Dimensions of Cognition in an Insect, the Honeybee

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Dimensions of Cognition in an Insect, the Honeybee Dimensions of Cognition in an Insect, the Honeybee Randolf Menzel Freie Universität Berlin Martin Giurfa Université–CNRS Paul Sabatier Emery, & Dickinson, 2003; Emery, 2006; Güntürkün, This review provides evidence for the enormous richness of 2005; Katz, 1950a). Thus, expectation, attention, planning, insect behavior, its high flexibility, and the cross-talk between and decision making could be useful terms for animals, different behavioral routines. The memory structure established even if we assume that they possess only implicit knowl- by multiple forms of learning represents sensory inputs and edge. If such terms are used, we assume that the animal relates behaviors in such a way that representations of complex retrieves a memory whose structure allows two or more environmental conditions are formed. Navigation and com- alternative outcomes to be evaluated and compared munication in social hymenoptera are particularly telling before any motor action is performed. In this article, we examples in this respect, but it is fair to conclude that similar will address the question as to whether it is reasonable to integrated forms of dealing with the environment will be found assume such forms of internal processing in an insect, in other insects when they are studied more closely. In this the honeybee. Specifically, we will ask whether these ani- sense, research addressing behavioral complexity and its under- mals can recollect the contents of their memory without lying neural substrates is necessary to characterize the real expression of sensory-motor routines and use them for potential of insect learning and memory. Usually, such an internal operations and decision making. There are approach has been used to characterize behavioral simplicity good reasons for rejecting this kind of questioning when rather than complexity. It seems therefore timely to focus on the it comes to understanding invertebrate behavior. On the latter by studying problem solving alongside and in addition to other hand, searching for experimental conditions that elemental forms of learning. would allow us to address the internal processing of the brain devoid of sensory-motor control, its spontaneity, Key Words: learning and memory in the honeybee, nav- and its creativity, even in such tiny brains as those of igation, comparative cognition, nonelemental forms insects, may help to do better justice to the complexity of of learning neural functions in bigger brains. WHY USE BEES FOR STUDIES One of the most important distinctions in conceptu- ON COGNITIVE COMPLEXITY? alizing cognition is that between implicit and explicit knowledge. Explicit knowledge may, in a strict sense, Three main reasons justify the use of the honeybee as exist only in humans. Animals might possess only implicit a model for studies on cognitive complexity: (a) behav- knowledge, but this does not exclude the possibility that ioral richness, including complex learning and memory certain forms of knowledge might reach an explicit-like faculties; (b) experimental access in terms of controlled status specific for the animal species in question in the training and testing; and (c) accessibility of the central sense that internal operations on memories are per- nervous system. With respect to the first characteristic, formed without motor expression of such operations. A bees exhibit developed learning and memory capabilities telling example of this form of knowledge is the choice Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews behavior of food-storing birds, which make their choices Volume 5 Number 1, March 2006 24-40 depending on which kind of food they have stored and DOI: 10.1177/1534582306289522 when and where they have stored it (Clayton, Bussey, © 2006 Sage Publications 24 Downloaded from bcn.sagepub.com at UNIV CALIFORNIA IRVINE on October 5, 2016 Menzel, Giurfa / DIMENSIONS OF COGNITION IN AN INSECT 25 that apply to local cues characterizing the places of inter- which has been described as the honeybee dance est, which are essentially the hive and the food sources. At (von Frisch, 1967) because of its use of ritualized, com- these places, honeybees learn, memorize, and discrimi- municative behavior indicating distance and direction to nate colors, shapes, patterns, odors, and textures, among a profitable food source, can be “read” by the experi- other characterstics (Menzel, 1985, 2001). These capaci- menter, who can record dances in one-frame observation ties allow enhanced foraging efficiency and, coupled with hives with glass walls. Dances therefore constitute true learning abilities for landmark constellations and for reports on what the outgoing forager perceived, what it celestial cues (azimuthal position of the sun, polarized learned, and what it considers worth reporting. This light pattern of the blue sky), ensure efficient navigation unique situation was ingeniously exploited by Karl von in a complex environment. The second characteristic is Frisch (1967), who discovered through it a vast range of what makes honeybees useful for the experimenter: They sensory- and experience-dependent capacities. are accessible for controlled training and testing. The work on honeybees has more recently gained Different paradigms have been used to this end, two of from cognitive perspectives (Cheng & Wignall, 2006; which deserve special attention: (a) the olfactory condi- Giurfa, 2003; Menzel, Giurfa, Gerber, & Hellstern, tioning of the proboscis extension response (PER) in har- 2001). Besides the battery of more