A Distributed Cognition Account of the Design of a Large Scale Software System

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

A Distributed Cognition Account of the Design of a Large Scale Software System DESIGNING THE SAKAI OPEN ACADEMIC ENVIRONMENT: A DISTRIBUTED COGNITION ACCOUNT OF THE DESIGN OF A LARGE SCALE SOFTWARE SYSTEM VOLUME 1 A Dissertation Presented to The Academic Faculty By Klara Benda In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in Human-Centered Computing in the College of Computing, School of Interactive Computing Georgia Institute of Technology August 2014 COPYRIGHT © 2014 BY KLARA BENDA DESIGNING THE SAKAI OPEN ACADEMIC ENVIRONMENT: A DISTRIBUTED COGNITION ACCOUNT OF THE DESIGN OF A LARGE SCALE SOFTWARE SYSTEM Approved by: Dr. Nancy J. Nersessian, Advisor Dr. Elizabeth Mynatt School of Interactive Computing School of Interactive Computing Georgia Institute of Technology Georgia Institute of Technology Dr. Colin Potts Dr. Hanne Andersen School of Interactive Computing Department of Physics and Astronomy Georgia Institute of Technology - Centre for Science Studies Aarhus University Dr. Geoffrey C. Bowker Department of Informatics University of Irvine Date Approved: [May 12, 2014] ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to thank my advisor, Professor Nancy Nersessian, for taking me on as a last student, and for the intellectual support and encouragement that she expended for my dissertation work. I also want to thank the School of Interactive Computing at Georgia Tech for the financial support I received throughout my doctoral studies. Finally, I will also thank my husband, Bence, for embarking on an international adventure with me, and having the patience and good humor to support me in all adversities. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS iii LIST OF TABLES x LIST OF FIGURES xi SUMMARY xv CHAPTER 1. Introduction 1 Outline of chapters 3 Chapter 1. Introduction 3 Chapter 2. Problem formulation 3 Chapter 3. Method 3 Chapter 4. Creating the space for innovation in domain-driven open source 4 Chapter 5. Constructing a new conceptual model of user experience 4 Chapter 6. UX-driven design 4 Chapter 7. The social-technical gap 4 Chapter 8. Knowing the contexts of use 4 Chapter 9. Infrastructural implosion 5 Chapter 10. Discussion 5 CHAPTER 2. Problem statement 6 2.1. Social approaches to technological change and software design 7 2.1.1. Responses to technological determinism 7 2.1.2. Two threads in social accounts of technological change 11 2.1.3. Human-centered approaches to the design of software 17 2.2. Cognitive processes in design 20 iv 2.2.1. Research on design and creativity 20 2.2.2. Distributed cognition in design 23 2.3. Participation in cognitive process 26 2.4. Revisiting context in light of distributed cognition 29 2.5. The practice framework 33 2.6. The research questions addressed by the research 38 CHAPTER 3. Methodology 40 3.1. General considerations behind research design 40 3.1.1. Validity 40 3.1.2. Reliability 42 3.2. Outline of the ethnographically-informed cognitive-historical method 43 3.2.1. Characteristics of practice studies 43 3.2.2. Applying practice-based methods to online practices 46 3.3. Data collection 50 3.4. Data analysis 55 3.4.1. Iteration between data collection and theorizing 56 3.4.2. The comparative approach 63 3.4.3. Theoretical sampling 65 CHAPTER 4. Creating the space for innovation in domain-driven open source 68 4.1. Introduction 68 4.2. Domain-driven open source and innovation 69 4.2.1. Building software for higher education from within 69 4.2.2. Licensing 75 v 4.2.3. Innovation in open source development 75 4.3. Making space for design 79 4.3.1. Open source governance in Sakai: creating the conditions of epistemic collaboration 80 4.3.2. Reflecting on architecture for organizing epistemic work with software 87 4.3.3. Creation of a design space for a new Sakai 94 4.3.4. Guiding design through the web of discourse 99 4.4. Discussion 105 CHAPTER 5. Constructing a new conceptual model of user experience 116 5.1. Designing a new interface: conceptual construction from content widget to group dashboard 119 5.1.1. The starting point: the content model 119 5.1.2. Groups enter the scene 123 5.1.3. Exploring new conceptual models of groups 133 5.1.4. Going beyond initial group conceptualizations 143 5.2. Discussion 157 CHAPTER 6. Design framed by professional methods 163 6.1. The framing of new technologies by the professions 163 6.2. A threefold strategy for the construction of meaning 165 6.2.1. Prototyping 167 6.2.2. UX-led design: tools from professional practice 174 6.2.3. User experience as a source of coherence in conceptual modeling 185 6.3. Unsuccessful professional framing efforts 189 vi 6.3.1. Investigation project: Glimpses of a socio-technical system 190 6.3.2. The Instructional Visioning initiative 196 6.4. Discussion 206 6.4.1. The role of prototypes and prototyping in framing the direction of design 206 6.4.2. Missing perspectives 209 6.4.3. The framing role of professions 211 VOLUME 2. 