History of English: Language Change and Development

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

History of English: Language Change and Development History of English: Course Structure Language Change 1. Language Change & Development and Development 2. Old English Nigel Musk 3. Middle English Teachers’ Programme 6161--9090 hp Depar tmen t of Cult ure & Commun icati on 4. Early Modern English Linköping University Language Variation & Change Change as a Social Phenomenon Language variation is a prerequisite for change Language change is most often described in linguistic terms, yet There i s al ways l anguage vari ati on w ithin a commun ity or soc ie ty language and language change is essentially a SOCIAL for many different (social) reasons: phenomenon. Both language and language change arise through communication. differing needs (occupation, leisure, interests , etc. ) People tend to adjust their language to become more like each differing social standing (sociolects) other (accommodation) differing contacts with other communities, e .g . with differing regional varieties (dialects) & languages Accommodating to others can operate across phonology (accent), lexis (vocabulary), grammar (morphology & syntax) But even one and the same person shows a tendency to speak and discourse (discursive features) (and write) differently in different social contexts/constellations Also at a societal level, the more social upheaval, the more Variation is facilitated byyggp the relative ease of geographical and lingu is tic c hange social mobility (mobility isn’t a new phenomenon!) Aspects of Language Subject to Categories of Change Change Distinction often made between: Phonology Internal change – including the normal “drift of language” Lexis (vocabulary) External change – due to language contact Morphology Grammar Syntax Discourse Conditioned Phonological Internal Change: Phonology Change 1 Assimilation – adjacent sounds become more alike A speaker tends not to make more effort than is necessary This can lead for example to co-articulation effects becoming e.g. OE blīðs/blīþs : bliss, permanent. OE gōdspell ‘good news’ : gospel Therefore a distinction can be made between: Palatalisation of velar consonants before front vowels: e.g. cheese OE. cēse = OS. kāsi, Du. kaas G. Käse ), conditioned (or combinatory) change, e.g. through co- yellow OE. geolu = OS. gelo, Du. gel, G. gelb articulation effects Modern distinction in past tense /d/ : /t/ : /ɪd/ unconditioned (or spontaneous) change Tendency for intervocalic consonants to become voiced (vowels are always voiced) Conditioned Phonological Conditioned Phonological Change 2 Change 3 Other phoneme losses Simplification of consonant clusters (elision) OE : ModE Reduction & loss of final unstressed vowels hlāf : loaf hlūd : loud hnecca : neck hnitu : nit hring : ring hrōf : roof hlǣfdige : lady niht : night OE sunu : son OE sunne : sun But note that question words retained breathiness longer: what, OE mōna : moon when, where OE steorra : star cnēo(w) : knee cnotta : knot gnætt : gnat includes vowels in plurals e.g. OE dagas : days camb,comb : comb, wamb,womb : womb with vowel reduction (weakening) first to –e and then -ə and Modern example: yod-dropping, e.g. suit, lute then lost Unconditioned Phonological Unconditioned Phonological Change 1 Change 2 Metathesis – reversal of two (mostly) adjoining phonemes Epenthesis – addition of a phoneme in the middle of a word e.g. OE ācsian : ask e.g. OE æmtig : empty OE brid(d) : bird, Sēo eorþe wæs æmtig (from Genesis) OE wæps (variation in OE too: wæsp) : wasp OE spin(e)l : spindle, hros (cf. OE hors, ON hross, Sw russ) : horse OE þunor : thunder Modern example: pretty (good) – ‘purty’ (good) Modern examples: glottal stop [ʔ]: something [sʌmʔθɪŋ], epenthetic vowel [ə]: in ScE/IrE film [fɪləm] Unconditioned Phonological Unconditioned Phonological Change 3 Change 4 Sound shifts Chain shifts Sound “laws” whereby the same phoneme changes in all words Push (to avoid merging) or pull effects(to mergers) (under the same conditions – stress, position, etc.) desk [ɛə/ɪə] busses Tendency to preserve [ɛ] bosses symmetry of phonological [ʌ] [ɔ] system – to optimise the bat head ppghonological space Northern Cities Chain Shift [æ] [ɑ] bloc k socks Unconditioned Phonological Change 5 Internal Change: Lexis 1 Mergers of phonemes Reasons for lexical change Front close vowels /i/ : /y/ (unrounding) New ideas and innovations give rise to new words OE lȳtel : little OE yfel : evil Through polysemy – words have different or multiple