CHAPTER 8 DEVELOPMENT

IN THIS CHAPTER distribute

REPRESENTATION OF KNOWLEDGE DEVELOPMENT OF EVENTor MEMORY IN INFANCY Script-Based Memory Preference for Novelty as an Indication of Role of Parents in “Teaching” Children to Memory Remember Kicking Up Their Heels CHILDREN AS EYEWITNESSES Deferred Imitation as a Measure of Memory Age Differences in Children’s Eyewitness Neurological Basis of Infant Memory post, INFANTILE Age Differences in Suggestibility Why Can’t We Remember Events From Final Thoughts on Children as Eyewitnesses Infancy and Early Childhood? REMEMBERING TO REMEMBER Infantile Amnesia and Hypnotic Age Regression copy,KEY TERMS AND CONCEPTS SUGGESTED not

lthough rare, some people have one or personally poignant memory to my mother. She more vivid memoriesDo from infancy or listened carefully and then told me that I had Aearly childhood. -One of us (DB), for never had the croup; my younger brother Dick example, recalls a memory stemming from the had the croup as a toddler. I was about 4 years first year of life. My memory is of me as a sick old at the time. My “memory” was a reconstruc- baby. I had the croup (something like ­bronchitis). tion—and of an event I had only observed, not When I this memory, I can feel the conges- one I had actually experienced. Most memories tion in my chest, hear the vaporizer whir, smell of infancy, it seems, are like mine—reconstruc- the Vicks ProofVapoRub, and see the living room of tions of events that never happened or, perhaps, my grandparents’ house while looking through that happened to someone else, but what one the bars of my crib. The memory is like a mul- is remembering is the retelling of that event by tisensory snapshot. I have no story to tell, only other people. the recall of an instant of my life as a sickly It’s hard to overestimate the significance of Draftbaby. My mistake was relating this vibrant and memory for our lives. Our memories define for

300 Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. CHAPTER 8 MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 301

us what we’ve done, who and what we know, dynamically interacting factors that vary over and even who we are. Nearly all acts of time. Despite the wealth of information we involve memory. A 4-month-old looks longer at have about children’s memory today, we are a new picture than at one he has seen repeat- just beginning to develop an appreciation for edly, a 3-year-old recounts her class field trip to the factors and contexts that influence children’s a bakery, a 7-year-old lists for her mother the memory performance and the development of names of all her classmates in preparing to send those abilities. Valentine’s Day cards, and a high school sopho- In this chapter, we examine research and more attempts to remember everything his father theory dealing with the development of mem- asked him to get at the corner store. Each of these ory in children. We open the chapter with a diverse activities involves memory. The 4-month- brief examination of the differentdistribute ways knowl- old can recognize a new only if he has edge can be represented in memory. We then some notion that it is different from a previously examine memory developmentor in infancy, fol- experienced but currently unseen one. The mem- lowed by a look at children’s implicit memory. ory requirements for the three older children are ­Children’s memories for events—specifically, more demanding, but all involve retrieving from autobiographical memories—are discussed memory some previously stored information. next. We also review research on children as Memory is not a unitary phenomenon. Infor- eyewitnessespost, and the factors that influence mation must be encoded and possibly related their suggestibility. This is followed by a brief with other information known to the individual. look at the development of “remembering to What knowledge already resides in memory remember,” or . Through- influences the ease with which new information out the chapter we describe social-cultural is stored and later retrieved. copy,influences on memory, as well as the adap- Memory development is one of the oldest, tive nature of memory, from an evolutionary continuously researched topics in the field of perspective. . But how it is researched,not and the theoretical focus of the researchers, is much different today than it was 30 years ago. In the previous chapter,Do we discussed that REPRESENTATION how much children remember- is influenced OF KNOWLEDGE by developmental differences in basic infor- mation-processing abilities of , stor- As we saw in Chapters 5 and 6, how people rep- age, and retrieval and by the strategies they resent information changes with age, and this is a use to intentionally learn information. Today, central issue in . We take it for however, there is an increasing awareness that granted that knowledge is represented somehow in memory isProof used for specific purposes and in spe- our brains and that we can access it whenever we cific social contexts (Ornstein & Light, 2010). want. But knowledge is not quite so simple. Is It is not enough merely to assess children’s everything we know represented in such a way memory behavior in one context, particularly that we can easily (and consciously) retrieve it on Drafta context devoid of social meaning. How and demand? Might there be some things we know what ­children remember depends on a host of that our thoughts and behavior that are