212 CHAPTER 7. THE SOCIAL-TECHNICAL GAP 213 7.1. Introduction 213 7.2. Case study: the lack of social perspectives in design 214 7.2.1. The failures of Sakai 214 7.2.2. The origins of the kernel and the kernel team 219 7.2.3. The introduction and failure of Jackrabbit 221 7.2.4. Participants explain the failure to themselves 226 7.2.5. Making components work together: the kernel team’s approach to development 229 7.2.6. The failure of Sparse Map and Solr 241 7.2.7. The disconnect of the back end 247 7.3. Discussion 257 CHAPTER 8. Knowing the CONTEXTS OF USE – a distributed cognitive-epistemic strategy 275 8.1. The empirical nexus: mediating contexts of use to software design 275 8.2. Introduction to case studies 288 vii 8.3. First case study: Conceptually grounded collections 290 8.3.1. A glossary 292 8.3.2. A spreadsheet outlining group types in higher education. 296 8.3.3. Mental models collection 300 8.3.4. Contextual collections 315 8.3.5. Discussion of the first case study 330 8.4. Second case study: Memory practices and the contexts of use 332 8.5. Discussion: reconfiguring the empirical and analytic stance in the social approach to software design 341 CHAPTER 9. Infrastructural implosion 346 9.1. Designing within an evolving software landscape 346 9.2. Case study 349 9.2.1. Practical reasons for the use of open-source components 349 9.2.2. Epistemic engagement with open source 352 9.2.3. Contribution to the design space through promise and validation 365 9.2.4. Infrastructural implosion 374 9.3. Discussion 378 9.3.1. What is Sakai 3? 378 9.3.2. Infrastructural implosion as an alternative evolutionary account of technology 381 CHAPTER 10. DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION 386 10.1. Discussion of the analysis of the case studies 386 viii 10.1.1. A distributed cognitive account of the domain-driven design of a software system in higher education 389 10.1.2. Replacing social distribution of interpretations with the social distribution of cognitive process 398 10.1.3. Social-technical imagination within a distributed cognitive process of design 401 10.1.4. Reinserting conceptual construction within social theory 404 10.2. Limitations 408 10.3. Contributions 412 10.4. Implications for further work and practice 413 References 417 ix LIST OF TABLES Table 3.1: Overview of content in the core corpus ........................................................... 53 Table 3.2: Example of data display created for a Confluence page .................................. 59 Table 3.3: Example of data display created for the same Confluence page ..................... 60 Table 3.4: Excerpt of the timeline used for keeping track of the evolution of S/OAE ..... 62 Table 4.1: Overview of the social-organizational ecosystem of the Sakai community .... 74 Table 4.2: Overview of projects related to Sakai 3 ........................................................... 84 Table 6.1: Professional methods in Sakai 3 projects ...................................................... 175 Table 7.1: Overview of the server team's account of epistemic reasons behind Sakai OAE's challenges ............................................................................................................ 229 Table 8.1: Examples of perspectives given as columns for describing group types in the spreadsheet ...................................................................................................................... 297 Table 8.2: Selection of entries from the Group types matrix spreadsheet ...................... 299 Table 8.3: Overview of domains covered by the contextual collections ........................ 316 Table 8.4: Epistemic grounding of contextual scenarios in terms of educational context and software .................................................................................................................... 330 x LIST OF FIGURES Figure 4.1: Tools within Sakai 2’s portal architecture ...................................................... 