meanings, OE synn : sin e.g. common words like get, go Over time one or more meanings may fall out of use and new meanings develop Great vowel shift included one merger Compare: speak [spɛːk] and feed [feːd]inME] in ME By association with other words, e.g. metaphors, metonymy Disadvantages of mergers: more homonyms arise = potential detriment to communication TidtbtiffidththttTo avoid taboo, negative, offensive words or those that are too e.g. to : two : too; their : there; son (OE sunu) : sun (sunne) direct - euphemisms Reasons for lexical change 1 Reasons for lexical change 2 Metaphors - association by similarity Metonymy - association by contiguity, e.g. cause & effect, concrete & abstract toast [LME] There is a connection between the toast you eat and the toast you make with a raised glass. Toast is based on Latin eavesdrop [OE] In Old English eaves, then spelled efes, was a torrere ‘to parch, scorch, dry up’, the source also of torrid singular word, but the -s at the end made people think it was a [E17th], and torrent [LME] a rushing or ‘boiling’ flow of water. ‘To plural, which is how we treat it today. If you eavesdrop you parc h’ was t he ea rliest m eanin g of th e En gli sh w or d, an d bef or e secretly listen to a conversation. The word was formed in the long it was used to describe browning bread in front of a fire. early 17th century from the old word eavesdropper [LME], ‘a Drinking toasts goes back to the late 17th century, and person who listens from under the eaves’. Eavesdropper came originated in the practice whereby a drinker would name a lady from the noun eavesdrip or eavesdrop, ‘the ground on to which and request that all the people present drink her health. The water drips from the eaves’. This was a concept in an ancient idea was that the lady's name flavoured the drink like the pieces law which banned building closer than two feet from the of spiced toast that people sometimes added to wine in those bdboundary o f your ldiland, in case you damage d your ne ihb'ighbour's days. Oxford Dictionary of Word Origins land by ‘eavesdrop’. Oxford Dictionary of Word Origins Reasons for lexical change 3 Internal Change: Lexis 2 Euphemism - (the use of) a mild, comforting, or evasive Patterns of lexical change expression that takes the place of one that is taboo, negative, offensive, or too direct: Gosh God, Broadening of meaning – refers to a wider range of meanings terminate kill, sleep with have sex with, pp,ass water, (referents) relieve oneself urinate. Narrowing of meaning WORD COMMON EUPHEMISMS lavatory bog (slang), comfort station, convenience, little boys' room, little Amelioration house, loo, restroom (AmE), washroom (AmE), water closet (WC) die depart this life, give up the ghost, kick the bucket (slang), pass away, pass on Deterioration Pocke t Fo wl er' s Mode rn En gli sh Usage Patterns of lexical change 1 Patterns of lexical change 2 Broadening of meaning 1 Broadening of meaning 2 food [OE] Recorded since the beginning of the 11th century, food small [OE] A word recorded since around AD 700. In Old English it is related to fodder [OE] and foster [OE], originally found in the could refer to something slender or narrow as well as something sense ‘feed, nourish’. It can refer to mental as well as physical more generally of less than usual size. From the 16th century nourishment—the expression food for thought to indicate small beer was a term for weaker beer, the sort that people som ethin g th at dese rv es seri ous co n si der ati on h as been in use dra nk fo r br eakf ast wh en w ater suppli es w er e un saf e. In since the early 19th century. Cannon fodder for soldiers Macbeth Iago dismisses women as fit only to ‘chronicle small regarded as expendable dates from the First World War. beer’, and from this sort of use developed the sense of Oxford Dictionary of Word Origins something insignificant. [… ] Oxford Dictionary of Word Origins food what is taken to support life. Late OE. fōda :- *fōðan-, a small (dial.) slender, thin: †narrow; of limited size or extent; of fine unique formation, the synon. words in other Gmc. langs. being f. texture OE.; of low strength or power XII. OE. smæl = OS. (Du.), *fōðjan FEED . v iz. ON. fœ ði, fœð a OHG. smal (G . sc hma l), ON . sma lr. Concise Oxford Dictionary of English Etymology Concise Oxford Dictionary of English Etymology Patterns of lexical change 3 Patterns of lexical change 4 Narrowing of meaning 1 Narrowing of meaning 2 In Anglo-Saxon times starve simply meant ‘to die’, especially a [ME] A poison does not necessarily need to be in liquid form, but in lingering death from hunger, cold, disease, or grief. People early use the word meant a drink or medicine, specifically a continued to use the word in this way for many centuries, and in potion with a harmful or dangerous ingredient. The source was northern English dialect starve can still mean ‘to die of cold’. Old French poison ‘magic potion’, from Latin potio, also the OOodctoayoxford Dictionary of W odOgsord Origins sour ce of poti on [ME ]. Th e sayin g on e m an' s m eat i s an oth er starve †die OE.; die of hunger; cause to die of hunger, cold, etc. man's poison has been around for centuries and was being XVI.
