English Right Dislocation

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

English Right Dislocation PACLIC 29 English Right Dislocation Kohji Kamada Chiba University 1-33, Yayoicho, Inage Ward, Chiba-shi, Chiba, 263-8522 Japan [email protected]/[email protected] (2) *That they spoke to the janitor about that Abstract robbery yesterday is terrible, the cops . (Ross, 1986: 258) A number of researchers claim that the derivation of the Right Dislocation Construction (RDC) involves movement (e.g., However, there is a construction that violates the Chung, 2012, for Korean; Ott & de Vries, 2012, RRC but is still acceptable, as seen in (3). 2015, for Dutch and German; Tanaka, 2001 and Abe, 2004, for Japanese; Whitman, 2000, for (3) [That they spoke to the janitor about that English, Japanese, and Korean). However, the robbery yesterday] is terrible, I mean, the RDC in English does not obey movement cops. (Whitman, 2000: 450) constraints such as the Coordinate Structure Constraint and the Left Branch Condition; that The sentence in (3) differs from that in (2) only in is, there are acceptable sentences that seem to that it has I mean inserted between the preceding violate these movement constraints. This suggests that the derivation of the English RDC clause and the dislocated element. This suggests should not involve movement. The present that the derivation of the RDC should at least not 2 paper demonstrates that some syntactic involve rightward movement. Note that the properties of the English RDC can be explained relevant pronoun is not a “resumptive” pronoun instead through the interaction of independently that repairs an island violation; it would otherwise motivated parsing strategies with a licensing be difficult to account for the unacceptability of the condition for adjoined elements. example in (2), in which the pronoun seems to play no role in repairing the violation of the RRC. 3 Further acceptable examples that appear to 1 Introduction violate movement constraints exist, as in (4). The Right Dislocation Construction (RDC) is a construction in which a dislocated NP appearing in (4) a. I saw Mary and him downtown yesterday, sentence-final position refers to a pronoun, as your friend from Keokuk . observed in example (1), with the relevant pronoun (Ross, 1986: 260) in italics and the dislocated NP in boldface. b. I noticed his car in the driveway last night, your friend from Keokuk . (ibid .) (1) He is real smart, John . In (4), it is possible to connect the dislocated NPs As (2) shows, the dislocated NP cannot occur with him and his , respectively. If the dislocated NP outside the embedded clause that contains the in (4a) were extracted from the position occupied relevant pronoun. This seems to suggest that the by the pronoun him , a conjunct could be moved. dislocated NP is derived by movement, because a Likewise in (4b), if the dislocated NP were violation of a movement constraint—namely, the extracted from the position occupied by his , an Right Roof Constraint (RRC)—appears to be present (Ross, 1986: 179). 1 2 An example of the type in (3) was originally provided by Tsubomoto (1995), who argues against a movement analysis for the RDC and accounts for some of its properties in terms of information structure. 3 If movement were involved in the derivation of the RDC and 1 Another possibility is a violation of the Sentential Subject the relevant pronoun were a resumptive pronoun, the RRC Constraint. would be a condition on a representation. 221 29th Pacific Asia Conference on Language, Information and Computation pages 221 - 230 Shanghai, China, October 30 - November 1, 2015 Copyright 2015 by Kohji Kamada PACLIC 29 element could be moved out of the specifier demonstrate that it fails to account for several position of the NP. properties of the English RDC. Irrespective of whether an element moves Whitman (2000) follows Kayne (1994) in rightward or leftward, however, English observes claiming that a sentence like that in (1) is derived the Coordinate Structure Constraint (CSC) and the from the biclausal structure shown in (7), as in (8). Left Branch Condition (LBC), as shown in (5) and (6), respectively. (7) XP (5) a. * What sofa i will he put the chair between CP 1 XP some table and ti? (ibid .: 97) b. *I saw Mary and ti downtown yesterday, He is real smart [e] CP 2 your friend from Keokuk i. (ibid .: 260) John is real smart (6) a. * Whose i did you steal ti money? (adapted from Whitman, 2000: 452) (McCawley, 1998: 526) b. *I noticed ti car in the driveway last night, (8) [ CP1 He is real smart], John i, [ CP2 ti is real smart] your friend from Keokuk i. (Ross, 1986: 260) As (8) shows, John is left-adjoined/dislocated to If the derivation of the RDC involved rightward CP 2, and the remaining elements (i.e., the movement in any way, the examples in (4) would underlined parts) are deleted under an identity violate the movement constraints, resulting in condition, thereby generating (1). 