<<

The Condor90:395-400 0 The Cooper Ornithological Society 1988

CRECHING BEHAVIOR OF BRANDTS’ CHICKS ’

HARRY R. CARTER Point ReyesBird Observatory,4990 ShorelineHighway, StinsonBeach, CA 94970

KEITH A. HOBSON Department of Zoology, Universityof Manitoba, Winnipeg,Manitoba R3T 2N2, Canada

Abstract. Creching behavior of Brandt’s Cormorant (Phalacrocoraxpenicillatus) chicks was studied at SoutheastFarallon Island, California, in July and August 1984. Three stages of creching were described: (1) nest-site crechesinvolved small chicks (10 to 25 days old) from adjacent nests huddling on nest sitesbefore and just after parents stoppedcontinuous attendance of chicks; (2) subcolony creches involved medium-sized chicks (over 20 days old) standingtogether between nest sites and on the periphery of natal subcolonies;and (3) fledglingcreches involved large chicks (over 25 days old) from several subcoloniesstanding together outside of subcoloniesand nearer to the ocean. Nest-site crechesoccurred mainly at night and probably have a thermoregulatoryfunction. Fledglingcreches occurred through- out the day and night and may facilitate fledging behaviors. Chicks in fledgling creches entered the ocean, swam, dove, bathed, and returned to these crecheswhere they were fed by adults up to and beyond the time when they couldfly. Key words: Creche;Brandt s‘ Cormorant; Phalacrocoraxpenicillatus; thermoregulation; fledgling; roost.

INTRODUCTION penicillatus) chicks on SoutheastFarallon Island Creching (the formation of groups of chicks from (SEFI), California. two or more broods in or adjacent to the breeding colony) occurs in most penguins, all ground-nest- METHODS ing , several gulls and terns (reviews in Evans 1980, 1984a; Davis 1982), and in several From 28 July to 29 August 1984, we observed of waterfowl (reviews in Gorman and creching behavior from a blind overlooking a Milne 1972, Munro and Bedard 1977). Adaptive colony of Brandt’s (composed of 10 functions advanced for creching include ther- spatially distinct subcolonies) on a rocky pen- moregulation and protection from predation, insula on the northwest side of SEFI (Fig. 1). permitting chicks to be left unattended while both Most nests were placed on rocky or dirt sub- parents forage. Other explanations consider strateswith little or no slope,although some nests creching to be accidental outcomes of crowding were placed on flat areas within steeper slopes or aggressive territorial encounters. Although (subcolonies 3, 5, 6, 9, and 10). We monitored creching is well-known in the Pelecanidae, it has chick locations and activities daily from 28 July not been studied in the Phalacrocoracidae. to 9 August (Period 1) and every secondday from Nevertheless, this behavior may be widespread 11 to 29 August (Period 2). In Period 1 (early in cormorants. For example, Shag (Phalucroco- chick period), we observed chicks in 1-hr ses- rax aristotelis) and Crowned Cormorant (P. cor- sionsevery other hour from 05:30 to 20:30 (PDT) onatus) chicks are known to form crechesin the for nine full days and on two other days where late chick period (Snow 1963, Williams and Coo- the first sessionwas missed. During Period 2 (late per 1983) and Double-crested Cormorant (P. au- chick period), we observed chicks in 2-hr ses- ritus) chicks are known to form creches in re- sions twice per day (lo:30 to 12:30 and 18:OOto sponse to disturbances (Lewis 1929, Mendall 20:00) on every second day. The durations of 1936, Brechtel 1983). In this paper, we describe crechesat nest sites were determined during Pe- creching behavior in Brandt’s Cormorant (P. riod 1. If they were present during the first 30 min of an hourly sessionbut broke up before the end of the hour they were classedas being 0.1 to ’ ’ Received 3 August 1987. Final acceptance24 No- 1.0 hr long. Those still present at the same site vember 1987. at the start of the next session were classed as