Recommended publications
  • Social Dimensions of Language Change
    Social dimensions of language change Lev Michael 1 Introduction Language change results from the differential propagation of linguistic vari- ants distributed among the linguistic repertoires of communicatively inter- acting individuals in a given community. From this it follows that language change is socially-mediated in two important ways. First, since language change is a social-epidemiological process that takes place by propagating some aspect of communicative practice across a socially-structured network, the organization of the social group in question can affect how a variant propagates. It is known, for example, that densely connected social net- works tend to be resistant to innovations, where as more sparsely connected ones are more open to them. Second, social and cultural factors, such as lan- guage ideologies, can encourage the propagation of particular variants at the expense of others in particular contexts, likewise contributing to language change. The purpose of this chapter is to survey our current understanding of the social factors that affect the emergence and propagation of linguistic variants, and thus language change, by bringing together insights from vari- ationist sociolinguistics, sociohistorical linguistics, linguistic anthropology, social psychology, and evolutionary approaches to language change. It is im- portant to note that there are, as discussed in Chapter 1, important factors beyond the social ones discussed in this chapter that affect variant propaga- 1 tion and language change, including factors related to linguistic production and perception, and cognitive factors attributable to the human language faculty (see Chapter 1). 1.1 Theorizing variation and language change As Weinreich, Labov, and Herzog (1968) originally observed, theories of lan- guage that assume linguistic variation to be noise or meaningless divergence from some ideal synchronically homogeneous linguistic state { to be elim- inated by `averaging' or `abstraction' { encounter profound difficulties in accounting for language change.
    [Show full text]
  • The Origin of Language and Communication
    Athena and Eve — Johnson Papers lution, editors Jones, Martin and Pilbeam conceded that The origin of ‘there are no non-human languages’, and then went on to observe that ‘language is an adaptation unique to humans, and yet the nature of its uniqueness and its biological basis language and are notoriously difficult to define’ [emphasis added].3 In his book, The Symbolic Species: The Co-Evolution of Language communication and the Brain, Terrance Deacon noted: ‘In this context, then, consider the case of Brad Harrub, Bert Thompson and human language. It is one of the most distinctive Dave Miller behavioral adaptations on the planet. Languages evolved in only one species, in only one way, with- By age four, most humans have developed an ability out precedent, except in the most general sense. to communicate through oral language. By age six And the differences between languages and all other 4 or seven, most humans can comprehend, as well as natural modes of communicating are vast.’ express, written thoughts. These unique abilities of What events transpired that have allowed humans to communicating through a native language clearly speak, while animals remain silent? If we are to believe the separate humans from all animals. The obvious evolutionary teaching currently taking place in colleges and question then arises, where did we obtain this dis- universities around the world, speech evolved as a natural tinctive trait? Organic evolution has proven unable to process over time. Yet no-one is quite sure how, and there elucidate the origin of language and communication. are no known animals that are in a transition phase from Knowing how beneficial this ability is to humans, non-speaking to speaking.