5,6 unacceptability—contrary to the actual situation. According to Whitman (2000), the RRC effect Furthermore, the examples in (4) suggest that the displayed in (2) is explained as follows: As in (1), derivation of the RDC involves no rightward (2) is formed by first conjoining two clauses, and 4 movement. then, as shown in (9), the cops is extracted from This paper is structured as follows. In section 2, the sentential subject in CP 2 to adjoin to the left I argue that the derivation of the RDC involves no side of CP 2. This extraction, however, violates the movement, by pointing out empirical problems Sentential Subject Constraint, resulting in the RRC with the argument by Whitman (2000), who claims effect. that the derivation of the RDC in English involves the operation of deletion after leftward movement. (9) *[ CP1 That they spoke to the janitor about that In section 3, I first set out a number of robbery yesterday] is terrible, [the cops] i, [ CP2 independently motivated principles, such as [that ti spoke to the janitor about that robbery parsing principles and a licensing condition for yesterday] is terrible]. (Whitman, 2000: 458) adjoined elements, and then I demonstrate that the interaction of the licensing condition with these However, the analysis above is empirically principles can account for the cases with which problematic, because (3) would be excluded in the movement analyses fail to cope. Section 4 concludes the paper. 5 Whitman (2000) claims that his analysis is also applicable to the RDC in Japanese and Korean. Similar proposals are made 2 Problems with a Biclausal + Deletion by, e.g., Chung (2012) for Korean, Ott and de Vries (2012, Analysis 2015) for Dutch and German, and Endo (1996), Tanaka (2001) and Abe (2003) for Japanese. What these proposals have in In the previous section, I discussed certain common is that the RDC has a biclausal structure and empirical problems with rightward movement undergoes left-adjoinment to the second clause before deletion analyses. In this section, I take up Whitman (2000) under an identity condition. Hence, the application of these approaches to English RDCs will face similar sorts of as an example of leftward movement analyses, and empirical problems to those Whitman (2000) does. 6 The identity condition is not clearly defined in Whitman (2000). Incidentally, Ott and de Vries (2012) follow Merchant 4 It is assumed that the CSC and the LBC are regarded as (2001) in assuming that “the deleted domain in CP 2 and its conditions on movement rather than on representations. antecedent domain in CP 1 must be semantically equivalent….” 222 PACLIC 29 7 same way as (2) is. Furthermore, the analysis is (12) *The one who [topic-comment structure i not adequate to account for the examples in (4). doesn’t understand ti] is me. That is, your friend from Keokuk (’s ) would be (adapted from Gundel, 1988: 151) extracted from the respective second clauses [ I saw Mary and your friend from Keokuk downtown Hence, the structure in (11b) would not be yesterday ] and [I noticed your friend from appropriate either. Keokuk’s car in the driveway last night ]. These The biclausal + deletion analysis might claim extractions, however, violate the CSC and the LBC, that the internal structure of the embedded clause as discussed in section 1. Thus, the biclausal + in (10) is different from that of the relative clause deletion analysis also cannot account for the in a sentence like (12). If so, the analysis would be acceptability of the examples in (4) (see footnote unable to cope with an unacceptable example such 4). 8 as (13), in which the embedded clause appears to Moreover, the biclausal + deletion analysis faces have the same structure as that in (10). another empirical problem. (13) *Bill gave the girl who [ate it , the potato (10) The girl who ate it , the potato salad , was salad ], a dollar. rushed to the hospital. 9 (Gundel, 1988: 132) Thus, biclausal + deletion analyses such as that of The example in (10) shows that the RDC is Whitman (2000) have empirical problems. On the possible inside an embedded clause. 10 There are at basis of the discussion in sections 1 and 2 here, it least two possible ways for (10) to be derived seems safe to say that the derivation of the English under the analysis in question. The relevant RDC does not involve movement (i.e., that the possible structures corresponding to that in (8) RDC is base-generated). before deletion takes place would be those in (11), with the content of CP 1 in (11a) ignored. 3 A Base-Generation Analysis 3.1 Parsing strategies (11) a. [CP1 … ], the potato salad i [CP2 the girl who ate ti was rushed to the hospital] Concerning a parsing strategy, I follow Pritchett b.