traditional assays nessed bees and (b) the training of the approach flight allowing the study of behavior mentioned above, new toward a visual target in free-flying bees. In the former, paradigms have been conceived both for studies on free- harnessed bees are trained to associate an odor with a flying bees conditioned mostly with visual stimuli and for sucrose reward delivered to their antennae, which elicits studies on olfactory learning with restrained bees. We the PER (Bitterman, Menzel, Fietz, & Schäfer, 1983; will focus on three main topics that allow appreciating to Takeda, 1961), and then to the proboscis; the association which extent cognitive thinking has impregnated new formed is of Pavlovian nature and enables the odor to studies on bee learning and memory and how this think- release the PER in a subsequent test. In the latter, free- ing has changed our vision of the complexity of this flying individually marked bees are pretrained to fly insect’s behavior. We will concentrate on learning and toward the training/test place and are rewarded with memory as applied in navigation, communication, and sucrose solution whenever they choose the appropriate discrimination. For the first context, studies on naviga- visual target at that place (von Frisch, 1967). In this way, tion between the nest and a goal (a profitable food bees can be trained to visual stimuli such as colors, shapes, source) have in recent years overtaken the traditional patterns, depth and motion contrast, among others. The conceptual framework, which posited that navigation associations built in this context link visual stimuli and along well-defined routes is the main strategy bees use. reward but also the animal’s response (e.g., landing) and The structure of spatial memory in bees was reconsid- the reward, thus representing an instrumental form of ered by adopting a cognitive approach and applying learning. Finally, the third characteristic that makes bees novel forms of experimentation. For the latter case, interesting for cognitive complexity studies is the fact that questions are emerging about the nature of the memo- the neural bases of such complexity can be accessed ries that honeybee dances may activate in dance follow- because of the relative simplicity of the bee nervous sys- ers. This questioning contrasts with the traditional view tem (Menzel, 2001; Giurfa, 2003). PER conditioning has of bees “blindly” following instructions from dance the advantage that it can be combined with a physiologi- behavior with the notion that their own memories about cal approach, allowing the study of the bee brain while the environment may be reactivated by dances that the bee learns or recalls memorized information in vivo. would, in turn, allow combining instructions concerning As the bee is immobilized in such a preparation, it is pos- a goal with known routes toward it. Finally, discrimina- sible to expose its brain through a small window cut in the tion learning in bees also required a change of frame- cuticle of the head. Physiological correlates of the differ- work as the accent is shifted from the study of sensory ent forms of olfactory conditioning can be found at capacities to the question as to how these capacities are different levels, ranging from the molecular and pharma- implemented to solve nonelemental discriminations. cological ones to single identified neurons and neuronal ensembles whose activity can be monitored using modern LEARNING IN A NAVIGATION CONTEXT optophysiological recording techniques. In most behavioral studies, inference about internal Navigation is an orientation strategy that allows ani- processing can be made only indirectly by observing mals to travel between defined locations without having changes in the behavior in question. In the honeybee, direct sensory access to these locations. In this sense, nav- one can additionally study a communicative process igation differs from general orientation and guidance, between foraging animals, transcending the usual limita- which
Recommended publications
  • Do Insects Have Emotions? Some Insights from Bumble Bees David Baracchi, Mathieu Lihoreau, Martin Giurfa
    Do Insects Have Emotions? Some Insights from Bumble Bees David Baracchi, Mathieu Lihoreau, Martin Giurfa To cite this version: David Baracchi, Mathieu Lihoreau, Martin Giurfa. Do Insects Have Emotions? Some Insights from Bumble Bees. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, Frontiers, 2017, 11, 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00157. hal-02105101 HAL Id: hal-02105101 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-02105101 Submitted on 20 Apr 2019 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. PERSPECTIVE published: xx August 2017 doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00157 Do Insects Have Emotions? Some Insights from Bumble Bees David Baracchi 1, 2*, Mathieu Lihoreau 1 and Martin Giurfa 1 1 Research Center on Animal Cognition, Center for Integrative Biology, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, University of Toulouse, Toulouse, France, 2 Laboratoire d’Ethologie Expérimentale et Comparée, Université Paris 13, Paris, France While our conceptual understanding of emotions is largely based on human subjective experiences, research in comparative cognition has shown growing interest in the existence and identification of “emotion-like” states in non-human animals. There is still ongoing debate about the nature of emotions in animals (especially invertebrates), and certainly their existence and the existence of certain expressive behaviors displaying internal emotional states raise a number of exciting and challenging questions.
