Social Comparisons of Subjective Well-Being

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Social Comparisons of Subjective Well-Being Is the grass always greener on the other side? Social comparisons of subjective well-being THOMAS GOETZ1, CHRISTOPH EHRET2, SIMONEJULLIEN1, & NATHAN C. HALL3 I University 0/ Munich, Germany, 2University 0/ Regensburg, Germany, and 3University o/California, Irvine, USA Abstract This study investigates subjective well being (SWB) among residents of Munich (n 380) and Venice (n 545) with respect to their individual SWB and their judgments of SWB for residents of their own city and those living in the other city. Our results indicate that egocentrism rather than striving for self enhancement guided people's judgments of SWB. For people with low individual SWB, a below average effect was found, whereas for people with high individual SWB, a better than average effect emerged. Also in line with the egocentrism approach, judgments of individual SWB were positively related to the judgments of SWB for residents of their own city, but unrelated to SWB of those living in the other city. Implications for future research are discussed. Keywords: Subjective well being, satisfaction with life, social compan'sons Introduction abilities of other groups (outgroups; Abrams & Hogg, 1988). This phenomenon is called the ingroup Many studies have shown that most people believe bias (Mullen, Brown, & Smith, 1992) and is a central they are better than average (see Alicke & Govorun, component of Social Identity Theory developed 2005; Armor & Taylor, 1998; Krueger, 1998; Taylor by Tajfel and Turner (1979, 1986; see Rubin & Brown, 1988, 1994, for reviews). This effect has & Hewstone, 1998, for a review on research on the been found in various domains: with respect to ingroup bias). driving ability (Svenson, 1981), social competencies While there are numerous findings that have (Alicke, Klotz, Breitenbecher, Yurak, & Vredenburg, confirmed the BTAE, some recent studies reported 1995), attractiveness (Gabriel, Critelli, & Ee, 1994), a below-average effect. Kruger (1999), for example, and intelligence (Stankov & Crawford, 1997). The found below-average effects in participants' com­ tendency to overestimate one's own abilities has parative ability judgments for difficult tasks. These been investigated primarily in the domain of social and other empirical findings have led to a rethinking psychology and has often been referred to as the of the "classical" explanations for the BTAE, "better-than-average effect" (BTAE). Some studies which are motivational in nature, namely the striving have also called this relative overestimation of one's for self-enhancement (Alicke & Govorun, 2005). own attributes as the "above-average effect," "I am Recently, non-motivated accounts like egocentrism better than my group's average (IBTA) effect," have been discussed for explaining both the BTAE "illusory superiority," the "sense of relative superi­ and the below-average effect (Chambers & ority," the "leniency error," the "superior con­ Windschitl, 2004; Kruger, 1999). formity of the self," or the "primus inter pares" (first Although the BTAE has often been demonstrated among equals) effect (see Hoorens, 1993, for a with respect to numerous constructs, few studies review). have examined the potential influence of the BTAE The BTAE has also been found with respect to on affective variables (see Smith, 1993, 1999). individuals' assessments of group abilities. That is, Existing research has investigated the BTAE for people do not only overestimate their individual affective constructs on either an individual level abilities in relation to the abilities of others, they also (e.g., contentment; Klar & Giladi, 1999; Lykken & overestimate the overall abilities of the groups to Tellegen, 1996; Schkade & Kahneman, 1998) or on which they belong (ingroups) in relation to the a group level (e.g., higher-order emotions such as Correspondence: Thomas Goetz, Department of Psychology, University of Munich, Leopoldstrasse 13, D 80802 Munich, Germany. Tel: 49 89 2180 5086. Fax: 49 89 2180 5250. Email: [email protected] muenchen.de 174 affection, admiration, pride, conceit, nostalgia, bolster their self-worth by engaging in social com­ remorse, and rancor; Plutchik, 1994; see also parison processes that result in personal superiority. Leyens et aI., 2000). Results of these studies Because the result of the BTAE is to perceive Qneself suggest that most people rate their emotional as "better than the average," it seems obvious that a experiences more favorably than those of the plausible motive for explaining this effect both at the average individual. individual and group levels is self-enhancement The present study aims to contribute to these (Alicke & Govorun, 2005; Hoorens, 1993). studies in examining the BTAE for judgments of subjective well-being (SWB), a construct explored mainly within the context of "positive psychology" Nonmotivated factors leading to biases in (see Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). However, social judgments we aimed to contribute to this research by further Other research suggests that motivational concerns differentiating people's judgments of SWB concern­ are not the exclusive or even most dominant source ing "the average individuaL" Specifically, we asked of the BTAE (Alicke & Govorun, 2005; Chambers, them to distinguish in their judgments of SWB Windschitl, & Suls, 2003), with recent empirical between ingroup and outgroup members. Thus, the studies showing results that are incongruent with present study incorporates recent research on social motivational accounts (for a review, see Chambers & comparisons and subjective well-being in examining Windschitl, 2004). Kruger (1999), for example, the implications of the BTAE with respect to how found below-average effects in participants' com­ people evaluate their individual SWB, the SWB of parative ability judgments for difficult tasks, a finding other inhabitants of their city (ingroup), and the that is incongruent with the motive of self-enhance­ SWB of inhabitants of another city (outgroup). ment. Thus, recent research has focused increasingly A similar study conducted by Schkade and on nonmotivated accounts to explain biases that Kahneman (1998) asked US residents living in the cannot be adequately captured by motivated expla­ midwest and southern California to judge satisfaction nations. Nonmotivated accounts "locate the source with life for either themselves or someone similar to of the biases within aspects of the judgment process themselves in one of the two regions. Thus, each and information-processing limitations rather than in person's evaluation in Schkade and Kahneman's self-enhancement motives or other related motives study referred to only one target individual. In such as mood maintenance" (Chambers & contrast to the Schkade and Kahneman investiga­ Windschitl, 2004, p. 813). However, as Alicke tion, our study assessed peoples' judgments of SWB (1985) points out, biases due to nonmotivated with respect to three different target individuals: aspects could also have unintended self-enhancing themselves, ingroup members (inhabitants of their consequences. own city), and outgroup members (inhabitants of The most prominent nonmotivated explanation another city). for the BTAE is egocentrism (Ali eke & Govorun, 2005; Moore & Kim, 2003). Differing from other nonmotivated accounts, egocentrism is assumed to Motivated factors leading to biases in be involved when people's comparative evaluations social judgments are assessed using indirect methods, in which Traditionally, the BTAE has been assumed "to be absolute judgments concerning oneself and others mediated by a motivation for people to see them­ are made using separate scales. Other prominent selves or depict themselves in the best possible light" nonmotivated accounts (e.g., focalism) appear to be (Chambers & Windschitl, 2004, p. 813). Classic involved when using a direct method of assessment research by James (1890), Allport (1937), and (Chambers & Windschitl, 2004), that is, when Horney (1937) postulated that a central motive of people are asked to directly compare themselves human beings is self-enhancement. This motive to with others as part of a single scale (with "average" as enhance one's self-worth influences not only self­ the midpoint). Because we used the indirect method worth-enhancing behaviors (e.g., working hard in. for the comparative assessment of SWB, egocentrism order to achieve good results) but also how people was assumed to be the most relevant potential compare their own abilities with the abilities of nonmotivated explanation to be evaluated in this others. As such, self-enhancement theory represents study. a theoretical elaboration of Festinger's (1954) theory According to the egocentrism approach, judg­ of social comparison processes. It suggests that ments related to oneself and to others are inherently although people may seek to accurately evaluate egocentric, in that people focus primarily on their their own abilities by comparing themselves with own skills and behaviors and fail to adequately similar others, individuals are also motivated . to consider the skills and behaviors of others. 175 Several mechanisms are assumed to mediate the In sum, for high levels of individual SWB, both effects of egocentric judgments. For example, when the motivated account (self-enhancement) and the people have greater access to information concerning nonmotivated account (egocentrism) predict themselves rather than others, they often give the BTAE. However, for people with low individual more attention to themselves and use different SWB,
Recommended publications
  • Dunning–Kruger Effect - Wikipedia, the Free Encyclopedia
    Dunning–Kruger effect - Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Dunning–Kruger_effect Dunning–Kruger effect From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia The Dunning–Kruger effect is a cognitive bias in which unskilled individuals suffer from illusory superiority, mistakenly rating their ability much higher than average. This bias is attributed to a metacognitive inability of the unskilled to recognize their mistakes.[1] Actual competence may weaken self-confidence, as competent individuals may falsely assume that others have an equivalent understanding. David Dunning and Justin Kruger of Cornell University conclude, "the miscalibration of the incompetent stems from an error about the self, whereas the miscalibration of the highly competent stems from an error about others".[2] Contents 1 Proposal 2 Supporting studies 3 Awards 4 Historical references 5 See also 6 References Proposal The phenomenon was first tested in a series of experiments published in 1999 by David Dunning and Justin Kruger of the Department of Psychology, Cornell University.[2][3] They noted earlier studies suggesting that ignorance of standards of performance is behind a great deal of incompetence. This pattern was seen in studies of skills as diverse as reading comprehension, operating a motor vehicle, and playing chess or tennis. Dunning and Kruger proposed that, for a given skill, incompetent people will: 1. tend to overestimate their own level of skill; 2. fail to recognize genuine skill in others; 3. fail to recognize the extremity of their inadequacy; 4. recognize and acknowledge their own previous lack of skill, if they are exposed to training for that skill. Dunning has since drawn an analogy ("the anosognosia of everyday life")[1][4] with a condition in which a person who suffers a physical disability because of brain injury seems unaware of or denies the existence of the disability, even for dramatic impairments such as blindness or paralysis.