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. 302 CHILDREN’S THINKING

difficult or impossible to bring to ? is, you just can’t ask someone to remember And if so, how do these things develop? something he or she knows only implicitly). (1987, 2005) proposed that Consider the classic case of amnesia, where a information in long-term memory can be rep- person walks into a police station and announces resented in one of two general ways: declarative to the desk sergeant that he has no idea who he is memory and nondeclarative memory. ­Declarative or how he got there—the perfect beginning of a memory refers to facts and events and comes mystery novel. Now consider a person who knows in two types: and semantic very well who she is and can carry on a conversa- memory. Episodic memory—literally, memory tion just fine, but 5 minutes after meeting you, for episodes, such as what you had for breakfast she has forgotten who you are and anything you this morning, the gist of a conversation you had talked about. Yet were you to meetdistribute with her every with your mother last night, and the Christmas day and teach her how to tie a complicated knot, visit to your grandparents when you were 5 years after a week of practice sheor would be able to old—can be consciously retrieved. Such memory tie the knot expertly without having any aware- is sometimes called , which refers ness of doing it before. These are both forms of to the fact that it is available to conscious aware- amnesia ( in the first case and ness and can be directly (explicitly) assessed by in the second), but different tests of recall or recognition memory. memory systemspost, are involved. In the first case, the refers to our knowledge of person has lost his personal history. He remem- , rules, and concepts. So, for instance, bers nothing about “the .” In the second case, the meaning of the term democracy or the rules the person’s sense of self and personal history is for multiplication are examples of semantic intact. However, she can learn no new informa- memory. For instance, the definition for the copy,tion other than some procedures (tying knots), word perfunctory is part of my (KC) semantic and she will have no recollection of having ever memory, but my recollections of the events sur- learned them. In both cases, people keep their rounding my the word (preparingnot for knowledge of their language, multiplication tables, comprehensive exams in graduate school) are and basic facts of the world. For example, if they part of my episodic memory. were ­American citizens, they would likely know The second general type ofDo memory has been the current president and who the first U.S. presi- termed nondeclarative memory- (or procedural dent was. The example of the person with retro- memory). Nondeclarative memory refers to grade amnesia who forgot who he was displays a knowledge of procedures that are unconscious. deficit in a form of explicit/declarative memory— For example, some have argued that the learn- specifically, episodic memory. The example of the ing and memory observed in classical and oper- person with anterograde amnesia displays access ant conditioningProof are unconscious, as are many to past episodic memories but an inability to form familiar routines once they have become well new ones (usually because of damage to the hippo- practiced (tying one’s shoe, for example). Such campus), although she can form new is sometimes called implicit memory, memories. The existence of these dissociations— which refers to the fact it is unavailable to instances where one form of memory is impaired Draft­conscious awareness (“memory without aware- while others­ remain intact—provides evidence for ness”) and can be assessed only indirectly (that independent memory systems that serve specific

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. CHAPTER 8 MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 303

FIGURE 8.1 Classification of different types of memory. The human memory system can be divided into two general types of memory: explicit, or declarative, which is available to consciousness, and implicit, or nondeclarative, which is not available to consciousness, with both types able to be divided further. Age differences are greater in explicit than implicit memory.

Types of long-term memories

Implicit distribute Explicit (nondeclarative/ (declarative) prodecural) With conscious recall Without consciousor recall

Facts–general Personally Skills–motor Dispositions–classical knowledge experienced events and cognitive and operant (“semantic memory”) (“episodic memory”) post, conditioning effects

Source: © Cengage Learning.

functions. Figure 8.1 shows the various compo- nents of the explicit and implicit memory systems.copy,Section Review In addition to tapping different types of mem- • Memory is multifaceted, involves a host of ories, different areas of the brain are involved other cognitive operations, and is involved in declarative and nondeclarative notmemories in all complex forms of thinking. (Schacter, 1992). This supports the argument • Declarative (explicit) memory, which includes that memory is not a single phenomenon (that is, episodic and semantic memory, is proposed to be available to consciousness and is of- domain general) but, rather, is a set of domain- Do ten contrasted with nondeclarative (proce- specific mental operations- that may show differ- dural or implicit) memory, which is unavail- ent patterns of developmental function. able to consciousness. Ask Yourself . . . MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 1. When you ride a bike, what type of memory Proof are you using? IN INFANCY 2. Think about some movies you’ve seen that depict different types of memory loss or Babies obviously remember things. The ques- amnesia (The Vow, for instance, or Finding or ). What types of amne- tions of interest are when and under what con- Nemo Memento sia are depicted? What form of memory is ditions infants demonstrate memory and how Draft impaired, and what remains intact? long these memories last. For example, research