    [Show full text]
  • Sociolinguistics: Language Change Wardhaugh Chapter 8
    Sociolinguistics: Language Change Wardhaugh chapter 8 (Labov’s homepage: http://www.ling.upenn.edu/~wlabov/home.html) Non-prestige dialects of English and Language Dispersion A common preconception about non-prestige dialects or colloquial forms of English is that they are unsystematic and 'lazy' forms of language, and that they either reflect or even encourage illogical thought. Over the past 4 decades many linguists have studied non-prestige and colloquial forms of English (and other languages) and arrived at the conclusion that these varieties are just as systematic as prestige varieties of English, that their 'non-standard' features are typically features found in prestige varieties of other languages, and that there is no basis for claiming that their phonology, morphology or syntax reflects 'illogical' or lazy thinking. We focus here on the dialect of English that has received the most attention: it is known variously as "Black English Vernacular" (BEV), "African American Vernacular English" (AAVE) or "Ebonics". The first two terms are the most commonly used terms used in sociolinguistic research; the third term has achieved wide recognition in the wake of a highly controversial resolution of the Oakland (CA) Board of Education involving the role of Ebonics in K-12 education. In what follows I will use the term AAVE, which is the most widespread term in current linguistic research. Some excellent readily accessible articles on AAVE are available on-line. Note1: the colloquial English spoken by African American communities spans a wide varieties of styles, often identical to or barely distinguishable from the English spoken by other ethnic groups, including the prestige white variety.
    [Show full text]
  • Language Change in Apparent and Real Time: the Community and the Individual
    University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics Volume 10 Issue 2 Selected Papers from NWAVE 32 Article 17 2005 Language Change in Apparent and Real Time: The Community and the Individual Natalie Schilling-Estes Follow this and additional works at: https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl Recommended Citation Schilling-Estes, Natalie (2005) "Language Change in Apparent and Real Time: The Community and the Individual," University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics: Vol. 10 : Iss. 2 , Article 17. Available at: https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol10/iss2/17 This paper is posted at ScholarlyCommons. https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol10/iss2/17 For more information, please contact [email protected]. Language Change in Apparent and Real Time: The Community and the Individual This working paper is available in University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics: https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol10/iss2/17 Language Change in Apparent and Real Time: The Community and the Individual1 Natalie Schilling-Estes 1 Introduction Since William Labov's ( 1963) pioneering work on language variation and change in Martha's Vineyard and New York City in the 1960s, most varia­ tionist investigations of language change in progress have investigated change in apparent time---that is, in the speech of individuals of different generations at a given moment in time---rather than real time. This approach is based on the APPARENT TIME CONSTRUCT, which holds that, for the most part, the core features of an individual's vernacular language variety are so­ lidified for life by the time they reach their late teens. The robustness of the apparent time construct has been borne out in a number of studies over the past four decades (e.g.
    [Show full text]
  • Language Change: Progress Or Decay?