    [Show full text]
  • Infographic I.10
    The Digital Health Revolution: Leaving No One Behind The global AI in healthcare market is growing fast, with an expected increase from $4.9 billion in 2020 to $45.2 billion by 2026. There are new solutions introduced every day that address all areas: from clinical care and diagnosis, to remote patient monitoring to EHR support, and beyond. But, AI is still relatively new to the industry, and it can be difficult to determine which solutions can actually make a difference in care delivery and business operations. 59 Jan 2021 % of Americans believe returning Jan-June 2019 to pre-coronavirus life poses a risk to health and well being. 11 41 % % ...expect it will take at least 6 The pandemic has greatly increased the 65 months before things get number of US adults reporting depression % back to normal (updated April and/or anxiety.5 2021).4 Up to of consumers now interested in telehealth going forward. $250B 76 57% of providers view telehealth more of current US healthcare spend % favorably than they did before COVID-19.7 could potentially be virtualized.6 The dramatic increase in of Medicare primary care visits the conducted through 90% $3.5T telehealth has shown longevity, with rates in annual U.S. health expenditures are for people with chronic and mental health conditions. since April 2020 0.1 43.5 leveling off % % Most of these can be prevented by simple around 30%.8 lifestyle changes and regular health screenings9 Feb. 2020 Apr. 2020 OCCAM’S RAZOR • CONJUNCTION FALLACY • DELMORE EFFECT • LAW OF TRIVIALITY • COGNITIVE FLUENCY • BELIEF BIAS • INFORMATION BIAS Digital health ecosystems are transforming• AMBIGUITY BIAS • STATUS medicineQUO BIAS • SOCIAL COMPARISONfrom BIASa rea• DECOYctive EFFECT • REACTANCEdiscipline, • REVERSE PSYCHOLOGY • SYSTEM JUSTIFICATION • BACKFIRE EFFECT • ENDOWMENT EFFECT • PROCESSING DIFFICULTY EFFECT • PSEUDOCERTAINTY EFFECT • DISPOSITION becoming precise, preventive,EFFECT • ZERO-RISK personalized, BIAS • UNIT BIAS • IKEA EFFECT and • LOSS AVERSION participatory.
    [Show full text]
  • Dunning–Kruger Effect – Why Once a Fool, Always a Fool
    Dunning–Kruger Effect – Why Once a Fool, Always a Fool David Lacko Psychologický ústav FFMU, Brno [email protected] The majority of people believe that they are better-than-average. In psychology, this phenomenon is called the illusory superiority. A specific form of illusory superiority is Dunning–Kruger’s effect which states that people overestimate their abilities based on their real abilities. The rule is that the less competent a person is, the more they overestimate themselves. And, on the other hand, able people have a tendency to underestimate themselves slightly. A considerable part of this phenomenon is also the fact that the less able people do not change their attitudes even after being confronted with the reality. The majority of people believe that they are better-than-average. Some sources (Boyd, 2014) mention that as much as 93 % of population consider themselves better than average, even though it is logical, that it is statistically impossible.In psychology, this phenomenon is called theillusory superiority, or theAbove Average Effect / Better than Average Effect. Out of interest, I will give you a few examples.The majority of people think that they are more intelligent and more attractive than the majority of others (Whittlestone, 2012), and on the scale from 1 to 10, where 10 is the highest better-than-average, they ascribe themselves number 7 (Ghose, 2013). 88 % of people think that they drive better-than-average (Svenson, 1981); even the majority of elderly people rank themselves among better-than-average drivers (Marottoli and Richardson, 1998). If you suppose that this is only the case in more stupid people, you are wrong.94% of professors assume that they are better-than-average in comparison to their colleagues (Cross, 1977).