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. 304 CHILDREN’S THINKING

examining infants’ search behavior, as reflected within the capacity of most infants during their by object permanence tasks (see Chapter 4), indi- first months of life. cates changes in memory with age during the first Perhaps the most influential work dem- year (Diamond, 1985). Recall Adele Diamond’s onstrating memory in infants using the findings that the amount of delay necessary to ­preference-for-novelty paradigm is that of yield the A-not-B error increased with each suc- Joseph Fagan (1973, 1974). One study showed cessive month between 7 and 12 months of age. that 5- and 6-month-old babies formed visual Although Diamond proposed that developmental memories following brief exposures (5 to 10 sec- differences in the ability to inhibit a prepotent onds) and that these memories lasted as long as response were partly responsible for this effect, 2 weeks (Fagan, 1974). Fagan’s procedures have she also acknowledged that such results reflect been widely used by researchers, distributeand later work age changes in memory during this 6-month suggested a relationship between individual dif- period (Diamond, Cruttenden, & Neiderman, ferences in preference for noveltyor during infancy 1994). and and . This research is examined in Chapter 13. Subsequent research has shown that even Preference for Novelty as 1-month-old infants demonstrate relatively an Indication of Memory long-lived memories.post, For example, in one study, the mothers of 1- and 2-month-old infants read The bulk of research assessing infant memory, par- their babies one of two nursery rhymes over the ticularly in the early days of such research, used course of 2 weeks. Infants were then brought variants of the habituation/dishabituation para- into the laboratory, and their preferences for digm discussed in previous chapters. To review, copy,the familiar versus a novel nursery rhyme were infants’ declines as a result of repeated tested. This was done by permitting infants to presentation of a stimulus (habituation) but returns choose hearing either the familiar or a novel to its previously high levels when a new stimulusnot is nursery rhyme by modifying their sucking on a presented (dishabituation). Such a finding not only pacifier (for example, increase sucking rate to indicates that infants can discriminate between the hear one rhyme, decrease sucking rate to hear two stimuli but also implicates Domemory, in that the the other). After a 3-day delay between the time discrimination is being made- between one stimulus infants last heard their mothers read the familiar that is physically present and another that is pres- nursery rhyme and being tested in the lab, even ent only memorially. In a related procedure, infants 1-month-old babies showed a preference for the are familiarized with a stimulus and later shown familiar rhyme, indicating memory for the audi- two stimuli: the original, familiarized stimulus and tory event (Spence, 1996). a novel one. As in the habituation/dishabituation It’s worth noting here that infants’ prefer- paradigm,Proof preference (or longer looking times) for ence in Melanie Spence’s (1996) study was for the novel stimulus is (usually) taken as evidence of the familiar stimulus, not for the novel one. memory for the original. Using these preference- To demonstrate memory, all that is required for-novelty paradigms, memory for visual stimuli is that infants show a decided bias for one Drafthas been found for some newborns. Basic visual stimulus over the other. As we noted in ear- memory is an early developing ability—certainly lier chapters, infants’ preferences for novel

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. CHAPTER 8 MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 305

versus familiar objects/events varies as a func- FIGURE 8.2 Mean proportion of total looking tion of their age and stage of learning in a task, time infants directed to the novel stimulus during among other factors (Bogartz & Shinskey, the 1-minute, 1-day, and 1-month delays. Chance is 0.5. mean looking time significantly greater than 1998). For example, Mary Courage and Mark chance reflects a preference for novelty. Mean looking Howe (2001) showed 3.5-month-old infants a time significantly less than expected by chance reflects a ­stimulus for 30 seconds and then tested their preference for familiarity. preference for the old (familiar) versus a new (novel) stimulus after delays of 1 minute, 1 0.8 day, and 1 month. The researchers reported a bias for the novel stimulus after the 1-minute 0.7 delay, no bias after the 1-day delay, and a bias distribute 0.6 for the familiar stimulus after the 1-month delay (see Figure 8.2). Following the theoriz- 0.5 or ing of Lorraine Bahrick and Jeffrey Pickens (1995), Courage and Howe (2001) interpreted 0.4 these findings as indicating that infants’ atten- tion to novel versus familiar stimuli varies as a Stimulus to Novel 0.3 function of the strength of the familiar infor- post, mation in long-term memory at the time of Time Looking Total of Proportion 0.2 testing. Infants will attend more to novel stim- 0.1 uli when memory traces are strong (after the 1 Minute 1 Day 1 Month 1-day delay) and attend more to familiar stim- Retention Interval uli when the memory traces are weak (after thecopy, Source: Courage, M. L., & Howe, M. L. (2001). Long-term retention 1-month delay). Null effects (that is, neither in 3.5-month-olds: Familiarization time and individual differences in attentional style. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 79, a preference for the novel nor a preference 271–293. © 2001 Elsevier. Reprinted with permission. for the familiar stimuli) reflect a transitionnot phase in which both stimuli compete equally for attention. Richard Aslin and colleagues Kicking Up Their Heels have since characterized thisDo phenomenon as the Goldilocks effect, which- we introduced in Other research by Carolyn Rovee-Collier and Chapter 4 (and see Kidd et al., 2012). Accord- her colleagues has used conditioning tech- ing to their interpretation, infants are implic- niques, demonstrating retention over relatively­ itly motivated to maintain intermediate rates long periods for very young infants (see Rovee- of stimulation and encoding. In this way, they Collier & Cuevas, 2009, for a review). In their avoid wasting cognitive resources on overly conjugate-reinforcement procedure, a rib- simply orProof overly complex events. When the bon is tied to an infant’s ankle and connected stimulus is still familiar, they will attend to the to a mobile that is suspended over the crib relatively novel stimulus, but after some time (see Photo 8.1). Infants quickly learn that the and , they will return their attention mobile moves when they kick their feet, and Draftto the familiar stimulus, strengthening the fad- they soon make repeated kicks, controlling the ing memory trace as a result. movement of the mobile overhead. In a typical