    [Show full text]
  • Research Center on Animal Cognition CRCA
    Research units HCERES report on research unit: Research Center on Animal Cognition CRCA Under the supervision of the following institutions and research bodies: Université Toulouse 3 - Paul Sabatier - UPS Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique - CNRS Evaluation Campaign 2014-2015 (Group A) Research units In the name of HCERES,1 In the name of the experts committee,2 Didier HOUSSIN, president Martine HAUSBERGER, chairwoman of the committee Under the decree No.2014-1365 dated 14 november 2014, 1 The president of HCERES "countersigns the evaluation reports set up by the experts committees and signed by their chairman." (Article 8, paragraph 5) 2 The evaluation reports "are signed by the chairman of the expert committee". (Article 11, paragraph 2) Research Center on Animal Cognition, CRCA, U Toulouse 3, CNRS, Mr Martin GIURFA Evaluation report This report is the result of the evaluation by the experts committee, the composition of which is specified below. The assessments contained herein are the expression of an independent and collegial deliberation of the committee. Unit name: Research Center on Animal Cognition Unit acronym: CRCA Label requested: UMR Present no.: 5169 Name of Director Mr Martin GIURFA (2014-2015): Name of Project Leader (2016-2020): Mr Martin GIURFA Expert committee members Chair: Ms Martine HAUSBERGER, Université de Rennes 1 Experts: Ms Fabienne AUJARD, CNRS, Brunoy (CoNRS representative) Mr Jérôme ESTAQUIER, Université de Laval, Québec, Canada Ms Heike FELDHAAR, Bayreuth University, Germany Ms Sylvie GRANON, Centre
    [Show full text]
  • Way-Finding in Displaced Clock-Shifted Bees Proves Bees Use a Cognitive Map
    Way-finding in displaced clock-shifted bees proves bees use a cognitive map James F. Cheesemana,b,1, Craig D. Millarb,c, Uwe Greggersd, Konstantin Lehmannd, Matthew D. M. Pawleya,e, Charles R. Gallistelf,1, Guy R. Warmana,b, and Randolf Menzeld aDepartment of Anaesthesiology, School of Medicine, and bSchool of Biological Sciences, University of Auckland, Auckland, 1142, New Zealand; cAllan Wilson Centre for Molecular Ecology and Evolution, Palmerston North, PN4442, New Zealand; dInstitute of Biology–Neurobiology, Free University of Berlin, 14195 Berlin, Germany; eInstitute of Natural and Mathematical Sciences, Massey University, Auckland, 0745, New Zealand; and fDepartment of Psychology and Center for Cognitive Science, Rutgers University, Piscataway, NJ 08854-8020 Contributed by Charles R. Gallistel, May 1, 2014 (sent for review January 22, 2014) Mammals navigate by means of a metric cognitive map. Insects, whether these abilities rest on the construction and use of most notably bees and ants, are also impressive navigators. The a metric cognitive map (11–18). question whether they, too, have a metric cognitive map is Way-finding, the ability to set a course from one familiar but important to cognitive science and neuroscience. Experimentally otherwise arbitrarily chosen location to another location that is captured and displaced bees often depart from the release site in not perceptible from the first, is the signature of a metric cog- the compass direction they were bent on before their capture, nitive map. Displacing an animal from its current location to an even though this no longer heads them toward their goal. When experimenter-chosen location within the territory the animal is they discover their error, however, the bees set off more or less presumed to be familiar with, and then observing its subsequent directly toward their goal.
    [Show full text]
  • The Honey Bee Dance Language
    The Honey Bee Dance Language Background Components of the dance language Honey bee dancing, perhaps the most intriguing aspect of their biology, is also one of the most fascinating behaviors When an experienced forager returns to the colony with in animal life. Performed by a worker bee that has returned a load of nectar or pollen that is sufficiently nutritious to to the honey comb with pollen or nectar, the dances, in warrant a return to the source, she performs a dance on the essence, constitute a language that “tells” other workers surface of the honey comb to tell other foragers where the where the food is. By signaling both distance and direction food is. The dancer “spells out” two items of information— with particular movements, the worker bee uses the dance distance and direction—to the target food patch. Recruits language to recruit and direct other workers in gathering then leave the hive to find the nectar or pollen. pollen and nectar. Distance and direction are presented in separate compo- The late Karl von Frisch, a professor of zoology at the Uni- nents of the dance. versity of Munich in Germany, is credited with interpret- ing the meaning of honey bee dance movements. He and Distance his students carried out decades of research in which they carefully described the different components of each dance. When a food source is very close to the hive (less than 50 Their experiments typically used glass-walled observation meters), a forager performs a round dance (Figure 1). She hives and paint-marked bee foragers.