[3951 396 HARRY R. CARTER AND KEITH A. HOBSON

SUBCOLONIES present at the same site during the last evening u count (20:30) and at least during the first morning FLEDGLING CRECHES @ count of the following day (05:30). We counted all after-hatching-year (AHY) present in and near subcolonies during each session. Am- bient weather conditions at SEFI, including wind speedand direction, air temperature, and cloud cover were monitored daily at 08:00, 12:00, 16:00, and 20:O0. We considered chicks to be fledglingsbetween the time that they first entered the water to when they left the colony area. Dur- ing this period, chicks developed the ability to fly. On 25 July, we banded 149 chicks with indi- vidually color-taped color bands, another color band indicating hatching year, and an incoloy USFWS band. On 16 August, we banded another 63 chicks (without the color-taped bands). Band- ing was performed at night (00:45 to 03:OO) in to reducedisturbance and to minimize pre- dation of eggsand small chicks by Western Gulls 0.& (Lams occidentalis). Chicks were banded at a slightly earlier age than usual (see Boekelheide FIGURE 1. Schematic map of the Brandt’s Cor- morant colony showinglocations of 10 subcolonies(see et al., in press),enabling us to band many chicks Table 1) and three largefledgling creches on a peninsula on natal nests.We searchedfor individual chicks on the northwest side of SEFI. Small arrows indicate between 25 and 27 July and confirmed the lo- directions of movement of banded chicks from nests cation of many natal nest sites. On 25 July (after to fledgling creches. Large arrows indicate directions of movement from fledgling crechesto the ocean. Let- the first banding), we counted the number of well- ter R indicatesnight roostingareas used by AHY birds. built nests,chicks, and eggsvisible from the blind Dashed lines indicate approximate elevations. in each of nine subcolonies(Table 1). During the first banding, we found about 50 to 60 chicks in being at least 2.1 to 3.0 hr long. Those not present areas out of view of the blind (including subcol- were classed as being 1.1 to 2.0 hr long, and so ony 10 and part of subcolony 8) and about 20 on. We disregarded creches that lasted less than chicks were mobile enough to escapeour capture 5 min. We defined overnight creches as those attempts.

TABLE 1. Numbers of active nest sites and nest-site crechesof Brandt’s Cormorants at nine subcolonieson SEFI in 1984.

Nest-sitecreches ’ 25 July censusi NUlllbeI 96attended by Subcolony’ Nest sites Number chicks Number eggs observed % sitesused adult(s)

1 18 24 37 55.6 81.1 2 9 10 71.4 60.0 3 3: 53 - 4 4 8 ii 75.0 90.0 5 3 5 9 66.7 88.9 6 14 20 0 39 50.0 53.8 35 56 7 119 62.9 72.3 : 19 13 0 4 15.8 75.0 9 23 21 I - - - Total I160 215 22 238 52.0 72.3 LSee Figure I for locations. i Well-built nestswith eggsor chicks. ’ Dunng Period 1 (28 July to 9 August).Subcolomes 3, 9, and IO werenot followed(see text). CRECHING IN CORMORANTS 397

Nest-site creches tended to occur wherever at leasttwo nestswith similar-aged chickswere close together (52 m). Banded chicks did not move more than 2 to 3 m from their natal site, so there was no interchange of chicks between subcolo- nies. During Period 1, the number of chicks in nest- site crecheschanged significantly throughout the day (One-Way ANOVA: F = 9.23, df = 8, P < 200 1 0.001; Fig. 2). More birds were involved in nest- 100 site crechesat the 05:30 count than at other times I of day (Tukey’s range test, P < 0.05). From 11:30 0530 0730 0930 ,130 ,330 ,530 1730 1930 to 15:30, numbers of birds in nest-site creches