Recommended publications
  • Contrasting Contrastive Left-Dislocation Explications Marcel Den Dikken Bala´Zs Sura´Nyi
    Contrasting Contrastive Left-Dislocation Explications Marcel den Dikken Bala´zs Sura´nyi Of the three logically possible approaches to contrastive left-disloca- tion (CLD) (base-generation cum deep anaphora; movement cum sur- face anaphora; elliptical clausal juxtaposition cum resumption), two are represented prominently in the recent literature. Ott’s (2014) ac- count treats CLD uniformly in terms of clausal juxtaposition, the first clause being stripped down to its contrastive topic via an ellipsis opera- tion said to be akin to sluicing. He argues that this analysis is superior to Grohmann’s (2003) approach, featuring movement within a single prolific domain and late spell-out of a resumptive element. Using data mainly from Hungarian and Dutch, we reveal problems for Ott’s biclausal account that undermine its apparent conceptual appeal and compromise its descriptive accuracy. We show that the ellipsis oper- ation required is sui generis, that the approach fails to assimilate the crosslinguistic variation attested in the availability of multiple CLD to known cases of parametric variation in the left periphery, and that it undergenerates in several empirical domains, including P-stranding and ‘‘floated’’ arguments. Grohmann’s movement-cum- surface-anaphora analysis as it stands also cannot handle all these data, but it can be fixed to fit the facts. For Ott’s analysis, no patches seem available. Some further empirical properties of CLD appear underiva- ble from either of these approaches. For these, the base-generation- cum-deep-anaphora analysis can be considered. Keywords: contrastive left-dislocation, topicalization, right-disloca- tion, resumption, deep vs. surface anaphora, ellipsis, P-stranding, mor- phological connectivity, Dutch, Hungarian 1 Contrastive Left-Dislocation within the Landscape of Left-Dislocation Phenomena 1.1 The Empirical Landscape Contrastive left-dislocation (henceforth CLD1) is a member of a family of left-dislocation phenom- ena that also includes ‘‘ordinary’’ topicalization (TOP) and hanging-topic left-dislocation (HTLD).
    [Show full text]
  • Defining Left-Dislocation
    chapter 2 Defining Left-Dislocation 2.1 Syntactic and Pragmatic Aspects of Left-Dislocation in Latin In this section, I present the syntactic definition for left-dislocation in Latin. This is followed by an exposition of the pragmatic framework that will be used in the analysis of this book. I then take a closer look on Latin relative clause syntax and its implications for Latin left-dislocation in section 2.2. Left-dislocation is a sentence structure in which a referential constituent that could function as an argument or adjunct within the predicate-argument structure of the clause occurs outside the left-peripheral boundaries of the clause containing the predicate. This dislocated constituent is prototypically followed by a grammatically complete clause, in which a co-referential pro- nominal element typically occurs.This pronominal element represents the role of the referent of the dislocated phrase as an argument or adjunct of the pred- icate (Lambrecht 1994: 182; Lambrecht 2001: 1050; Westbury 2014: 98).1 The following four attributes have been used to define prototypical left- dislocation (Lambrecht 2001:1050, modified in Westbury 2014: 101):2 1. The extra-clausal position of a constituent preceding the matrix clause. 2. A possible canonical intra-clausal position for the dislocated constituent within the matrix clause. 3. Anaphoric co-indexation between the dislocated constituent and an overt pronoun within the matrix clause. 4. A separate intonation contour for the dislocated constituent. A Latin example of a prototypical left-dislocation is (1): (1) Epidamniensis ill’ quem dudum dixeram geminum illum puerum qui surrupuit alterum, ei liberorum nisi diuitiae nil erat Plaut.