    [Show full text]
  • John Collins, President, Forensic Foundations Group
    On Bias in Forensic Science National Commission on Forensic Science – May 12, 2014 56-year-old Vatsala Thakkar was a doctor in India but took a job as a convenience store cashier to help pay family expenses. She was stabbed to death outside her store trying to thwart a theft in November 2008. Bloody Footwear Impression Bloody Tire Impression What was the threat? 1. We failed to ask ourselves if this was a footwear impression. 2. The appearance of the impression combined with the investigator’s interpretation created prejudice. The accuracy of our analysis became threatened by our prejudice. Types of Cognitive Bias Available at: http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/List_of_cognitive_biases | Accessed on April 14, 2014 Anchoring or focalism Hindsight bias Pseudocertainty effect Illusory superiority Levels-of-processing effect Attentional bias Hostile media effect Reactance Ingroup bias List-length effect Availability heuristic Hot-hand fallacy Reactive devaluation Just-world phenomenon Misinformation effect Availability cascade Hyperbolic discounting Recency illusion Moral luck Modality effect Backfire effect Identifiable victim effect Restraint bias Naive cynicism Mood-congruent memory bias Bandwagon effect Illusion of control Rhyme as reason effect Naïve realism Next-in-line effect Base rate fallacy or base rate neglect Illusion of validity Risk compensation / Peltzman effect Outgroup homogeneity bias Part-list cueing effect Belief bias Illusory correlation Selective perception Projection bias Peak-end rule Bias blind spot Impact bias Semmelweis
    [Show full text]
  • Narcissists¬タル Negative Perception of Their Counterpart¬タルs
    Personality and Individual Differences 74 (2015) 196–201 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Personality and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Narcissists’ negative perception of their counterpart’s competence and benevolence and their own reduced trust in a negotiation context ⇑ Dejun Tony Kong Jepson School of Leadership Studies and Robins School of Business, University of Richmond, Richmond, VA 23173, USA article info abstract Article history: Despite its important role in interpersonal interactions, narcissism has rarely been examined in the con- Received 10 March 2014 text of negotiations in which individuals cooperate and compete with their counterpart. As negotiations Received in revised form 10 October 2014 occur frequently in daily life, empirical findings of the role of narcissism in negotiation settings can Accepted 11 October 2014 enhance the understanding of the functions of narcissism in common situations in which self- and Available online 11 November 2014 other-interests are in conflict. By analyzing 35 negotiation dyads’ within-dyad differences using multi- level analysis, I found that negotiators’ narcissism was negatively related to their perception of their Keywords: counterpart’s competence but was not significantly related to their individual economic gain (objective Narcissism negotiation performance), suggesting narcissists’ inflated agentic self or deflated perception of their Trust Competence counterpart’s competence. Additionally, narcissism was negatively related to trust,
    [Show full text]
  • Seeing What You Want to See: Priors for One's Own Actions Represent Exaggerated Expectations of Success
    ORIGINAL RESEARCH ARTICLE published: 27 June 2014 BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00232 Seeing what you want to see: priors for one’s own actions represent exaggerated expectations of success Noham Wolpe 1,2*, Daniel M. Wolpert 3† and James B. Rowe 1,2 † 1 Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK 2 Medical Research Council Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK 3 Computational and Biological Learning Laboratory, Department of Engineering, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Edited by: People perceive the consequences of their own actions differently to how they perceive Angela Roberts, University of other sensory events. A large body of psychology research has shown that people also Cambridge, UK consistently overrate their own performance relative to others, yet little is known about Reviewed by: how these “illusions of superiority” are normally maintained. Here we examined the visual James W. Moore, Goldsmiths, University of London, UK perception of the sensory consequences of self-generated and observed goal-directed Atsushi Sato, University of Toyama, actions. Across a series of visuomotor tasks, we found that the perception of the Japan sensory consequences of one’s own actions is more biased toward success relative *Correspondence: to the perception of observed actions. Using Bayesian models, we show that this bias Noham Wolpe, Department of could be explained by priors that represent exaggerated predictions of success. The Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Herchel Smith Building, degree of exaggeration of priors was unaffected by learning, but was correlated with Cambridge CB2 0SZ, UK individual differences in trait optimism. In contrast, when observing these actions, priors e-mail: [email protected] represented more accurate predictions of the actual performance.