90 Figure 4.2: The Announcements Widget, relying on Sakai 2 data, is set in parallel with other web-based widgets on a user’s desktop ................................................................... 91 Figure 4.3: The division of labor between Java and UX programmer in a presentation by Ian Boston about the widget approach. ............................................................................. 92 Figure 5.1: My diagram of the widget model as a context of functionality and content 120 Figure 5.2: A comparison of Sakai 2’s course site with a dashboard-type page, based on my diagrams .................................................................................................................... 120 Figure 5.3: A chart with a conceptual
Recommended publications
  • Distributed Cognition As the Basis for Adaptation and Homeostasis In
    DISTRIBUTEDCOGNITIONASTHEBASISFORADAPTATION ANDHOMEOSTASISINROBOTS james henry stovold Doctor of Philosophy University of York Computer Science July 2016 ABSTRACT Many researchers approach the problem of building autonomous systems by looking to biology for inspiration. This has given rise to a wide-range of artificial systems mimicking their biological counterparts—artificial neural networks, artificial endocrine systems, and artificial musculoskeletal systems are prime examples. While these systems are succinct and work well in isolation, they can become cumbersome and complicated when combined to perform more complex tasks. Autonomous behaviour is one such complex task. This thesis considers autonomy as the complex behaviour it is, and proposes a bottom-up approach to developing autonomous beha- viour from cognition. This consists of investigating how cognition can provide new approaches to the current limitations of swarm systems, and using this as the basis for one type of autonomous behaviour: artificial homeostasis. Distributed cognition, a form of emergent cognition, is most often described in terms of the immune system and social insects. By taking inspiration from distributed cognition, this thesis details the development of novel algorithms for cognitive decision-making and emergent identity in leaderless, homogenous swarms. Artificial homeostasis is provided to a robot through an architecture that combines the cognitive decision-making algorithm with a simple associative memory. This architecture is used to demonstrate how a simple architecture can endow a robot with the capacity to adapt to an unseen environment, and use that information to proactively seek out what it needs from the environment in order to maintain its internal state. iii CONTENTS Abstract iii Table of Contents v List of Figures viii List of Tables xi List of Code Listings xii Acknowledgements xiii Declaration xv 1 introduction 1 1.1 Motivation .
    [Show full text]
  • GB-Human Computation Handbook
    From Human Computation to the Global Brain: the self-organization of distributed intelligence Francis Heylighen Global Brain Institute Vrije Universiteit Brussel Introduction The present chapter wishes to investigate the wider context of human computation, viewing it as merely one approach within the broad domain of distributed human-computer symbiosis. The multifarious developments in the “social” Internet have shown the great potential of large-scale col- laborative systems that involve both people and the various information and communication technologies (ICT) that process, store and distribute data. Here, I wish to explore this development in the broadest sense, as the self-organization of a distributed intelligence system at the planetary level—a phenomenon that has been called the “global brain”. To get there, I will first define and illustrate the fundamental concept of distributed intelligence. Then I will review how such an intelligent net- work emerges and grows through a combination of self-organization and design. Finally, I will sketch some potential applications of the anticipated global brain. Human-computer complementarity The rationale for human computation is that people have certain intrinsic skills that are difficult to reproduce in computer programs. A computation system that requires those skills must therefore include people as informa- tion-processing agents. Thus, in human computation, people and comput- ers are supposed to work together synergetically, the one complementing the other. The reason for this complementarity lies in the fact that humans and com- puters process information in very different ways. Computers excel at ac- curately storing and retrieving discrete items, such as numbers or strings of characters.
