Dyer, 2002. the Biology of the Dance Language

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Dyer, 2002. the Biology of the Dance Language 1 Nov 2001 11:4 AR AR147-29.tex AR147-29.SGM ARv2(2001/05/10) P1: GSR Annu. Rev. Entomol. 2002. 47:917–49 Copyright c 2002 by Annual Reviews. All rights reserved THE BIOLOGY OF THE DANCE LANGUAGE Fred C. Dyer Department of Zoology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan 48824; e-mail: [email protected] Key Words Apis, honey bee, communication, navigation, behavioral evolution, social organization ■ Abstract Honey bee foragers dance to communicate the spatial location of food and other resources to their nestmates. This remarkable communication system has long served as an important model system for studying mechanisms and evolution of complex behavior. I provide a broad synthesis of recent research on dance commu- nication, concentrating on the areas that are currently the focus of active research. Specific issues considered are as follows: (a) the sensory and integrative mechanisms underlying the processing of spatial information in dance communication, (b) the role of dance communication in regulating the recruitment of workers to resources in the environment, (c) the evolution of the dance language, and (d ) the adaptive fine-tuning of the dance for efficient spatial communication. CONTENTS INTRODUCTION .....................................................918 THE DANCE AS A SPATIAL COMMUNICATION SYSTEM .................918 SPATIAL-INFORMATION PROCESSING IN DANCE COMMUNICATION ..................................................921 Measurement of Distance .............................................922 Measurement of Direction: The Celestial Compass .........................923 Dance Orientation: Coding Flight Direction into Dances .....................925 Distance Signal: Coding Flight Distance into Dances .......................926 Information Transfer from Dancer to Follower .............................927 Does the Waggle Dance Communicate Height? ............................930 DANCE COMMUNICATION AND DECISION MAKING BY COLONIES ......................................................930 EVOLUTIONARY ORIGIN OF THE DANCE LANGUAGE ...................932 Origins: Insights from the Genus Apis ...................................933 Origins: Insights from Other Social Bees .................................936 ADAPTIVE DESIGN OF DANCES FOR EFFICIENT SPATIAL COMMUNICATION .........................................938 Distance Dialects ....................................................938 Tuned Error in the Divergence Angle ....................................940 Migration Dances ....................................................941 FUTURE DIRECTIONS ................................................942 0066-4170/02/0101-0917$14.00 917 1 Nov 2001 11:4 AR AR147-29.tex AR147-29.SGM ARv2(2001/05/10) P1: GSR 918 DYER INTRODUCTION More than a half century ago, Karl von Frisch put forth the astonishing hypothesis that honey bees (genus Apis) communicate the location of food and other resources through body movements he called dances. These dances, done by foragers on their return to the nest, had been described by many observers over several centuries and had long been assumed to play some role in communication about food. Von Frisch’s realization that dances carry spatial information was surely one of the major discoveries in behavioral biology in the twentieth century. Along with discoveries by other ethologists such as Lorenz and Tinbergen, the elucidation of the dance language opened our eyes to the sophistication and complexity of animal behavior and helped establish the study of behavior as a rigorous empirical science. Furthermore, experimental studies of dance language have provided a window to the subjective world, or Umwelt (115), of the honey bee. This window has provided an unusually clear view not only of what it is like to be a bee, but more generally of what it is like to be an insect. In the 1960s, von Frisch published a masterly review of research on the dance language by him and his students (116a). Since that book’s publication, work on the dance language has been pursued vigorously, leading to a greatly expanded under- standing of the sensory basis of dance communication, the role of the dance in the foraging strategy of the honey bee colony, and the evolution of this remarkable be- havior. Reviews of this more recent research have generally focused on one or a few aspects of the dance language (19, 38, 40, 41, 58, 66, 97). I adopt a broad, synthetic approach to convey the full range of questions that are addressed by research on the dance language and to review major recent developments and current frontiers. My review relies most heavily on studies of the several races of the western hive bee Apis mellifera, which is the species von Frisch studied and is still widely used as a model organism. However, I also incorporate insights from studies of other species of Apis, all of which live in Asia (87, 104). These species share a number of traits with A. mellifera, including highly eusocial colonies, the construction of a wax comb in which brood are reared and food is stored, and communication based on dances. However they also exhibit striking differences in body size, colony size, nest architecture, and properties of the dances. Study of these differences has exposed new insights into both the mechanisms and evolution of the dance (19). THE DANCE AS A SPATIAL COMMUNICATION SYSTEM In a typical instance of dance communication (116a), a successful forager returns home from a rich food source and is greeted by other workers who, if she is carrying nectar, induce her to regurgitate her load to them. If this welcome is enthusiastic enough, the forager begins dancing on the vertical sheet of comb. The dance