TIME Of DAY (PDT) were similar (P > 0.05). Nest-site creches thus FIGURE 2. Number of Brandt’s Cormorantchicks occurred mainly at dawn and dusk, presumably presentin nest-siteand othercreches at 2-hr intervals many formed throughout the night. We recorded throughthe day, expressedas the total sum of chicks 21 overnight creches at 13 sites. These had a participatingbetween 28 July and 9 August. mean duration of 14.4 * 2.1 (SD) hr. Diurnal nest-site crechesaveraged 1.9 * 1.3 (SD) hr and RESULTS ranged from 5 min to at least 9 hr (n = 18 1 nest- site crechesover nine full days). They frequently NEST-SITE CRECHES broke up during late morning and early afternoon Nest-site creches were the first indication of when adults returned to feed their chicks and crechingbehavior. These crecheswere composed drove other chicks from the nest. In addition, of small, relatively immobile chicks (about 10 to nest-sitecreches not attendedby adults often were 25 days old) from adjacent nests(confirmed with disrupted by subadult birds wandering through banded chicks) which huddled closely while sit- the colony stealing nest material or attempting ting or lying down together on and about a nest to usurp nest sites(see Boekelheide et al., in press). bowl. Nest-site crechesoccurred before and just When disbanded by AHY birds, nest-site creches after parents stopped continuous attendance of usually reformed nearby. Chicks also left these chicks at nest sites (when parents returned only groups to beg from parents and often returned to feed chicks). Attending parents, which stood to nest-site crechesafter being fed. beside the nest or on the edge of the nest bowl, During Period 1, the number of chicks in nest- only rarely interfered with neighboring chicks as site crecheswas negatively correlated with am- they joined a nest-site creche. During Period 1, bient temperature (r = -0.45, n = 45, P < 0.01). we recorded 238 nest-site crechesat 52 nest sites Strong negative correlations occurred on 6 Au- in the seven subcolonies we observed (Table 1). gust (r = -0.98, n = 4, P < 0.05) and 7 August They occurred at 52.0% of the 100 nest sites (r = -0.94, n = 5, P < 0.05). On these days, followed, ranging between 15.8 and 75.0% of sites temperatures ranged from 12.3” to 15.5”C and in each subcolony. They often occurred at the 12.1” to 15.8”C, respectively. The number of samesites (mean [ f SD] number of creches/site= chicksin nest-sitecreches was not correlatedwith 4.6 + 3.8, range = l-l 5), up to three times/day, wind speed(r = 0.0 1, n = 5 1, P > 0.05) or cloud and over several days (mean [&SD] days site cover (r = 0.09, n = 49, P > 0.05), despite the used = 3.4 f 2.2, range = l-8). In 334 counts study colony being located on the most exposed of the 238 nest-site crecheson 11 days, creche part of SEFI. However, these variables did not sizeranged from two to 14 chicks,averaging 4.5 * show a marked die1 pattern as did temperature. 1.9 (SD) chicks. Most (72.3%) of the 238 nest- site crecheswere attended by adult(s) when first SUBCOLONY AND FLEDGLING CRECHES recorded (within 1 hr of initiation), ranging be- Subcolony and fledgling crecheswere composed tween 53.8 and 90.0% in different subcolonies of medium- to large-sized chicks (over about 20 (Table 1). Creche sites occurred widely in each days old). Smaller subcolony crechesformed first subcolony, although several adjacent sites were in large spacesbetween nests within subcolonies usedinterchangeably by the same banded chicks. and around subcolony edges after parents had 398 HARRY R. CARTER AND KEITH A. HOBSON

stoppedcontinuous attendance of chicks and in- volved chicks from the same subcolony standing in loose associations(or rarely huddled). Larger fledgling creches formed outside subcolonies shortly after subcolony crecheshad become com- mon, and involved chicks from several subcol- onies. During Period 1, we recorded 128 sub- colony (mainly) and fledglingcreches. Since chicks in small subcolony creches were fairly mobile, creche locations shifted continuously, making it difficult to determine their durations. In 5 10 counts of the 128 subcolony and fledgling creches on 11 days, thesecreches ranged in size from two to 34 chicks, averaging 9.3 f 5.7 (SD) chicks/ creche. Larger fledgling creches(up to 78 chicks) formed later in Period 2 when chicks from sev- eral subcolonies grouped farther from subcolo- nies and nearer to ocean accesspoints at three permanent locations (Fig. 1). Chicks were rela- tively inactive in subcolony creches, spending most of the time walking, preening, sleeping,and