    [Show full text]
  • Curriculum Vitae Fabienne Dupuy
    Curriculum Vitae Fabienne Dupuy Date of birth: January18th, 1980 Place of birth: Saint-Gaudens (31) France Professional address : University of Cambridge Department of zoology Downing Street Cambridge CB2 3EJ United Kingdom Tel : +44 (0)1223 336622 E-Mail : [email protected] Web page: http://casas-lab.irbi.univ-tours.fr/dupuy.html Research Interests Neurobiology, Neuroethology, Sensory system Expertise Behavioural Techniques (conditioning procedures) Optophysiological measurements of neuronal activity (calcium imaging recording in the insect nervous system) Intra-cellular and extra-cellular electrophysiology Computing programmation in Matlab software Informatic tools (R, Statistica, Linux) Current position Since 2009 Postdoctoral fellowship : neural basis of the steering behaviour in Gryllus bimaculatus. Department of zoology, University of Cambridge, UK. Funded by the BBRSC. Advisor : Dr. Berthold Hedwig University education 2005 - 2009 PhD thesis (PhD course Santé sciences technologies) : Coding of mechanosensory information in the sensorial system of the cricket Nemobius sylvestris. I.R.B.I. CNRS/Univ. Faculté des Sciences et Techinques, Tours, France. Supported by CILIA European project. Advisor: Prof Dr. Claudio Lazzari. 2004 - 2005 First year of PhD (PhD course CLESCO): Behavioural and neurobiological basis of the olfactive learning in the ant Camponotus spp. C.R.C.A CNRS/Univ. Paul Sabatier, Toulouse, France. Advisor: Prof Dr. Martin Giurfa. 2003 - 2004 MSc degree: Neurosciences (speciality: Neurosciences , Behavior and Cognition) obtained with honours: Olfactory learning and coding in the ants Camponotus spp. C.R.C.A CNRS-Univ. Paul Sabatier, Toulouse, France and University of Buenos Aires, Argentina Advisors: Prof. Dr. Martin Giurfa and Dr. Roxana Josens. 1999 - 2003 BSc degree: Biological Sciences, Université Paul Sabatier, Toulouse, France.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 51 Animal Behavior
    Chapter 51 Animal Behavior Lecture Outline Overview: Shall We Dance? • Red-crowned cranes (Grus japonensis) gather in groups to dance, prance, stretch, bow, and leap. They grab bits of plants, sticks, and feathers with their bills and toss them into the air. • How does a crane decide that it is time to dance? In fact, why does it dance at all? • Animal behavior is based on physiological systems and processes. • An individual behavior is an action carried out by the muscular or hormonal system under the control of the nervous system in response to a stimulus. • Behavior contributes to homeostasis; an animal must acquire nutrients for digestion and find a partner for sexual reproduction. • All of animal physiology contributes to behavior, while animal behavior influences all of physiology. • Being essential for survival and reproduction, animal behavior is subject to substantial selective pressure during evolution. • Behavioral selection also acts on anatomy because body form and appearance contribute directly to the recognition and communication that underlie many behaviors. Concept 51.1: A discrete sensory input is the stimulus for a wide range of animal behaviors. • An animal’s behavior is the sum of its responses to external and internal stimuli. Classical ethology presaged an evolutionary approach to behavioral biology. • In the mid-20th century, pioneering behavioral biologists developed the discipline of ethology, the scientific study of how animals behave in their natural environments. • Niko Tinbergen, of the Netherlands, suggested four questions that must be answered to fully understand any behavior. 1. What stimulus elicits the behavior, and what physiological mechanisms mediate the response? 2.