consists of a series of repeated waggling runs in which the bee moves in a particular direction along the comb while waggling her body from side to side. 1 Nov 2001 11:4 AR AR147-29.tex AR147-29.SGM ARv2(2001/05/10) P1: GSR HONEY BEE DANCE LANGUAGE 919 During the waggling run she also emits a burst of sound by buzzing her wings. After each waggling run, the dancer circles around and realigns herself to begin the next waggling run. As the bee dances, she is encircled by 1–6 other bees that face toward the dancer and follow her movements. The dance followers observe several waggling runs and then leave the nest. Many of these eventually reach the same feeding place that the dancer had found or a feeding place close by. The orientation of the waggling run and its duration are highly correlated with the direction and distance that the forager has flown to the food (Figure 1). Speci- fically the angle of the waggling run relative to the upward direction on the comb Figure 1 Waggle dance of honey bees (Modified from 92). During the flight to food or another resource, honey bees measure the direction (relative to the sun) and distance to the food. Direction is encoded in the orientation of the waggling run relative to gravity (or relative to the sun if celestial cues are visible during the dance). Distance is encoded in the duration of the waggling run. Different populations have different functions relating flight distance to waggling run duration. Other bees observing the dance use the spatial information it contains to fly to the general location of the food and odors carried by the dancer to pinpoint the actual resource. 1 Nov 2001 11:4 AR AR147-29.tex AR147-29.SGM ARv2(2001/05/10) P1: GSR 920 DYER correlates with the direction of flight relative to the sun and sun-linked patterns of polarized sky light. Dancers may also be oriented to these celestial cues directly if they can see them (e.g., when they are dancing on the surface of a reproductive swarm). The duration of the waggling run increases monotonically with flight distance, as can be observed in dances of bees trained to feeders at known flight distances. By placing arrays of feeders or baits in the environment, von Frisch found that recruits searched preferentially at baits near the one being visited by the dancer, suggesting that they had found their way there by using spatial information obtained from the dance. Von Frisch also described another form of the dance that he called a round dance because the bee circled repeatedly in place, occasionally changing the direction of turning. This type of dance is done by bees that have flown to locations near the nest. Von Frisch suggested that round dances signal recruits to search near the nest, but they convey no information about direction. More recent research has revealed that many round dances actually contain directional information (56). Dancers produce sounds during round dances, when their bodies are aligned in the direction corresponding to that of the food. Thus, round dances may best be interpreted as waggle dances with short-distance signals. On the other hand, recruits that have followed round dances search in all directions near the nest (116a), thus they may have difficulty obtaining directional information from such dances. Von Frisch emphasized an important role for odors in the recruitment process. Specifically, he suggested that floral odors and other environmental chemicals cling to the body of the foragers and are detected by the dance followers. Foragers also release an attractive pheromone on their return to a familiar feeding place. The spatial information in the dance allows recruits to get only to the general vicinity of the food; odors allow them to pinpoint the resource indicated by the dancer (116a). A powerful source of odors can even lead recruits to ignore the spatial information in the
Recommended publications
  • Way-Finding in Displaced Clock-Shifted Bees Proves Bees Use a Cognitive Map
    Way-finding in displaced clock-shifted bees proves bees use a cognitive map James F. Cheesemana,b,1, Craig D. Millarb,c, Uwe Greggersd, Konstantin Lehmannd, Matthew D. M. Pawleya,e, Charles R. Gallistelf,1, Guy R. Warmana,b, and Randolf Menzeld aDepartment of Anaesthesiology, School of Medicine, and bSchool of Biological Sciences, University of Auckland, Auckland, 1142, New Zealand; cAllan Wilson Centre for Molecular Ecology and Evolution, Palmerston North, PN4442, New Zealand; dInstitute of Biology–Neurobiology, Free University of Berlin, 14195 Berlin, Germany; eInstitute of Natural and Mathematical Sciences, Massey University, Auckland, 0745, New Zealand; and fDepartment of Psychology and Center for Cognitive Science, Rutgers University, Piscataway, NJ 08854-8020 Contributed by Charles R. Gallistel, May 1, 2014 (sent for review January 22, 2014) Mammals navigate by means of a metric cognitive map. Insects, whether these abilities rest on the construction and use of most notably bees and ants, are also impressive navigators. The a metric cognitive map (11–18). question whether they, too, have a metric cognitive map is Way-finding, the ability to set a course from one familiar but important to cognitive science and neuroscience. Experimentally otherwise arbitrarily chosen location to another location that is captured and displaced bees often depart from the release site in not perceptible from the first, is the signature of a metric cog- the compass direction they were bent on before their capture, nitive map. Displacing an animal from its current location to an even though this no longer heads them toward their goal. When experimenter-chosen location within the territory the animal is they discover their error, however, the bees set off more or less presumed to be familiar with, and then observing its subsequent directly toward their goal.