picking up pebbles and displaced bits of nesting JULY AUGUST material. Chicks not yet able to fly were observed FIGURE 3. A. Numbers of Brandt’s Cormorant entering the ocean, swimming, diving, bathing, chicks participating in nest-site creches (19:30) and and returning to fledgling creches. They often other creches(11:30) on each day examined between were observed preening and using spread-wing 28 July and 29 August. B. Number of AHY Brandt’s Cormorants present in the colony and in night roosts posturesafter being in the water. (19:30) between 28 July and 29 August. During Period 1, numbers of chicks partici- pating in subcolony (mainly) and fledgling crech- es tended to increaseduring the day and decrease but we observed banded parents feeding only toward dusk (Fig. 2) although differences were their own chicks. Siblings beggedvigorously and not significant (One-Way ANOVA: F = 0.62, incessantly once their parent had arrived. df = 8, P > 0.05). This resulted partly from tem- No correlations were found between the num- porary breakups of nest-site crechesduring the bers of chicks in subcolony and fledgling creches day when these chicks sometimes formed sub- and ambient temperature (r = 0.03, n = 44, P > colony creches for short periods. After parents O.OS),wind speed (r = 0.19, n = 45, P > 0.05), stopped continuous attendance of chicks, chicks or cloud cover (r = 0.34, n = 43, P > 0.05). We began to spend all day and night in fledgling never observed Western Gulls attacking creche- crechesoutside subcolonies. This resulted in lit- aged chicks whether adults were present or not. tle interchange of chicks between nest-site and On only a few occasions,gulls stole food by ha- other creches(confirmed with banded chicks). It rassing adults as they fed chicks, causing them appeared that subcolony crecheswere part of a to regurgitate onto the ground. Few chicks died transitional stagebetween nest-site and fledgling between the time parents stoppedcontinuous at- creches. For a few days during this transition, tendance of chicks and when chicks permanently chickswould return to nest sitesat dusk and then left the colony (also see Boekelheide et al., in return to fledgling creches shortly after dawn. press). Adults fed chicks during the day, often leading Numbers of chicks in subcolony and fledgling (or being chased by) medium-sized chicks part creches increased throughout Period 1 but os- or all the way back to their nest sitesbefore feed- cillated in Period 2 (Fig. 3A). Three peaks in ing them. However, older chicks and fledglings numbers occurred on 8, 17 to 29, and 25 August were fed within or on the periphery of fledgling and corresponded with changes in the sizes of creches.Often severalchicks would approachand large fledgling crechesas chicksjoined from sub- beg weakly from an adult returning to feed chicks colonies with different breeding chronologies. CRECHING IN CORMORANTS 399