    [Show full text]
  • Some New Observations on the Cartography of Topic and Focus
    SOME NEW OBSERVATIONS ON THE CARTOGRAPHY OF TOPIC AND FOCUS Anna Cardinaletti Università Ca’ Foscari Venezia Abstract: The goal of this paper is to discuss the cartography of Topic and Focus in Italian left periphery, to show that Topics can occur on either side of Focus, as in Rizzi’s (1997) original proposal. The data provided by Benincà and Poletto (2004) to argue against this possibility are shown to raise both empirical and theoretical questions. Additional evidence is provided to support the view that Foci can be followed by Familiar Topics, as in Frascarelli and Hinterhölzl’s (2007) typology of Topics. The discourse contribution of left- and right-dislocated Familiar Topics is also discussed. Keywords: Cartography, Left Periphery, Focus, Familiar Topics, Right Dislocation 1. Introduction1 In the cartographic approach to the left periphery of the clause, the relative order of the Topic and Focus projections in Italian is debated. While Rizzi (1997) suggested that Topics can freely occur in the left periphery and, in particular, that they appear both before and after Foci, Benincà and Poletto (2004) suggested that no free occurrence of Topics is possible and that the order is fixed: Topics always precede Foci. When a Topic appears to follow a Focus, it should be analysed as a Focus itself, and although it is not intonationally marked, it enters a Focus Field together with the (intonationally) focused constituent. In this paper, I put together two apparently unrelated observations by Rizzi (2001) and Benincà and Poletto (2004) to show that Rizzi (1997) was correct in proposing that topicalised elements can both precede and follow focused elements.
    [Show full text]
  • Right Dislocation As a Biclausal Phenomenon Evidence from Romance Languages
    ADVERTIMENT. Lʼaccés als continguts dʼaquesta tesi queda condicionat a lʼacceptació de les condicions dʼús establertes per la següent llicència Creative Commons: http://cat.creativecommons.org/?page_id=184 ADVERTENCIA. El acceso a los contenidos de esta tesis queda condicionado a la aceptación de las condiciones de uso establecidas por la siguiente licencia Creative Commons: http://es.creativecommons.org/blog/licencias/ WARNING. The access to the contents of this doctoral thesis it is limited to the acceptance of the use conditions set by the following Creative Commons license: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/?lang=en Right dislocation as a biclausal phenomenon Evidence from Romance languages Javier Fernández-Sánchez Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona Departament de Filologia Espanyola Centre de Lingüística Teòrica PhD Thesis supervised by Prof. Dr. Maria Lluïsa Hernanz Carbó Javier Fernández-Sánchez M. Lluïsa Hernanz Carbó Submitted in fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor in Cognitive Science and Language Bellaterra, May 2017 A mis padres, por vuestro constante apoyo y amor incondicional A la memoria de la yaya Aurora, por la devoción que nos teníamos. Acknowledgements There is something quite comforting in writing the acknowledgements of a dissertation: realizing this is the end. The end not only of a piece of research, but of a period of life too. As I write these lines I become aware that despite the hectic and turbulent last months of the writing process, my years as a PhD student have been an amazing experience. And of course, this is due to the fact that the sometimes turtuous road to the end is fortunately not a lonely ride.