    [Show full text]
  • The Honey Bee Dance Language
    The Honey Bee Dance Language Background Components of the dance language Honey bee dancing, perhaps the most intriguing aspect of their biology, is also one of the most fascinating behaviors When an experienced forager returns to the colony with in animal life. Performed by a worker bee that has returned a load of nectar or pollen that is sufficiently nutritious to to the honey comb with pollen or nectar, the dances, in warrant a return to the source, she performs a dance on the essence, constitute a language that “tells” other workers surface of the honey comb to tell other foragers where the where the food is. By signaling both distance and direction food is. The dancer “spells out” two items of information— with particular movements, the worker bee uses the dance distance and direction—to the target food patch. Recruits language to recruit and direct other workers in gathering then leave the hive to find the nectar or pollen. pollen and nectar. Distance and direction are presented in separate compo- The late Karl von Frisch, a professor of zoology at the Uni- nents of the dance. versity of Munich in Germany, is credited with interpret- ing the meaning of honey bee dance movements. He and Distance his students carried out decades of research in which they carefully described the different components of each dance. When a food source is very close to the hive (less than 50 Their experiments typically used glass-walled observation meters), a forager performs a round dance (Figure 1). She hives and paint-marked bee foragers.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 51 Animal Behavior
    Chapter 51 Animal Behavior Lecture Outline Overview: Shall We Dance? • Red-crowned cranes (Grus japonensis) gather in groups to dance, prance, stretch, bow, and leap. They grab bits of plants, sticks, and feathers with their bills and toss them into the air. • How does a crane decide that it is time to dance? In fact, why does it dance at all? • Animal behavior is based on physiological systems and processes. • An individual behavior is an action carried out by the muscular or hormonal system under the control of the nervous system in response to a stimulus. • Behavior contributes to homeostasis; an animal must acquire nutrients for digestion and find a partner for sexual reproduction. • All of animal physiology contributes to behavior, while animal behavior influences all of physiology. • Being essential for survival and reproduction, animal behavior is subject to substantial selective pressure during evolution. • Behavioral selection also acts on anatomy because body form and appearance contribute directly to the recognition and communication that underlie many behaviors. Concept 51.1: A discrete sensory input is the stimulus for a wide range of animal behaviors. • An animal’s behavior is the sum of its responses to external and internal stimuli. Classical ethology presaged an evolutionary approach to behavioral biology. • In the mid-20th century, pioneering behavioral biologists developed the discipline of ethology, the scientific study of how animals behave in their natural environments. • Niko Tinbergen, of the Netherlands, suggested four questions that must be answered to fully understand any behavior. 1. What stimulus elicits the behavior, and what physiological mechanisms mediate the response? 2.
