain size change 200,000-70,000 ya • Decrease in genetic diversity • Need comparative data for evidence (genetic and fossil) Homo sapiens were not alone Neanderthals, Hobbits and Denisovans Homo neanderthalensis • First hominin fossil discovered, 1856 • 200,000-35,000 ya • Europe, Middle East • Brain size 1420 cc Homo neanderthalensis • Tools, fire • Language? • Art? • Ritual burial Homo floresiensis • 18,000 ya • Flores, Indonesia • Brain 400 cc • Tools ! Denisovans • 40,000 ya • Siberia • Small fossil fragment only Neanderthal brains • Olfaction, vision, reduced prefrontal cortex Hobbit brains (Homo floresiensis) Comparative genomics: Homo sapiens /early hominins • FOXP2 – language • Brain size • Autism • Nerve development Denisovian genes • 8 associated with brain function or nervous system development (NOVA1,
    [Show full text]