    Language change: progress or decay? Third edition JEAN AITCHISON Professor of Language and Communication, University of Oxford published by the press syndicate of the university of cambridge The Pitt Building, Trumpington Street, Cambridge, United Kingdom cambridge university press The Edinburgh Building, Cambridge CB2 2RU, UK www.cup.cam.ac.uk 40 West 20th Street, New York, NY 10011–4211, USA www.cup.org 10 Stamford Road, Oakleigh, Melbourne 3166, Australia Ruiz de Alarcón 13, 28014 Madrid, Spain © Cambridge University Press 1991, 2001 This book is in copyright. Subject to statutory exception and to the provisions of relevant collective licensing agreements, no reproduction of any part may take place without the written permission of Cambridge University Press. First published by Fontana Press in 1981 Second edition published by Cambridge University Press in 1991 Reprinted 1992, 1993, 1994, 1995, 1998 Third edition 2001 Printed in the United Kingdom at the University Press, Cambridge Typeface 10/12 pt Photina [gc] A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library ISBN 0 521 79155 3 hardback ISBN 0 521 79535 4 paperback Contents Preface page ix Part 1 Preliminaries 1 The ever-whirling wheel 3 The inevitability of change 2 Collecting up clues 19 Piecing together the evidence 3 Charting the changes 37 Studying changes in progress Part 2 Transition 4 Spreading the word 55 From person to person 5 Conflicting loyalties 68 Opposing social pressures 6 Catching on and taking off 84 How sound changes spread through a language
    [Show full text]
  • The Origins and the Evolution of Language Salikoko S. Mufwene
    To appear in a shortened version in The Oxford Handbook of the History of Linguistics, ed. by Keith Allan. I’ll appreciate your comments on this one, because this project is going to grow into a bigger one. Please write to [email protected]. 6/10/2011. The Origins and the Evolution of Language Salikoko S. Mufwene University of Chicago Collegium de Lyon (2010-2011) 1. Introduction Although language evolution is perhaps more commonly used in linguistics than evolution of language, I stick in this essay to the latter term, which focuses more specifically on the phylogenetic emergence of language. The former, which has prompted some linguists such as Croft (2008) to speak of evolutionary linguistics,1 applies also to changes undergone by individual languages over the past 6,000 years of documentary history, including structural changes, language speciation, and language birth and death. There are certainly advantages, especially for uniformitarians, in using the broader term. For instance, one can argue that some of the same evolutionary mechanisms are involved in both the phylogenetic and the historical periods of evolution. These would include the assumption that natural selection driven by particular ecological pressures applies in both periods, and social norms emerge by the same 1 Interestingly, Hombert & Lenclud (in press) use the related French term linguistes évolutionnistes ‘evolutionary linguists’ with just the other rather specialized meaning, focusing on phylogenesis. French too makes a distinction between the more specific évolution du langage ‘evolution of language’ and the less specific évolution linguistique ‘linguistic/language evolution’. So, Croft’s term is just as non-specific as language evolution and évolution linguistique (used even by Saussure 1916).
    [Show full text]
  • Syntactic and Semantic Features for Code-Switching Factored Language Models Heike Adel, Ngoc Thang Vu, Katrin Kirchhoff, Dominic Telaar, and Tanja Schultz
    This is the author’s version of an article that has been published in this journal. Changes were made to this version by the publisher prior to publication. The final version of record is available at http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/TASLP.2015.2389622 IEEE/ACM TRANSACTIONS ON AUDIO, SPEECH AND LANGUAGE PROCESSING 1 Syntactic and Semantic Features For Code-Switching Factored Language Models Heike Adel, Ngoc Thang Vu, Katrin Kirchhoff, Dominic Telaar, and Tanja Schultz Abstract This paper presents our latest investigations on different features for factored language models for Code-Switching speech and their effect on automatic speech recognition (ASR) performance. We focus on syntactic and semantic features which can be extracted from Code-Switching text data and integrate them into factored language models. Different possible factors, such as words, part-of-speech tags, Brown word clusters, open class words and clusters of open class word embeddings are explored. The experimental results reveal that Brown word clusters, part-of-speech tags and open-class words are the most effective at reducing the perplexity of factored language models on the Mandarin-English Code-Switching corpus SEAME. In ASR experiments, the model containing Brown word clusters and part-of-speech tags and the model also including clusters of open class word embeddings yield the best mixed error rate results. In summary, the best language model can significantly reduce the perplexity on the SEAME evaluation set by up to 10.8% relative and the mixed error rate by up to 3.4% relative. I. INTRODUCTION The term Code-Switching (CS) denotes speech with more than one language.