    [Show full text]
  • Displacement Activities During the Honeybee Transition from Waggle Dance to Foraging
    Animal Behaviour 79 (2010) 935–938 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Animal Behaviour journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/anbehav Displacement activities during the honeybee transition from waggle dance to foraging Meredith Root-Bernstein* Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, U.S.A. article info Displacement activities, which are typically locomotory, grooming and object-manipulation behaviours, Article history: have been shown to reduce stress. However, their function within the motivational system remains Received 17 June 2009 unclear. I tested the hypothesis that displacement activities have a functional role during transitions Initial acceptance 3 August 2009 between motivational states, using the honeybee model system. I observed a number of locomotory and Final acceptance 12 January 2010 grooming behaviours performed when foraging honeybees returned to the hive to dance. These focal Available online 19 February 2010 behaviours occurred significantly more frequently during the period of transition from waggle dancing to MS. number: A09-00396R exiting the hive to forage than during the periods before the waggle dance or during the waggle dance. By contrast, the control behaviour, trophallaxis, was distributed across time periods significantly Keywords: differently, occurring with equal frequency in all periods. These results are consistent with the Apis mellifera hypothesis that displacement activities have a functional role during motivational transitions. Evidence displacement activity from other species suggests that the most likely function is facilitation, rather than inhibition, of the honeybee trophallaxis transition. The wide range of species in which displacement activities have been identified suggests that waggle dance they are a universal feature of motivational control.
    [Show full text]
  • The Dancing Bees: Karl Von Frisch, the Honeybee Dance Language
    The Dancing Bees: Karl von Frisch, the Honeybee Dance Language, and the Sciences of Communication By Tania Munz, Research Fellow MPIWG, [email protected] In January of 1946, while much of Europe lay buried under the rubble of World War Two, the bee researcher Karl von Frisch penned a breathless letter from his country home in lower Austria. He reported to a fellow animal behaviorist his “sensational findings about the language of the bees.”1 Over the previous summer, he had discovered that the bees communicate to their hive mates the distance and direction of food sources by means of the “dances” they run upon returning from foraging flights. The straight part of the figure-eight-shaped waggle dance makes the same angle with the vertical axis of the hive as the bee’s flight line from the hive made with the sun during her outgoing flight. Moreover, he found that the frequency of individual turns correlated closely with the distance of the food; the closer the supply, the more rapidly the bee dances. Von Frisch’s assessment in the letter to his colleague would prove correct – news of the discovery was received as a sensation and quickly spread throughout Europe and abroad. In 1973, von Frisch was awarded the Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine together with the fellow animal behaviorists Konrad Lorenz and Niko Tinbergen. The Prize bestowed public recognition that non-human animals possess a symbolic means of communication. Dancing Bees is a dual intellectual biography – about the life and work of the experimental physiologist Karl von Frisch on the one hand and the honeybees as cultural, experimental, and especially communicating animals on the other.
    [Show full text]
  • The Vibration Signal, Modulatory Communication and the Organization of Labor in Honey Bees, Apis Mellifera
    Apidologie 35 (2004) 117–131 © INRA/DIB-AGIB/ EDP Sciences, 2004 117 DOI: 10.1051/apido:2004006 Review article The vibration signal, modulatory communication and the organization of labor in honey bees, Apis mellifera Stanley S. SCHNEIDER*, Lee A. LEWIS Department of Biology, University of North Carolina, Charlotte, NC 28223, USA (Received 10 July 2003; revised 17 October 2003; accepted 15 December 2003) Abstract – Cooperative activities in honey bee colonies involve the coordinated interactions of multiple workers that perform different, but interrelated tasks. A central objective in the study of honey bee sociality therefore is to understand the communication signals used to integrate behavior within and among worker groups. This paper focuses on the role of the “vibration signal” in organizing labor in honey bee colonies. The vibration signal functions as a type of “modulatory communication signal”. It is directed toward diverse recipients, causes a non-specific increase in activity that may alter responsiveness to a wide array of stimuli, and thus may influence the performance of many different tasks simultaneously. We review the empirical evidence that the signal is involved in coordinating at least three colony-level activities: food collection and foraging-dependent tasks, queen behavior during swarming and queen replacement, and house hunting by honey bee swarms. Signals that function like the vibration signal may be widespread in highly social insects and social animals in general, and may help to fine-tune the collective decision-making processes that underlie cooperative actions in a wide array of species. vibration signal / modulatory communication / collective decision making / Apis mellifera / multimodal signals 1.