    [Show full text]
  • Current Perspectives in Cognitive Processing by Domesticated Animals
    EDITORIAL published: 23 August 2021 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.736717 Editorial: Current Perspectives in Cognitive Processing by Domesticated Animals Katherine Bruce 1†, David A. Leavens 2† and Sarah T. Boysen 3,4*† 1 Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Wilmington, Wilmington, NC, United States, 2 School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton, United Kingdom, 3 Center for Animal Welfare Science, Department of Pathobiology, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, United States, 4 Comparative Cognition Project, Sunbury, OH, United States Keywords: animal cognition, animals, non-human cognition, cognitive processing, behavior Editorial on the Research Topic Current Perspectives in Cognitive Processing by Domesticated Animals Recently, studies of cognitive processing in domestic animals, especially dogs, seem to have increased exponentially, and research with more typical laboratory animals, such as rats, pigeons, and non-human primates seems to be declining. Funding for behavioral and/or cognitive work with animals has always been challenging, and as costs for animal housing, care, and per diems have increased significantly in the past two decades, researchers have looked to other subject pools that do not require major funding for conducting relevant and important studies that can contribute significantly to our field. Thus, companion animals, notably dogs, have become an important Edited and reviewed by: resource for studies of animal cognition, as well as other accessible and less-studied species like Watanabe Shigeru, goats, horses, and pigs, among others. The Research Topic, entitled “Current Perspectives in Keio University, Japan Cognitive Processing in Domestic Animals,” included 10 papers covering a range of topics and *Correspondence: species, with summaries of each paper provided here.
    [Show full text]
  • Minds Without Spines: Evolutionarily Inclusive Animal Ethics
    Animal Sentience 2020.329: Mikhalevich & Powell on Invertebrate Minds Call for Commentary: Animal Sentience publishes Open Peer Commentary on all accepted target articles. Target articles are peer-reviewed. Commentaries are editorially reviewed. There are submitted commentaries as well as invited commentaries. Commentaries appear as soon as they have been reviewed, revised and accepted. Target article authors may respond to their commentaries individually or in a joint response to multiple commentaries. INSTRUCTIONS FOR COMMENTATORS Minds without spines: Evolutionarily inclusive animal ethics Irina Mikhalevich Department of Philosophy, Rochester Institute of Technology Russell Powell Department of Philosophy, Boston University Abstract: Invertebrate animals are frequently lumped into a single category and denied welfare protections despite their considerable cognitive, behavioral, and evolutionary diversity. Some ethical and policy inroads have been made for cephalopod molluscs and crustaceans, but the vast majority of arthropods, including the insects, remain excluded from moral consideration. We argue that this exclusion is unwarranted given the existing evidence. Anachronistic readings of evolution, which view invertebrates as lower in the scala naturae, continue to influence public policy and common morality. The assumption that small brains are unlikely to support cognition or sentience likewise persists, despite growing evidence that arthropods have converged on cognitive functions comparable to those found in vertebrates. The exclusion of invertebrates is also motivated by cognitive-affective biases that covertly influence moral judgment, as well as a flawed balancing of scientific uncertainty against moral risk. All these factors shape moral attitudes toward basal vertebrates too, but they are particularly acute in the arthropod context. Moral consistency dictates that the same standards of evidence and risk management that justify policy protections for vertebrates also support extending moral consideration to certain invertebrates.
    [Show full text]
  • Displacement Activities During the Honeybee Transition from Waggle Dance to Foraging
    Animal Behaviour 79 (2010) 935–938 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Animal Behaviour journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/anbehav Displacement activities during the honeybee transition from waggle dance to foraging Meredith Root-Bernstein* Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, U.S.A. article info Displacement activities, which are typically locomotory, grooming and object-manipulation behaviours, Article history: have been shown to reduce stress. However, their function within the motivational system remains Received 17 June 2009 unclear. I tested the hypothesis that displacement activities have a functional role during transitions Initial acceptance 3 August 2009 between motivational states, using the honeybee model system. I observed a number of locomotory and Final acceptance 12 January 2010 grooming behaviours performed when foraging honeybees returned to the hive to dance. These focal Available online 19 February 2010 behaviours occurred significantly more frequently during the period of transition from waggle dancing to MS. number: A09-00396R exiting the hive to forage than during the periods before the waggle dance or during the waggle dance. By contrast, the control behaviour, trophallaxis, was distributed across time periods significantly Keywords: differently, occurring with equal frequency in all periods. These results are consistent with the Apis mellifera hypothesis that displacement activities have a functional role during motivational transitions. Evidence displacement activity from other species suggests that the most likely function is facilitation, rather than inhibition, of the honeybee trophallaxis transition. The wide range of species in which displacement activities have been identified suggests that waggle dance they are a universal feature of motivational control.