    [Show full text]
  • Left Dislocation in Hungarian
    Left Dislocation in Hungarian Peter´ Szucs˝ University of Debrecen Proceedings of the LFG’19 Conference Australian National University Miriam Butt, Tracy Holloway King, Ida Toivonen (Editors) 2019 CSLI Publications pages 293–313 http://csli-publications.stanford.edu/LFG/2019 Keywords: left dislocation, left peripheral constructions, syntactic integration, Hun- garian Szucs,˝ Peter.´ 2019. Left Dislocation in Hungarian. In Butt, Miriam, King, Tracy Holloway, & Toivonen, Ida (Eds.), Proceedings of the LFG’19 Conference, Aus- tralian National University, 293–313. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications. Abstract This paper investigates left dislocation constructions in Hungarian (whereby some discourse-prominent entity is placed at the left periphery of the clause, with a subsequent co-referential pronoun). Two subtypes are distinguished: a) “topic left dislocation”, which is a syntactically integrated construction, sharing properties with Germanic-type left dislocation and clitic left dislocation in certain Romance languages; b) “free left dislocation”, which is a loosely integrated structure, similar to various hanging topic left dislocations. The paper explores the structures’ morphosyntactic and semantic properties, how an LFG-theoretic account of them can be formulated as well as the cross- linguistic implications of Hungarian left dislocation. 1. Introduction Left dislocation (LD)1 is a common label for constructions whereby some discourse-prominent entity is placed at the left periphery of the clause, with a subsequent co-referential pronoun. The term itself originates in Ross (1967), who used it for sentences like (1). As usual in the literature, “left dislocation” will be used as a descriptive label here, without commitment to a particular analysis. Furthermore, I will use the label “host” for the prominent entity itself (John in (1)) and “associated pronoun” or “pronominal associate” for the co- referential pronoun.
    [Show full text]
  • Emily A. Hanink* Postsyntactic Inflection of the Degree Phrase in German Keywords. Concord; German; Inflection; Degree Phrases;
    2018. Proc Ling Soc Amer 3. 23:1-15. https://doi.org/10.3765/plsa.v3i1.4308 Postsyntactic inflection of the degree phrase in German Emily A. Hanink* Abstract. Recent treatments of concord contend that adjectival inflection occurs postsyntactically through the insertion of Agr nodes onto individual, concord-bearing heads after Spell-Out (i.a. Norris 2012, 2014). I examine these claims through the lens of degree modification in German, which demonstrates that current formulations of this approach are untenable. I argue however that a postsyntactic treatment of (ad- jectival) concord can in fact be maintained if Agr node insertion occurs phrasally at DegP, and not at adjectival heads. This account explains i) an observed difference be- tween the inflection of analytic vs. synthetic degree expressions in both simple and complex modifiers, and ii) a puzzle involving across-the-board inflection of coordi- nated adjectives, which I argue to involve pointwise attachment of Agr. Keywords . concord; German; inflection; degree phrases; degree modification 1. Introduction . Recent accounts of concord advance the view that inflection in the nominal domain is re-alized in the postsyntactic component of the grammar (building on i.a. Halle and Marantz 1993; Bobaljik 2008). In particular, Norris (2012, 2014) contends that DP-internal agreement is real-ized by the insertion of Agr nodes – a type of dissociated mopheme (Noyer 1997) – onto individ-ual, concord-bearing heads after Spell-Out (see also i.a. Baier 2015; Sigurðsson 2015; Deal 2016; Pietraszko 2017). A consequence of this proposal is that inflection is realized on heads only, and that if a head can realize inflection, it necessarily does so.
    [Show full text]
  • Left-Dislocations and Their Contribution to Information Structure Steven E
    Left-Dislocations and their Contribution to Information Structure Steven E. Runge Scholar-in-Residence, Logos Bible Software Biblical Greek Language and Linguistics Section SBL Annual Meeting, Nov. 22-25, 2008 1. Traditional explanations Left-dislocations have been referred to by NT grammarians using various terms, among them cleft constructions,1 hanging nominatives,2 pendent nominatives,3 casus pendens,4 or independent nominatives.5 There have also been various claims made about their function: Wallace: either emotion or emphasis6 Young: emphasis7 Porter: highlighting8 or a topic shifter9 2. Linguistic Explanation There is a significant cognitive constraint on the introduction of new entities to the discourse, posited by Wallace Chafe.10 He claims that there is a cross-linguistic preference to only introduce “one new concept at a time”, which has been confirmed by several other linguists.11 Example 1 Presentational clause There is a guy from the office. He asked me… Topic/Comment clause I know a guy from the office. He asked me… 1 Richard A. Young, Intermediate New Testament Greek: A Linguistic and Exegetical Approach (Nashville, Tenn.: Broadman & Holman, 1994), 15. 2 Ibid., 15. 3 Daniel B. Wallace, Greek Grammar Beyond the Basics - Exegetical Syntax of the New Testament (Zondervan Publishing House, 1999), 52. 4 Friedrich Blass et al., A Greek Grammar of the New Testament and Other Early Christian Literature (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1961), 244. 5 Stanley E. Porter, Idioms of the Greek New Testament (Sheffield: JSOT, 1999), 85. 6 Wallace, Greek Grammar, 52. 7 “A cleft construction is a focusing device that highlights information at the beginning of a sentence that is repeated later in the same sentence.” Young, Intermediate New Testament Greek, 15.