    [Show full text]
  • The Vibration Signal, Modulatory Communication and the Organization of Labor in Honey Bees, Apis Mellifera Stanley Schneider, Lee Lewis
    The vibration signal, modulatory communication and the organization of labor in honey bees, Apis mellifera Stanley Schneider, Lee Lewis To cite this version: Stanley Schneider, Lee Lewis. The vibration signal, modulatory communication and the organiza- tion of labor in honey bees, Apis mellifera. Apidologie, Springer Verlag, 2004, 35 (2), pp.117-131. 10.1051/apido:2004006. hal-00891881 HAL Id: hal-00891881 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00891881 Submitted on 1 Jan 2004 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Apidologie 35 (2004) 117–131 © INRA/DIB-AGIB/ EDP Sciences, 2004 117 DOI: 10.1051/apido:2004006 Review article The vibration signal, modulatory communication and the organization of labor in honey bees, Apis mellifera Stanley S. SCHNEIDER*, Lee A. LEWIS Department of Biology, University of North Carolina, Charlotte, NC 28223, USA (Received 10 July 2003; revised 17 October 2003; accepted 15 December 2003) Abstract – Cooperative activities in honey bee colonies involve the coordinated interactions of multiple workers that perform different, but interrelated tasks. A central objective in the study of honey bee sociality therefore is to understand the communication signals used to integrate behavior within and among worker groups.
    [Show full text]
  • Mechanosensory Hairs in Bumblebees (Bombus Terrestris) Detect Weak Electric Fields
    Mechanosensory hairs in bumblebees (Bombus SEE COMMENTARY terrestris) detect weak electric fields Gregory P. Suttona,1,2, Dominic Clarkea,1, Erica L. Morleya, and Daniel Roberta aSchool of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TH, United Kingdom Edited by Richard W. Aldrich, The University of Texas, Austin, TX, and approved April 26, 2016 (received for review January 29, 2016) Bumblebees (Bombus terrestris) use information from surrounding ecologically relevant magnitudes cause motion in both the antennae electric fields to make foraging decisions. Electroreception in air, a and body hairs, but only hair motion elicits a commensurate neural nonconductive medium, is a recently discovered sensory capacity of response. From these data we conclude that hairs are used by insects, yet the sensory mechanisms remain elusive. Here, we inves- bumblebees to detect electric fields. tigate two putative electric field sensors: antennae and mechanosen- sory hairs. Examining their mechanical and neural response, we Results show that electric fields cause deflections in both antennae and hairs. Bumblebee Hairs and Antennae Mechanically Respond to Electric Hairs respond with a greater median velocity, displacement, and Fields. The motion of the antennae and sensory hairs in response angular displacement than antennae. Extracellular recordings from to applied electric fields was measured by using a laser Doppler the antennae do not show any electrophysiological correlates to vibrometer (LDV) (Fig. 1). LDV measures the vibrational velocity these mechanical deflections. In contrast, hair deflections in response (v) of structures undergoing oscillations, which was transformed into to an electric field elicited neural activity. Mechanical deflections of displacement (x) and angular displacement (θ)(SI Results).
    [Show full text]
  • Behavioral Neuroscience Tali Kimchi [email protected]
    Behavioral Neuroscience Tali Kimchi [email protected] Lecture 1: Introduction to animal behavior- from classical ethology to neuroethology Lecture 2: Social behavior and brain sexual dimorphism- Hormonal and genetic regulation Lecture 3: The new era in the study of brain mechanisms underlying social behavior in animal models- pros and cons Behavioral Neuroscience Introduction to animal behavior: from classical ethology to neuroethology Why should we care for animal behavior Knowledge of animal behavior = human survival For example, understanding behavior of animals hunted for food * Cave animal paintings (ca. 30,000-10,000 BC) Why should we care for animal behavior May shed light on human beings behavior- many behaviors are conserved across species (e.g. territoriality/aggressive behavior, dominance hierarchy, sexual behavior) Conserved behavior: aggressive behavior- size comparison What is behavior? • The total movements made by the intact animal (Niko Tinbergen) • Anything an organism does that involves action (either alone or with other animals) and/or response to a stimulus" (Wallace et al 1991) -Stimulus can be external (environment) or internal. For example: Searching for food is triggered by food smell (external) when hormonal changes (internal) sign hunger state • Behavior can be defined as innate (e.g. reflex) or learned behavior What is behavior? • Behavior is crucial to the survival of the individual and of all species, serving a few main purposes that allow animals to: mate, find food (eat), avoid predators, raise
    [Show full text]