    [Show full text]
  • A Review of Optimism Bias, Planning Fallacy, Sunk Cost Bias and Groupthink in Project Delivery and Organisational Decision Making
    Behavioural Insights Team A review of optimism bias, planning fallacy, sunk cost bias and groupthink in project delivery and organisational decision making Accompanying An Exploration of Behavioural Biases in Project Delivery at the Department for Transport Client: Department for Transport Date: July 2017 1 © Behavioural Insights Ltd Contents Introduction ....................................................................................................................................... 3 How we make decisions and judgements ........................................................................................ 3 Optimism bias and the planning fallacy .............................................................................................. 5 How prevalent is the planning fallacy? ............................................................................................ 5 Causes of optimism bias and the planning fallacy ........................................................................... 6 Self-serving biases..................................................................................................................... 6 Illusion of control ....................................................................................................................... 7 Confirmation bias ...................................................................................................................... 8 Perception of risk ......................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive/Neuroscience Neurophysiological
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2019.12.26.888511; this version posted December 28, 2019. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license. Proposed Journal section: Cognitive/Neuroscience Neurophysiological Correlates of the Dunning-Kruger Effect Reveal Contributions of Episodic Memory to Metacognitive Judgments of Illusory Superiority Alana Muller1,2, Lindsey Sirianni1, & Richard J. Addante1 1California State University – San Bernardino, 5500 University Parkway, San Bernardino, CA 92407 2University of Arizona, 1423 E University Blvd, Bldg. 67, Rm 223, Tucson, AZ 85719 Corresponding Author: Richard J. Addante, PhD California State University – San Bernardino Department of Psychology 5500 University Parkway San Bernardino, CA 92407 [email protected] Running title: Physiological Correlates of Dunning-Kruger Effect Total number of words: manuscript (11, 804 words); the Abstract (250 words) Keywords: Metacognition, EEG, ERP, recollection, familiarity bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2019.12.26.888511; this version posted December 28, 2019. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license. Abstract. The Dunning-Kruger Effect is a metacognitive phenomenon of illusory superiority in which individuals who perform poorly on a task believe they performed well, yet individuals who performed very well believe they under-performed.
    [Show full text]
  • An Exploration of Spiritual Superiority: the Paradox of Self‐Enhancement
    | DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.2721 | Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands - cal effect of boosting superiority feelings. It can thus operate like other self-enhance- Radboud University, P.O. Box 9104, 6500 ment tools and contribute to a contingent self-worth that depends on one's spiritual HE Nijmegen, The Netherlands. Email: [email protected] accomplishments. In three studies (N = 533, N = 2,223, N = 965), a brief measure of spiritual superiority showed good internal consistency and discriminant validity. As Anouk Visser, Behavior Change Group, predicted, it was distinctly related to spiritual contingency of self-worth, illustrat- Nijmegen, The Netherlands ing that the self-enhancement function of spirituality is similar to other contingency domains. It was correlated with self-esteem and, more strongly, with communal nar- were consistently higher among energetically trained participants than mindfulness trainees and were associated with supernatural overconfidence and self-ascribed spiritual guidance. Our results illustrate that the self-enhancement motive is power- ful and deeply ingrained so that it can hijack methods intended to transcend the ego and, instead, adopt them to its own service. contingent self-worth, mindfulness, narcissism, self-enhancement, spirituality | They may become ambitious in climbing the spiritual ladder, onto the next level, in much the same way as in academics, sports, or work. Ego is able to convert anything to its own use, even And they may feel superior to others who lack the spiritual
    [Show full text]
  • Illusion of Control 1 Illusion of Control