    [Show full text]
  • Presupposition & Root Transforms in Adjunct Clauses
    LSA SATURDAY 4.JANUARY ’03 Presupposition & Root Transforms in Adjunct Clauses Richard K. Larson (Stony Brook Univ.) and Miyuki Sawada (Ming Chuan Univ.) Hopper & Thompson (1973: hereafter H&T) observe an interesting syntax-pragmatics correlation in adverbial clauses. As shown in (1a), adverbial , before- and after- clauses resist root transformations like Left Dislocation; correspondingly their content is presupposed (1b). By contrast, because-clauses like those in (1b) allow Left Dislocation (2a), and their content is asserted not presupposed: (1) a. *Mildred bought a Mercedes [ when/before/after her son, he purchased stock in Xerox ]. b. Mildred bought a Mercedes [ when/before/after her son purchased stock in Xerox ]. Presupposes: Mildred’s son purchased stock in Xerox. (2) a. Mildred drives a Mercedes [ because her son, he owns stock in Xerox ]. b. Mildred drives a Mercedes [ because her son owns stock in Xerox ]. Asserts: Mildred’s son purchased stock in Xerox. The robustness of the correlation between root transform availability and presupposition is shown by (3). Here presupposition is forced on the because-clause by association with negation; notice now that Left Dislocation is blocked. (3) *Sam is going out for dinner [ not because his wife, she can’t cook ], but because he wants to discuss Q-magic with Stella. (= H&T’s (245)) Presupposes: Sam’s wife can’t cook Hopper & Thompson’s observations raise some simple, but intriguing questions: • Why should a semantic/pragmatic phenomenon like presupposition be correlated with the possibility
    [Show full text]
  • Subject Doubling in Spoken French by Eleonore Blanquart a Thesis
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by ASU Digital Repository Subject Doubling in Spoken French by Eleonore Blanquart A Thesis Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Arts Approved April 2012 by the Graduate Supervisory Committee: Helene Ossipov, Co-Chair Mariana Bahtchevanova, Co-Chair Elly Van Gelderen ARIZONA STATE UNIVERSITY May 2012 ABSTRACT The purpose of this study is to explore the syntax and pragmatics of subject doubling in spoken French. Many prescriptivists have considered it a redundant and ungrammatical form, but over the years, it has gained more interest from syntacticians. It is widely acknowledged that dislocations involve topics, but the position of these structures is very disputed. Some linguists believe in base generation while others state there is movement. The status of subject clitics also comes into play and their role as arguments or agreement markers is crucial to understanding the issues at stake with a topic analysis. It is often argued that the clitics are undergoing a linguistic cycle whereby they lose their function of argument, and need to be reinforced by disjunct pronouns. In this study, I examined which analyses support my data and I attempted to determine what structures tend to be most dislocated by looking at the environment of the discourse in a corpus of spoken French. i ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I am greatly indebted to the patience, knowledge and support of Dr. Elly Van Gelderen and Dr. Mariana Bahtchevanova throughout this experience. I am also very grateful to Dr. Helene Ossipov.