    [Show full text]
  • Supported Collaborative Learning at the Workplace
    Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning Series Sean P. Goggins Isa Jahnke Volker Wulf Editors Computer- Supported Collaborative Learning at the Workplace CSCL@Work Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning at the Workplace COMPUTER-SUPPORTED COLLABORATIVE LEARNING SERIES Series Editors: Christopher Hoadley, New York University, New York, USA Naomi Miyake, Chukyo University, Aichi, Japan Editorial Board: Michael Baker, CNRS & Université Lumière Lyon, France Carl Bereiter, Ontario Institute for Studies in Education, Canada Yrjö Engeström, University of Helsinki, Finland Gerhard Fischer, University of Colorado, U.S.A. H. Ulrich Hoppe, University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany Timothy Koschmann, Southern Illinois University, U.S.A. Claire O’Malley, University of Nottingham, U.K. Roy Pea, SRI International, U.S.A. Clotilde Pontecorovo, University ‘La Sapienza’, Italy Jeremy Roschelle, SRI International, U.S.A. Daniel Suthers, University of Hawaii, U.S.A. The Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning Book Series is for people working in the CSCL fi eld. The scope of the series extends to ‘collaborative learning’ in its broadest sense; the term is used for situations ranging from two individuals performing a task together, during a short period of time, to groups of 200 students following the same course and interacting via electronic mail. This variety also concerns the computational tools used in learning: elaborated graphical whiteboards support peer interaction, while more rudimentary textbased discussion forums are used for large group interaction. The series will integrate issues related to CSCL such as collaborative problem solving, collaborative learning without computers, negotiation patterns outside collaborative tasks, and many other relevant topics. It will also cover computational issues such as models, algorithms or architectures which support innovative functions relevant to CSCL systems.
    [Show full text]
  • Human Enhancement Technologies and Our Merger with Machines
    Human Enhancement and Technologies Our Merger with Machines Human • Woodrow Barfield and Blodgett-Ford Sayoko Enhancement Technologies and Our Merger with Machines Edited by Woodrow Barfield and Sayoko Blodgett-Ford Printed Edition of the Special Issue Published in Philosophies www.mdpi.com/journal/philosophies Human Enhancement Technologies and Our Merger with Machines Human Enhancement Technologies and Our Merger with Machines Editors Woodrow Barfield Sayoko Blodgett-Ford MDPI • Basel • Beijing • Wuhan • Barcelona • Belgrade • Manchester • Tokyo • Cluj • Tianjin Editors Woodrow Barfield Sayoko Blodgett-Ford Visiting Professor, University of Turin Boston College Law School Affiliate, Whitaker Institute, NUI, Galway USA Editorial Office MDPI St. Alban-Anlage 66 4052 Basel, Switzerland This is a reprint of articles from the Special Issue published online in the open access journal Philosophies (ISSN 2409-9287) (available at: https://www.mdpi.com/journal/philosophies/special issues/human enhancement technologies). For citation purposes, cite each article independently as indicated on the article page online and as indicated below: LastName, A.A.; LastName, B.B.; LastName, C.C. Article Title. Journal Name Year, Volume Number, Page Range. ISBN 978-3-0365-0904-4 (Hbk) ISBN 978-3-0365-0905-1 (PDF) Cover image courtesy of N. M. Ford. © 2021 by the authors. Articles in this book are Open Access and distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license, which allows users to download, copy and build upon published articles, as long as the author and publisher are properly credited, which ensures maximum dissemination and a wider impact of our publications. The book as a whole is distributed by MDPI under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons license CC BY-NC-ND.
    [Show full text]
  • Distributed Cognition in Classical Antiquity Reveals Diverse Notions of Distributed Cognition in the Early Greek and Roman Worlds
    Edited by Miranda Anderson, THE EDINBURGH HISTORY OF DISTRIBUTED COGNITION Douglas Cairns and THE EDINBURGH HISTORY OF DISTRIBUTED COGNITION Mark Sprevak ‘Once we look for it, distributed cognition is ubiquitous in classical antiquity. This book is a fascinating examination of the tools that made thinking easier and of the complex boundaries between the individual mind and the group.’ Ruth Scodel, University of Michigan Distributed Cognition in Classical Antiquity Reveals diverse notions of distributed cognition in the early Greek and Roman worlds This collection brings together eleven essays by international specialists in classical antiquity and provides a general and a period-specific introduction to distributed cognition and the cognitive humanities. The essays look at the ways in which cognition is explicitly or implicitly conceived of as distributed across brain, body and world in Greek and Roman technology, science and medicine, material culture, philosophy and literary studies. This exploratory work will be valuable across the humanities as it reveals the historical foundations of our theoretical and practical attempts to comprehend and optimise the distributed nature of human cognition. Miranda Anderson is a Research Fellow at the University of Edinburgh. Douglas Cairns is Professor of Classics in the University of Edinburgh. Mark Sprevak is Senior Lecturer in Philosophy at the University of Edinburgh. Distributed Cognition in Classical Antiquity Cover image: © János Kass, etching made for the illustration of Madách Imre’s Az ember tragédiája (Tragedy of Man), Egypt Cover design: Bekah Mackenzie and Stuart Dalziel Edited by Miranda Anderson, Douglas Cairns and Mark Sprevak 2 Distributed Cognition in Classical Antiquity Distributed Cognition in Classical Antiquity Edited by Miranda Anderson, Douglas Cairns and Mark Sprevak Edinburgh University Press is one of the leading university presses in the UK.