These peaksroughly mirrored three earlier peaks pensate for reduced thermal properties of chicks of the numbers of chicks in nest-site crecheson under suboptimal conditions, especially when 30 July and 3 and 9 August (Fig. 3A). Numbers cold stressedas appeared to occur in 1984. In of chicks in fledgling crechesfell between peaks 1985 and 1986, nest-site crecheswere apparently becausesome chickspermanently left this colony much lessprevalent (S. Johnston and J. P. Feld- earlier than others (determined by observations man, pers. comm.). Since chicks were character- of banded chicks elsewhereon SEFI). Others re- istically huddled in nest-site crechesprimarily at mained and received feedingsin fledgling creches the coldest times of day (evening, night, early between swimming and diving bouts nearby. morning) and just after the period of continuous Several birds developed the ability to fly during parental brooding, this strongly suggesteda pri- this period (including wing exercising on land marily thermoregulatory role for nest-site crech- and water) and were observed flying and landing es (see Evans 1984a, 1984b; Vickery and Millar in or near creches, then being fed by parents. 1984). In addition, chicks left nests and formed Once chicks joined a fledgling creche, most did small groups on nearby nests during night band- not leave it before being able to fly except to be ing so it appearedthat nest-site crechesmay also fed or to enter the ocean. Only a few instances provide protection from predation. Similarly, of banded chicks temporarily wandering from Williams and Cooper (1983) noted that, by 15 one fledgling creche to another were noted but days of age, Crowned Cormorant chicks joined none were ever observed returning to natal nest other chicks on the nest farthest from the ob- sites. server when disturbed. Shaw (1985) also noted Numbers of adult and subadult birds in sub- that, at 28 days of age, Blue-eyed Shag (P. atri- colonies during midday (11:30) and before dusk ceps) chicks moved onto neighboring nests for (19:30) fell in three steps between 28 July and brief periods when approached by an observer. 29 August (Fig. 3B), as parents stopped contin- Brandt’s Cormorants differ from other creching uous attendance of chicks in different subcolo- specieswith altricial young (Evans 1980) in that nies. During this same period, however, the nondisturbed chicks creched at nest sites even numbers of AHY birds in night roosts increased before parents stoppedcontinuous attendance of (Fig. 3B). The two major roostswere adjacent to chicks. Toleration of such behavior by attending the two largest fledgling creches(Fig. 1) but little adults presumably reflectsthe development of at intermingling was observed. Once parents least adequate parent-chick recognition by the stopped roosting at nest sites, they joined these time nest-site crechesoccur (10 to 25 days after nearby roosts (confirmed with banded adults) hatch; review in Evans 1980). Snow (1963) noted which grew rapidly just before dusk and dis- several instancesof Shagchicks over 25 days old persed rapidly just after dawn. Thus, adults occurring in neighboring nests but she did not roostednear (but not beside) their chicks in fledg- mention if adults or neighboring chicks were ling creches.Such roostswere never present dur- present. ing the day. All Brandt’s Cormorant chicks (in the colony we observed)participated in fledgling creches(for DISCUSSION varying periods of time); thus, crechingwas char- The formation of nest-site crechescoincided with acteristic of the late chick period. In 1985 and the presumed delayed onset of thermoregulation 1986, even larger fledgling crechesformed in the and completion of growth of the dense coat of same locations as in 1984 (Fig. 1). This behavior natal down (seeDunn 1976; Evans 1980,1984b). shows strong social attractions between chicks, Brandt’s Cormorant chicks are brooded contin- as found in other creching studies (see Evans uously for 5 to 10 days after hatch and attended 1980, 1984a; Davis 1982) but the locations of continuously by adults for an average of 25 to crechesapparently were influenced by proximity 57 days, depending on the year (Table 2). In to ocean accesspoints. Although breeding adults 1984, cold northwest winds continued into late were freed of attending chicks during the day in summer (often warm and foggy at SEFI) and the later part of the creching period, they still Brandt’s Cormorants bred later, had smaller roosted near these crechesat night. These roosts clutch sizesand smaller brood sizes,and parents did not appear to confer any protection to chicks stopped continuous attendance of chicks earlier (seeDavis 1982). By occurringnearer to the ocean than usual (Table 2). Nest-site crechesmay com- than nest sites (Fig. l), fledgling crechesmay fa- 400 HARRY R. CARTER AND KEITH A. HOBSON

TABLE 2. Comparison of breedingbiology characteristicsof Brandt’s Cormorants at the study colony on SEFI in 1984 compared to previous years.

1971-1983’ Rangeof annual 1984’ Breeding biology characteristic Overall R t SD (n) means R k SD (n)

Clutch initiation date 14 May -C 9.8 (282) 3-31 May 27 May f 9.9 (8) Clutch size 3.1 * 0.7 (286) 2.5-3.8 2.7 + 0.8 (24) No. chicks leaving nest/successfulsite3 2.0 * 0.7 (234) 1.1-2.9 1.7 f 0.7 (19) No. days chicks attended continuouslyby adults4 35.4 f 6.1 (192) 25.1-56.9 29.9 ? 7.4 (16)