    [Show full text]
  • Studies in Historical Linguistics and Language Change Grammaticalization, Refunctionalization and Beyond
    Studies in Historical Linguistics and Language Change Grammaticalization, Refunctionalization and Beyond Edited by Dorien Nieuwenhuijsen and Mar Garachana Printed Edition of the Special Issue Published in Languages www.mdpi.com/journal/languages Studies in Historical Linguistics and Language Change Studies in Historical Linguistics and Language Change. Grammaticalization, Refunctionalization and Beyond Special Issue Editors Dorien Nieuwenhuijsen Mar Garachana MDPI • Basel • Beijing • Wuhan • Barcelona • Belgrade Special Issue Editors Dorien Nieuwenhuijsen Mar Garachana Utrecht University Barcelona University The Netherlands Spain Editorial Office MDPI St. Alban-Anlage 66 4052 Basel, Switzerland This is a reprint of articles from the Special Issue published online in the open access journal Languages (ISSN 2226-471X) from 2018 to 2019 (available at: https://www.mdpi.com/journal/languages/ special issues/Lingustics LanguageChange) For citation purposes, cite each article independently as indicated on the article page online and as indicated below: LastName, A.A.; LastName, B.B.; LastName, C.C. Article Title. Journal Name Year, Article Number, Page Range. ISBN 978-3-03921-576-8 (Pbk) ISBN 978-3-03921-577-5 (PDF) Cover image courtesy of Bob de Jonge. c 2019 by the authors. Articles in this book are Open Access and distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license, which allows users to download, copy and build upon published articles, as long as the author and publisher are properly credited, which ensures maximum dissemination and a wider impact of our publications. The book as a whole is distributed by MDPI under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons license CC BY-NC-ND. Contents About the Special Issue Editors ....................................
    [Show full text]
  • 29 Language Death and Dying
    764 Walt Wolfram 29 Language Death and Dying WALT WOLFRAM For as long as humans have used language to communicate, particular lan- guages have been dying. In an important sense, obsolescence is simply part of the natural life cycle of language. At the same time, language death has taken on heightened significance in recent decades because it is occurring in epi- demic proportions.1 According to Krauss (1992), up to 90 percent of the world’s estimated 6,000 languages face possible extinction in this century, including 80 percent of the languages of North America. By comparison with endangered biological species, language endangerment is astronomical; biologists estimate that less than 8 percent of all mammals and less than 3 percent of all birds are imperiled. Nonetheless there is little public concern over the state of the world’s languages while concern for endangered biological species is considered an international crisis. The proliferation of symposia, special interest groups, and publications (e.g. Dorian 1989, Hale et al. 1992, Robins and Uhlenbeck 1991, Wolfram 1997, Grenoble and Whaley 1998a) dedicated to obsolescing language varieties in the last couple of decades points to the escalating severity of language endan- germent. On a theoretical level, the loss of a language variety without ad- equate documentation deprives language scientists of an essential database for inquiry into the general knowledge of human language; on a social level, it deprives people of one of the most integral components of diverse cultural behavior (Hale 1998). Both the theoretical and humanistic issues underscore the concern for language death here, but our focus is on describing and under- standing the process of language death as a sociolinguistic phenomenon rather than the theoretical and/or practical implications of the situation.