    [Show full text]
  • Distributed Cognitions GENERAL EDITORS: ROY PEA Psychological and Educational JOHN SEELY BROWN Considerations
    Learning in doing: Social, cognitive, and computational perspectives Distributed cognitions GENERAL EDITORS: ROY PEA Psychological and educational JOHN SEELY BROWN considerations The construction zone: Working for cognitive change in school Denis Newman, Peg Grrfin, and Michael Cole Plans and situated actions: The problem of human-machine Edited by interaction Lucy Suchman GAVRIEL SALOMON Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation University of Ha$, Israel Jean Lave and Etienne Wenger Street mathematics and school mathematics Terezinha Nunes, Anahria Dim Schliemann, and David William Carraher Understanding practice: Perspectives on activity and context Seth Chaiklin andJean Lave (editors) I943 CAMBRIDGE UNIVERSITY PRESS Published by the Press Syndicate of the University of Cambridge The Pin Building, Trumpington Street, Cambridge CB2 1RI' 40 West 20th Street, New York, NY 10011-4211, USA Contents 10 Stamford Road, Oakleigh, Melbourne 3166, Australia 0 Cambridge Universify Press 1993 First phlished 1993 Printed in the United States of America Library of Conqcsr Catalo@n~-in-PublicdionData Disaibuted cognitions : psychological and educational considerations I / edited by Gavriel Salomon. List of contribufon page vii I p. cm. - (Learning in doing) Includes index. Series foreword ix ISBN 0-521-41406-7 (hard) xi 1. Cognition and culture. 2. Knowledge, Sociology of. 3. Cognition - Social aspects. 4. Learning. Psychology of - Social aswcts. I. Salomon. Gavriel. 11. Series. 1 A cultural-historical approach to distributed 92-41220 cognition CIP MICHAEL COLE AND YRJO ENGESTR~M A catalog record for this book is available from the British Library. 2 Practices of distributed intelligence and designs for education ISBN 0-521-41406-7 hardback ROY D. PEA 3 Person-plus: a distributed view of thinking and learning D.
    [Show full text]
  • The Emergence of Distributed Cognition: a Conceptual Framework
    submitted for Proceedings of Collective Intentionality IV, Siena (Italy), to be published as a special issue of Cognitive Systems Research The Emergence of Distributed Cognition: a conceptual framework Francis HEYLIGHEN, Margeret HEATH and Frank VAN OVERWALLE Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Pleinlaan 2, B-1050 Brussels, Belgium {fheyligh, mheath, fjvoverw}@vub.ac.be ABSTRACT: We propose a first step in the development of an integrated theory of the emergence of distributed cognition/extended mind. Distributed cognition is seen as the confluence of collective intelligence and “situatedness”, or the extension of cognitive processes into the physical environment. The framework is based on five fundamental assumptions: 1) groups of agents self-organize to form a differentiated, coordinated system, adapted to its environment, 2) the system co-opts external media for internal propagation of information, 3) the resulting distributed cognitive system can be modelled as a learning, connectionist network, 4) information in the network is transmitted selectively, 5) novel knowledge emerges through non-linear, recurrent interactions. The implication for collective intentionality is that such a self-organizing agent collective can develop “mental content” that is not reducible to individual cognitions. Extended Mind: collective intelligence and distributed cognition From a cybernetic perspective [Heylighen & Joslyn, 2001], a cognitive system cannot be understood as a discrete collection of beliefs, procedures, and/or modules. Cognition is a continuously evolving process which relates present perceptions via internal states to potential further perceptions and actions. It thus allows an agent to anticipate what may happen, adapt to changes in its environment, and moreover effect changes upon its environment [Kirsch & Maglio, 1994].