    [Show full text]
  • Clitic Left Dislocation in Absence of Clitics: a Study in Trilingual Acquisition
    University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics Volume 18 Issue 1 Proceedings of the 35th Annual Penn Article 6 Linguistics Colloquium 5-2012 Clitic Left Dislocation in Absence of Clitics: a Study in Trilingual Acquisition Megan Devlin University of Ulster Raffaella Folli University of Ulster Alison Henry University of Ulster Christina Sevdali University of Ulster Follow this and additional works at: https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl Recommended Citation Devlin, Megan; Folli, Raffaella; Henry, Alison; and Sevdali, Christina (2012) "Clitic Left Dislocation in Absence of Clitics: a Study in Trilingual Acquisition," University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics: Vol. 18 : Iss. 1 , Article 6. Available at: https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol18/iss1/6 This paper is posted at ScholarlyCommons. https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol18/iss1/6 For more information, please contact [email protected]. Clitic Left Dislocation in Absence of Clitics: a Study in Trilingual Acquisition Abstract This paper discusses an unusual structure in the English of a trilingual child acquiring English, Italian and Scottish Gaelic. The child uses a structure where it appears that an object DP is “dou-bled” by a pronoun for an extended period of time (10 months): (1) He don't like it dinosaur (2) He forget it the teddy In Italian, sentences that contain old information take two possible structures: they might contain a left dislocated topic resumed by a clitic: (3) Il libro, l'ho letto the book it-have.1SG read 'The book, I have read it' These are called Clitic Left Dislocation (CLLD) structures in the literature. Alternatively, the topic (the given information) can be introduced as a right dislocated element, again linked to a clitic: (4) L'ho letto, il libro It-have.1SG read the book ‘I have read it, the book’ These are called clitic right dislocation (CLRD) structures.
    [Show full text]
  • Parameters (Material Taken from Chapter 8 ‘Minimum of English Grammar’, Ms
    A Note on Parameters (Material taken from Chapter 8 ‘Minimum of English Grammar’, ms. 2008, Galasso) Generativist vs. Constructivist: A Converging Theory In brief, Constructivists’ accounts assume that grammatical knowledge initially consists of ‘constructions’ derived at by high frequency data-driven forms in the environment. While there may be internal mechanism which undergird such constructions, according to the constructivist view, the processes behind language acquisition are essential environmentally driven. The general idea is that language is built-up ‘piece-meal’ insofar as constructions (i.e., sound-to-meaning links) are based on similar phonological-to-semantic distributions found in the language input. It is their general claim that such a correlation between sound and meaning is strictly associative, and that it holds between the quantity and quality of the exemplars obtained of particular constructions with the constructions of more general schemes that underlie language use. The constructivist model therefore assumes a ‘bottom-up’ cognitive scaffolding of language learning (somewhat akin to what Piaget had earlier claimed regarding a cognitive underpinning to language development. See also Chomsky vs. Piaget: 1975). Generativists’ accounts, on the other hand, differ with constructivist models in one very simple respect—their models credit the speaker with having tacit syntactic knowledge, unrelated in any way to frequency or data-driven constructivist claims which define language as being tethered to cognition. Generativists in this sense draw upon (innate) parameter-setting mechanisms, as opposed to (environmental) data-driven mechanisms, thus continuing the debate on ‘nature vs. nurture’ with respect to language (in general) and to language acquisition (in particular).
    [Show full text]
  • Verbal Inflection at a Distance
    VERBAL INFLECTION AT A DISTANCE LINE MIKKELSEN University of California, Berkeley This paper examines inflection on fronted verbs in Danish. In both VP topicalization and VP left dislocation with resumption, the inflection on the fronted verb is governed by an in situ auxiliary, suggesting that the fronted VP originates as a sister to that auxiliary. This analysis is straightforward for VP topicalization, but fails for VP left dislocation. Yet, the two show identical patterns of inflection, down to systematic covariance in case of interspeaker variation. I take this as evidence that the two fronting constructions have the same syntax and only differ in whether the proform that mediates between the auxiliary and the fronted VP is overt or null. That in turn implies that verbal inflection can be governed at a distance, and that some mechanism other than the standard generative ones (affix-hopping, selection, and feature valuing) is involved. 1 Introduction Danish has a verbal system much like the English one in which each auxiliary determines the inflection of the following verbal element.1 In (1a), for instance, the modal må (must) requires the following auxiliary, perfect have (have), to appear in the infinitival form and have requires gemme (hide) to appear in the past participle form.2 Any other forms of have and gemme are ungrammatical in (1a). The relevant inflectional options are given in (2) below. (1) a. De må hav-e gem-t den godt. they must.PRES have-INF hide-PPC it well They must have hidden it well. b. Han kan vær-e blev-et stoppet i tolden.
    [Show full text]