L Data obtained exclusivelyfrom subcolonies 5 and 6 (Boekelheideetal., in press). 2Data for clutch initiationfrom subcolomes 5 and 6 only; data for other characteristics from all subcolonies (PRBO,unpubl. data) ’ Includessites where at least one chick left the site. 4Number of days from when first egg hatched to when site first unattended by adults. cilitate fledging behaviors (i.e., diving, bathing, DAVIS,L. 1982. Crechingbehavior of Adelie Penguin preening, flying, etc.) as well as the development chicks (Pygoscelis adeliae). N.Z. J. Zool. 9:279- 286. of roosting behaviors in chicks dependent on pa- DUNN, E. H. 1976. Development of endothermy and rental feedings. Similarly, Shag chicks sometimes existence energy expenditure of nestling Double- form creches at colonies or on nearby rocks where crested Cormorants. Condor 78:350-366. they are fed by parents up to and beyond the EVANS,R. M. 1980. Development of behavior in time when they can fly (Snow 1963). The ten- seabirds:an ecologicalperspective, p. 27 l-322. In J. Burger, B. L. Olla, and H. E. Winn [eds.], Be- dency for cormorant chicks to form fledgling (and havior of marine . Vol. 4. Marine birds. other) creches likely depends to a great extent on Plenum Publishing Corp., New York. nesting habitat. Creching is most feasible in flat EVANS,R. M. 1984a. Some causal and functional or gently sloping ground-nesting habitats (like correlatesofcreching in youngwhite pelicans.Can. J. Zool. 62:8 14-8 19. those used by Brandt’s Cormorants on SEFI), EVANS,R. M. 1984b. Development of thermoregu- whereas prefledging movement of chicks from lation in young white pelicans. Can. J. Zool. 62: nests located in cliff- or tree-nesting habitats are 808-813. less feasible. GORMAN,M. L., AND H. MILNE. 1972. Creche be- havior in the Common Eider Somateria m. mol- ACKNOWLEDGMENTS lissima. L. Omis Stand. 3:2 l-26. LEWIS,H. F. 1929. The natural history of the Double- We thank T. Gumbart for assistancein the field, the crestedCormorant (Phalacrocorax auritus auritus San FranciscoChapter of the OceanicSociety for trans- [Lesson]). Ph.D.diss., Cornell Univ., Ithaca, NY portation to and from SEFI, and the U.S. Fish and (published by Prov. Quebec Sot. Protect. Birds, Wildlife Servicefor permissionto conductour research Ru-mi-lou Books, Ottawa, Ontario). on the Farallon Islands National Wildlife Refuge. D. MENDALL,H. L. 1936. The home-life and economic G. Ainley, R. J. Boekelheide,E. H. Dunn, R. M. Evans, status of the Double-crested Cormorant. Univ. and P. C. Rasmussenprovided helpful comments on Maine Studies 38(ser. 2): l-l 59. various drafts. This is contribution no. 36 1 ofthe Point MUNRO,J., ANDJ. BEDARD. 1977. Creche formation Reyes Observatory. in the Common Eider. Auk 94:759-77 1. SHAW, P. 1985. Brood reduction in the Blue-eyed LITERATURE CITED ShagPhalacrocorax atriceps. Ibis 127:476-494. BOEKELHEIDE,R. J., S. H. MORRELL,D. G. AINLEY, SNOW,B. K. 1963. The behaviour of the Shag. Br. Birds 56:77-103, 164-186. AND T. J. LEWIS. In press. Brandt’s Cormorant. VICKERY,W. L., AND J. S. MILLAR. 1984. The en- In D. G. Ainley and R. J. Boekelheide [eds.], The Farallon Islands seabird community: ecology, ergeticsof huddling by endotherms. Oikos 43:88- structure, and dynamics in an upwelling system. 93. Stanford Univ. Press, Stanford, CA. WILLIAMS,A. J., ANDJ. COOPER.1983. The Crowned BRECHTEL,S. 1983. The reproductive ecologyof the Cormorant: breeding biology, diet, and offspring- Double-crested Cormorant in southern Alberta. reduction strategy.Ostrich 54:2 13-2 19. M.Sc.thesis,Univ. ofAlberta, Edmonton, Alberta.