    [Show full text]
  • The Extended Mind
    2015 48th Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences New frontiers for Crowdsourcing: the Extended Mind Roger M. Whitaker, Martin Chorley, Stuart M. Allen School of Computer Science and Informatics, Cardiff University 5 The Parade, Roath, Cardiff, CF24 3AA, UK {r.m.whitaker, m.j.chorley, s.m.allen}@cs.cardiff.ac.uk Abstract well-established form of collective intelligence, that has emerged through a number of successful commericial We introduce the concept of extended mind crowd- platforms and become a viable way to conduct survey- sourcing (EMC) which capitalises on the way in which based research [2]. humans naturally extend their cognition into the envi- As technology is developing, new opportunities and ronment, using external objects such as smartphones applications for crowdsourcing are emerging. In par- and applications to augment their mental capacity. ticular discovery of collective human behaviour and This phenomenon means that human computation is inherent culture is becoming possible. Recent exam- embedded in data and devices, representing a new way ples include the characterisation of international eat- through which human cognition can be accessed for col- ing habits [3], the grand vision of the FuturICT initia- lective discoveries. We relate EMC to existing socio- tive [4] and discovery of correlations between personal- logical and psychological concepts and argue that it lies ity and decisions in the places that we visit [5]. These at the intersection of human computation, social com- developments are occurring hand-in-hand with the pro- puting and crowdsourcing. gression of technologies capable of capturing more and EMC is a way in which new problems and discov- more of our individual activity and cognition.
    [Show full text]
  • Fafnir – Nordic Journal of Science Fiction and Fantasy Research Journal.Finfar.Org
    ISSN: 2342-2009 Fafnir vol 1, iss 2, pages 23–40 Fafnir – Nordic Journal of Science Fiction and Fantasy Research journal.finfar.org Intuitive Technologies. Models of Posthuman Subjectivity in Simon Ings' Hot Head and Hotwire Kaisa Kortekallio Abstract: This article analyzes two novels by the British writer Simon Ings, Hot Head (1992) and Hotwire (1995), from perspectives provided by second-order systems theory, philosophy of neuroscience and posthumanist philosophy. In Ings' cyberpunk fiction, the use of a particular novum, a programmable cerebral tissue called “datafat”, enables elaborate experimentation on different theories of mind and matter. Due to this experimentation, Ings’ work is able to convey a conception of cognition as an emergent effect produced in material processes that are both human and non-human. Ings’ work asserts the human subject as a complex system in a complex technological ecology and, consequentially, presents us with a model for subjectivity that might be called “posthuman”. Keywords: Simon Ings, Hot Head, Hotwire, posthuman subjectivity, posthumanism, philosophy of neuroscience in literature, complex systems in literature Biography and contact info: Kaisa Kortekallio is a doctoral student in the Doctoral Programme for Philosophy, Arts and Society at the University of Helsinki. Kortekallio holds a Master’s degree in General Literature. “I know that the molecules in my body are traceable to phenomena in the cosmos. That excites me. That makes me want to grab people on the street and say: ‘Have you HEARD THIS?” – astrophysicist Neil deGrasse Tyson in a talk given at the Beyond Belief Conference, 2006 There is profound reconfiguration going on in scientifically oriented minds – reconfiguration of the models of human existence and practice.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognition, Distributed
    Cognition, Distributed Cognition, Distributed 1. Mind in Society For many people, distributed cognition means cog- Like all other branches of cognitive science, distributed nitive processes that are distributed across the mem- cognition seeks to understand the organization of bers of a social group (Salomon 1993). The fun- cognitive systems. Like most of cognitive science, it damental question here is how the cognitive processes takes cognitive processes to be those that are involved we normally associate with an individual mind can be in memory, decision making, inference, reasoning, implemented in a group of individuals? A wide range learning, and so on. Also following mainstream of disciplines in the social sciences has explored this cognitive science, it characterizes cognitive processes question. in terms of the propagation and transformation of Treating memory as a socially distributed cognitive representations. function has a long history in sociology and anthro- What distinguishes distributed cognition from other pology. Durkheim, and his students, especially approaches is the commitment to two related theo- Halbwachs (1925), maintained that memory could not retical principles. The first concerns the boundaries of even be coherently discussed as a property of an the unit of analysis for cognition. While boundaries isolated individual. Roberts (1964) proposed that are often a matter of tradition in a field, there are some social organization could be read as a sort of archi- general rules one can follow. Bateson (1972) says one tecture of cognition at the community level. He should bound the unit so that things are not left characterized the cognitive properties of a society (its inexplicable.