    [Show full text]
  • A Probabilistic Approach to Language Change
    A Probabilistic Approach to Language Change Alexandre Bouchard-Cotˆ e´∗ Percy Liang∗ Thomas L. Griffithsy Dan Klein∗ ∗Computer Science Division yDepartment of Psychology University of California at Berkeley Berkeley, CA 94720 Abstract We present a probabilistic approach to language change in which word forms are represented by phoneme sequences that undergo stochastic edits along the branches of a phylogenetic tree. This framework combines the advantages of the classical comparative method with the robustness of corpus-based probabilis- tic models. We use this framework to explore the consequences of two differ- ent schemes for defining probabilistic models of phonological change, evaluating these schemes by reconstructing ancient word forms of Romance languages. The result is an efficient inference procedure for automatically inferring ancient word forms from modern languages, which can be generalized to support inferences about linguistic phylogenies. 1 Introduction Languages evolve over time, with words changing in form, meaning, and the ways in which they can be combined into sentences. Several centuries of linguistic analysis have shed light on some of the key properties of this evolutionary process, but many open questions remain. A classical example is the hypothetical Proto-Indo-European language, the reconstructed common ancestor of the modern Indo-European languages. While the existence and general characteristics of this proto-language are widely accepted, there is still debate regarding its precise phonology, the original homeland of its speakers, and the date of various events in its evolution. The study of how languages change over time is known as diachronic (or historical) linguistics (e.g., [4]). Most of what we know about language change comes from the comparative method, in which words from different languages are compared in order to identify their relationships.
    [Show full text]
  • INTD0112 Introduction to Linguistics
    Announcements INTD0112 ► Can’t think of any. Introduction to ► Questions? Linguistics Lecture #23 May 10th, 2007 Language death Language death ► You should have been dazzled by now by the ►Question: How does a language die and remarkable range of linguistic variation that we become extinct? discussed over the course of the term. Answer: When it ceases to be acquired as a ► Guess what! It could’ve been much more dazzling if we still had access to thousands of “extinct” native language by children. languages. ►But why does that happen? ► Estimates on the number of languages that could Several reasons. Linguists typically talk have existed today if none had become extinct about four types of language death. range from 10,000 to 600,000, with 150,000 being considered realistic by most researchers. Sudden language death Radical language death ► Sudden language death occurs when all of the ► Radical language death is similar to sudden speakers of the language die or get killed. This language death in its abruptness. Rather than the was the case with Tasmanian. In 1887, the last speakers dying, however, they all stop speaking speaker of a Tasmanian language died, and so did the language. Often the reason for this is survival the language. under the threat of political repression or even ► Another example occurred in 1962 when a (no genocide. doubt imported) influenza killed all speakers of ► Speakers, to avoid being identified as “natives”, Trumai, up until then spoken in a single village in simply stop speaking their native language. Brazil. Only 10 speakers were left. Children are thus unable to learn that language, and the language ends up dying.
    [Show full text]
  • LNGT0101 Introduction to Linguistics Language Change
    11/26/2012 Announcements LNGT0101 Introduction to Linguistics • HW5 is due on Wed (5pm by e‐mail; in class for hard copy). • I extended the original due date for the LAP report till Friday Dec 7th. Deadline: 12noon. • A linguistics talk this week: ‘The So‐called Japanese Subject Marker ‐ga‘ by Prof. Masako Hoye in Hillcrest 103 at 4:30pm. Lecture #20 Nov 26th, 2012 2 So, do you guys speak English? • Yes! • And so did Shakespeare: Language change A man may fish with the worm that hath eat of a king, and eat of the fish that hath fed of that worm. • Translation? Not really! 3 4 So, do you guys speak English? So, do you guys speak English? • Yes! And so did Chaucer: • Yes! And so did the author of Beowulf: Whan that Aprille with his shoures soote Wolde guman findan πone πe him on The droght of March hath perced to the roote. sweofote sare geteode. • Translation? • Translation? When April with its sweet showers He wanted to find the man who harmed The drought of March has pierced to the root. him while he slept. 5 6 1 11/26/2012 Languages change over time A puzzle regarding language change • So, you get the obvious point: Languages do change • Functionally, language change is problematic over time. since it hinders communication. • There are two main questions with regard to language change: • Socially, language change is also problematic, First, why does a language change? because it engenders negative stereotypes Second, how does a language change? that could lead to discrimination.
    [Show full text]