    [Show full text]
  • The Dancing Bees: Karl Von Frisch, the Honeybee Dance Language
    The Dancing Bees: Karl von Frisch, the Honeybee Dance Language, and the Sciences of Communication By Tania Munz, Research Fellow MPIWG, [email protected] In January of 1946, while much of Europe lay buried under the rubble of World War Two, the bee researcher Karl von Frisch penned a breathless letter from his country home in lower Austria. He reported to a fellow animal behaviorist his “sensational findings about the language of the bees.”1 Over the previous summer, he had discovered that the bees communicate to their hive mates the distance and direction of food sources by means of the “dances” they run upon returning from foraging flights. The straight part of the figure-eight-shaped waggle dance makes the same angle with the vertical axis of the hive as the bee’s flight line from the hive made with the sun during her outgoing flight. Moreover, he found that the frequency of individual turns correlated closely with the distance of the food; the closer the supply, the more rapidly the bee dances. Von Frisch’s assessment in the letter to his colleague would prove correct – news of the discovery was received as a sensation and quickly spread throughout Europe and abroad. In 1973, von Frisch was awarded the Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine together with the fellow animal behaviorists Konrad Lorenz and Niko Tinbergen. The Prize bestowed public recognition that non-human animals possess a symbolic means of communication. Dancing Bees is a dual intellectual biography – about the life and work of the experimental physiologist Karl von Frisch on the one hand and the honeybees as cultural, experimental, and especially communicating animals on the other.
    [Show full text]
  • Benvenuti, Anne (2017) Evolutionary Continuity. Animal Sentience 20(4) DOI: 10.51291/2377-7478.1287
    Benvenuti, Anne (2017) Evolutionary continuity. Animal Sentience 20(4) DOI: 10.51291/2377-7478.1287 This article has appeared in the journal Animal Sentience, a peer-reviewed journal on animal cognition and feeling. It has been made open access, free for all, by WellBeing International and deposited in the WBI Studies Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Animal Sentience 2017.092: Benvenuti on Peña-Guzmán on Animal Suicide Evolutionary continuity Commentary on Peña-Guzmán on Animal Suicide Anne Benvenuti University of Winchester Abstract: The principle of evolutionary continuity states that all animal capacities and behaviors exist — with variations in degree — in continuity with other species. Rather than assuming discontinuity, we should ask why any behavior observed in humans would not be found in at least some other sentient animals under similar conditions. In the case of suicide, the more pertinent issue might be the ethical one: our human responsibility for creating conditions under which other animals might deliberately seek to end their own lives. Keywords: evolutionary continuity, animal behavior, animal consciousness, animal grief, animal suicide Anne Benvenuti is the author of Spirit Unleashed: Reimagining Human-Animal Relations. An instructor of critical thinking, she works at the intersections of evolutionary biology, psychology, neuroscience, and philosophy. She is currently focused on articulating the implications of evolutionary continuity for human relations with nonhuman animals. www.annebenvenuti.com Peña-Guzmán (2017) asks whether animals are capable of suicide, citing established philosophical notions of forethought and intention in the definition of suicide. The idea is that one must form the intention intellectually and then choose freely and deliberately to bring about one’s own death.
    [Show full text]
  • The Vibration Signal, Modulatory Communication and the Organization of Labor in Honey Bees, Apis Mellifera
    Apidologie 35 (2004) 117–131 © INRA/DIB-AGIB/ EDP Sciences, 2004 117 DOI: 10.1051/apido:2004006 Review article The vibration signal, modulatory communication and the organization of labor in honey bees, Apis mellifera Stanley S. SCHNEIDER*, Lee A. LEWIS Department of Biology, University of North Carolina, Charlotte, NC 28223, USA (Received 10 July 2003; revised 17 October 2003; accepted 15 December 2003) Abstract – Cooperative activities in honey bee colonies involve the coordinated interactions of multiple workers that perform different, but interrelated tasks. A central objective in the study of honey bee sociality therefore is to understand the communication signals used to integrate behavior within and among worker groups. This paper focuses on the role of the “vibration signal” in organizing labor in honey bee colonies. The vibration signal functions as a type of “modulatory communication signal”. It is directed toward diverse recipients, causes a non-specific increase in activity that may alter responsiveness to a wide array of stimuli, and thus may influence the performance of many different tasks simultaneously. We review the empirical evidence that the signal is involved in coordinating at least three colony-level activities: food collection and foraging-dependent tasks, queen behavior during swarming and queen replacement, and house hunting by honey bee swarms. Signals that function like the vibration signal may be widespread in highly social insects and social animals in general, and may help to fine-tune the collective decision-making processes that underlie cooperative actions in a wide array of species. vibration signal / modulatory communication / collective decision making / Apis mellifera / multimodal signals 1.
    [Show full text]