    [Show full text]
  • Music and the Transhuman Ear: Ultrasonics, Material Bodies, and the Limits of Sensation
    Music and the Transhuman Ear: Ultrasonics, Material Bodies, and the Limits of Sensation “We favor morphological freedom – the right to modify and enhance one’s body, cognition and emotions.” (Transhumanist declaration, 2012)1 “I am apt to think, if we knew what it was to be an angel for one hour, we should return to this world, though it were to sit on the brightest throne in it, with vastly more loathing and reluctance than we would now descend into a loathsome dungeon or sepulchre.” (George Berkeley, 1732)2 PART I The materialist turn When Voyager probes 1 and 2 left the earth’s orbit in 1977, the committee tasked with assembling cultural artifacts followed scientist Lewis Thomas’ advice to include recordings of J. S. Bach among the many musical samples. This, it seems, was the best way to open a conversation with an unknown non-human interlocutor, wherein exchanges may be spaced hundreds of years apart. Many styles and genres were committed to the golden discs, but Thomas worried that by broadcasting Bach, the human race might be guilty of bragging to aliens: “we would be bragging, of course,” he admitted in 1972, “but it is surely excusable to put the best possible face on at the beginning of such an acquaintance.”3 His statement makes telling assumptions. To take one example, the three-voice C-major fugue Glenn Gould recorded (WTC, Book II) matches humans’ ability at auditory streaming, which, as David Huron has shown, tends to max out at around three streams, after which “confusions [over contrapuntal lines] become commonplace.”4 It would seem that part of the effect of Bach’s 1 Various, “Transhumanist Declaration,” The Transhumanist Reader, ed.
    [Show full text]
  • CSCL Proceedings Bleed Front 2
    Social Interaction, Constructivism and their Application within (CS)CL Theories Christian Hartmann, Jennifer Charlotte Angersbach and Nikol Rummel [email protected], [email protected], [email protected] Institute of Educational Research, Ruhr-Universität Bochum Abstract: We argue that the diversity of theories underlying CSCL research and the dispute concerning the distinction between the terms cooperation and collaboration may be explained (at least in part) by the variability of assumptions concerning the role of social interaction for individual learning. In this paper we identify different roles ascribed to social interaction in theories of learning and attempt to link them to different forms of constructivism in order to categorize theoretical conceptualizations of (CS)CL. This categorization may provide a starting point for developing a framework that enables consolidating various theoretical points of view. Keywords: cooperation, collaboration, constructivism, social interaction, learning theories, theoretical framework Introduction The field of Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning (CSCL) is influenced by several scientific disciplines (e.g. psychology, sociology, philosophy, computer science). Therefore CSCL research builds upon a variety of different theoretical points of view (Stahl, 2013). The phenomena that lie at the heart of CSCL research comprise social interaction and learning in the context of information technology (Ludvigsen & Mørch, 2010). To date, no single, overarching theory or paradigm is able to explain the relationship between those terms (or concepts) comprehensively (Goldbeck & El-Moslimany, 2013). We argue that a potential reason for this may be that there is no clear and universally valid definition of the concepts social interaction and learning. Some approaches appear to be compatible, while others seem incommensurable as they diverge concerning central assumptions concerning the nature of human cognition and social interaction.
    [Show full text]