  • Karl Von Frisch
    EntomologistBugBug BUMBLEBEEKARLSWEAT VON FRISCH BEE MIMIC ofof theof the the (1886 - 1982)(LaphriaSubject: flavicollis)Honey bees Month Field:(Agapostemon Ethology Nationality: spp.) Austrian MonthMonth The round dance (left) indicates resources that are a short distance from the hive, while the waggle dance (right) communicates longer distances. Credit: Oliver Feldwick Karl von Frisch is most famous for his discovery and interpretation of the honey bee dance language, for which he and his colleagues were awarded the Nobel Prize in 1973. His experiments relied on training paint-marked bees to feeders at known locations relative to their hive. Then, through careful observation, von Frisch discovered that honey bees per- form a ‘waggle dance’ to communicate the distance and direction of food resources to other Agapostemon is a widespread preference. Like other members of cases, multiple females may share foragers. von Frisch is less well known for many other important contributions that he made Atgenus first of bees glance, in North youAmerica, may flavicollisHalictidae, ,they is commonly are short-tongued found aand single beetles. nest entrance, Once but they each have towithbelieve the 14 studyknown this ofspeciesis honeya bumblebee. across bees, the which acrossand included have the difficulty eastern investigations Unitedextracting States. nectarinto honeyindividualcollected bee builds their vision and prey, andprovisions they the return her studyUnitedHowever, ofStates a microsporidianit’s and shortCanada. straight Most parasite
    [Show full text]
  • The 1973 Nobel Prize for Physiology Or Medicine
    RESEARCH NEWS The 1973 Nobel Prize for Physiology or Medicine The 1973 Nobel prize for Physiology rel.ated flowers. His thorough experi- leading students. When a colony of bees or Medicine has been awarded jointly ments in the 1920's settled in the af- is swarming, scouts fly out from the to three zoologists: Karl von Frisch, firmative the long-standing question teeming cluster of bees that have left 86 years old, of the University of Mu- whether fish could hear. Unsophisti- their former hive and search for a nich; Konrad Lorenz, 69 years old, of cated in the best sense, these experi- cavity where thousands, of bees can the Max Planck Institute for Behavioral ments have been amply confirmed in fly to establish a new colony. When a Physiology at Seewiesen, near Munich; later years with appropriate monochro- scout has located a suitable cavity, she and Nikolaas Tinbergen, 66 years old, mators and hydrophones. An ardent signals its location by the same dance of the Department of Zoology at Ox- Darwinian who successfully defended pattern used for food. Individual bees ford University, for their discoveries his views at his oral examination in exchange information about the suit- concerning organization and elicitation philosophy against a professor who did ability and location of various cavities, of individual and social behavior pat- not believe in evolution, von Frisch sometimes the same bee acting alter- terns. The award is a new departure was motivated by a naturalist's faith nately as transmitter and receiver of for the Nobel Committee of the Karo- that phenomena such as the colors and information.
    [Show full text]
  • Foundations of Animal Behaviour Roz Dakin Niaux Caves, France We've
    Foundations of animal behaviour Niaux caves, France Roz Dakin [email protected] 10-15,000 years ago Montgomerie Lab people travelled 5 km deep into tunnels... Room 4325 Biosciences (moving to 3520) We’ve always observed animals The course website roslyndakin.com/biol321 Lecture notes posted here To download them before class, answer the latest poll on the course blog The course website Breakdown roslyndakin.com/biol321 8% lab attendance and participation policies, schedule, syllabus 7% online discussion comments lab/assignment info 12.5% project 1 50% course blog 22.5% project 2 labs + assignments 15% midterm 1 50% tests • updates 15% midterm 2 • discussions 20% final test BIOL_321F on Moodle Foundations of animal behaviour • we’ll use this site to hand in project reports (these must be in pdf format) History of the field • we’ll also use Moodle for posting grades What are we studying, anyway? How do we study it? Some things we got right... Ethology comes of age in the 30s Ethology: the study of the behaviour of animals First written records of in their natural habitats mutualism and brood (from Greek) parasitism Ethos = character or habit ...but he believed some goats that live near the Logia = the study of sea never drink Aristotle 384-322 BC Ethology comes of age in the 30s Karl von Frisch Founding fathers: Honeybee social organization & communication Karl von Frisch, Konrad Lorenz Worked on the waggle dance from 1920s-40s & Niko Tinbergen Konrad Lorenz Niko Tinbergen Pioneering studies of genetically programmed Puzzled and fascinated by the navigational behaviours (instincts) in birds abilities of digger wasps (c.
    [Show full text]