    Illusion of control 1 Illusion of control The illusion of control is the tendency for people to overestimate their ability to control events, for instance to feel that they control outcomes that they demonstrably have no influence over.[1] The effect was named by psychologist Ellen Langer and has been replicated in many different contexts.[2] It is thought to influence gambling behavior and belief in the paranormal.[3] Along with illusory superiority and optimism bias, the illusion of control is one of the positive illusions. The illusion is more common in familiar situations, and in situations where the person knows the desired outcome.[4] Feedback that emphasizes success rather than failure can increase the effect, while feedback that emphasizes failure can decrease or reverse the effect.[5] The illusion is weaker for depressed individuals and is stronger when individuals have an emotional need to control the outcome.[4] The illusion is strengthened by stressful and competitive situations, including financial trading.[6] Though people are likely to overestimate their control when the situations are heavily chance-determined, they also tend to underestimate their control when they actually have it, which runs contrary to some theories of the illusion and its adaptiveness.[7] The illusion might arise because people lack direct introspective insight into whether they are in control of events. This has been called the introspection illusion. Instead they may judge their degree of control by a process that is often unreliable. As a result, they see themselves as responsible for events when there is little or no causal link.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Biases in Fitness to Practise Decision-Making
    ADVICE ON BIASES IN FITNESS TO PRACTISE DECISION-MAKING IN ACCEPTED OUTCOME VERSUS PANEL MODELS FOR THE PROFESSIONAL STANDARDS AUTHORITY Prepared by: LESLIE CUTHBERT Date: 31 March 2021 Contents I. Introduction ................................................................................................. 2 Background .................................................................................................. 2 Purpose of the advice.................................................................................... 3 Summary of conclusions ................................................................................ 4 Documents considered .................................................................................. 4 Technical terms and explanations ................................................................... 5 Disclaimer .................................................................................................... 5 II. How biases might affect the quality of decision-making in the AO model, as compared to the panel model .............................................................................. 6 Understanding cognitive biases in fitness to practise decision-making ............... 6 Individual Decision Maker (e.g. a Case Examiner) in AO model ......................... 8 Multiple Case Examiners .............................................................................. 17 Group Decision Maker in a Panel model of 3 or more members ....................... 22 III. Assessment of the impact of these biases,
    [Show full text]
  • Mother-Daughter Relationships in Adulthood: Attachment, Self-Esteem and Illusory Superiority Vanencia Jacquia Lynch
    XULAneXUS Volume 9 | Issue 1 Article 6 12-1-2011 Mother-Daughter Relationships in Adulthood: Attachment, Self-Esteem and Illusory Superiority Vanencia Jacquia Lynch Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.xula.edu/xulanexus Recommended Citation Lynch, Vanencia Jacquia (2011) "Mother-Daughter Relationships in Adulthood: Attachment, Self-Esteem and Illusory Superiority," XULAneXUS: Vol. 9 : Iss. 1 , Article 6. Available at: https://digitalcommons.xula.edu/xulanexus/vol9/iss1/6 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by XULA Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in XULAneXUS by an authorized editor of XULA Digital Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Lynch: Mother-Daughter Relationships in Adulthood: Attachment, Self-Este ! Volume 9, Issue 1, December 2011. Research Manuscript. 55-60. ! <http://xulanexus.xula.edu/textpattern/index.php?id=126> ! Mother-Daughter Relationships in Adulthood: Attachment, Self Esteem and Illusory Superiority Vanencia Jacquia Lynch, Psychology, Pre- Medicine Faculty Mentor: Dr. Elizabeth Yost Hammer, Psychology and Center for the Advancement of Teaching Abstract Nature as well as nurture can influence the emotional and Vanencia Lynch is a Psychology cognitive growth of a person. Nurture includes the level of major with a minor in Business emotion being shown toward an individual, which is then Administration from Houston, reciprocated. One way that nurture is often measured is through TX. Upon graduating Xavier attachment levels. Research suggests that different forms of next Fall, Lynch plans to get her attachment often contribute to the lifestyle and morals of adults. masters in clinical psychology In the present study, attachment levels between mothers and and then continue on to obtain daughters were examined, in relation to self-esteem and illusory her Psy.D., specializing in superiority.
    [Show full text]