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. 306 CHILDREN’S THINKING

experiment, for the first 3 minutes the ribbon 3-minute baseline? If the kicking rate is high on is not connected to the mobile, so kicks do these delayed trials, it reflects memory; if it is not cause it to move (baseline nonreinforce- low, it reflects forgetting. ment period). This is followed by a 9-minute Rovee-Collier and her colleagues have used reinforcement period in which the ribbon and this procedure successfully with infants as young mobile are connected, and infants quickly learn as 2 months of age. For example, ­Margaret to kick to make the mobile move. What will ­Sullivan, Carolyn Rovee-Collier, and Derek happen when the infants are hooked up to Tynes (1979) varied the delayed memory test the apparatus hours or days later? Will they between 48 and 336 hours (2 weeks) with resume kicking (even when the ribbon is not 3-month-old infants. The researchers reported connected to the mobile), or will their level of no forgetting by these young infantsdistribute for as much kicks be comparable to that observed during the as 8 days, and some babies displayed memory for the full 2-week interval. Inor related work, infants as young as 8 weeks demonstrated retention of PHOTO 8.1 An infant connected to a mobile in an experiment to assess memory used by Carolyn conditioned responses during a 2-week period, Rovee-Collier and her colleagues. although evidence of memory was obtained only under optimal conditions (distributing training overpost, several sessions) (Vander Linde, ­Morrongiello, & Rovee-Collier, 1985). These results indicate that young infants can remember events over long intervals, although these skills do improve over the first several months of life. copy,Subsequent research by Rovee-Collier and her colleagues focused on the role of context in infants’ memories. How similar must the learn- not ing environment and testing environment be for babies to show retention? This was assessed by a study in which different aspects of the learn- Do ing environment (in this case, the playpens in - which the infants were tested) were changed between the time of learning and the time of testing (Rovee-Collier et al., 1992). Six-month- old infants were tested using the conjugate- reinforcement procedure described earlier, but the testing situation was made very distinctive. Proof Infants sat in an infant seat that was placed in a playpen. The sides of the playpen were draped with a distinctive cloth (for example, yellow liner with green felt squares). Some infants Draft were tested 24 hours later with the same cloth, Thanks to Carolyn Rovee-Collier whereas others were placed in the playpen that

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. CHAPTER 8 MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 307

was draped with a blue liner and vertical red but changes in color did not (for other exam- felt stripes. The results of this experiment are ples, see Bhatt, Rovee-Collier, & Shyi, 1994; presented in Figure 8.3 (expressed as kicking Fagen et al., 1997). Rovee-Collier and Gary rate during testing relative to kicking rate dur- Shyi (1992) speculated that infants’ reliance ing baseline). As you can see, infants in the on specific aspects of a context prevents them no change condition demonstrated significantly from retrieving memories in “inappropriate” better retention of the learned behavior than situations. This may be especially important infants in the context change condition, indi- for infants with poor inhibitory abilities, who cating the important role that context plays in would be apt to retrieve previously acquired reinstating infants’ memories. memories (actions) in a wide range of often- Rovee-Collier et al. (1992) performed six inappropriate situations unless theredistribute were some other experiments, varying different aspects of potent constraints (such as context specificity) the context. They concluded that infants do not on the memory system. Theor role of inhibitory respond to the context as a whole but, rather, factors in infant cognition has been addressed seem to process individual components of a by several researchers, most notably Adele context. For instance, changes in visual patterns ­Diamond, and some of this work is discussed (for example, stripes versus squares) or reversal in Chapter 2. of the foreground and background (for exam- Rovee-Collierpost, and her colleagues have also ple, yellow liner with green squares versus green used conjugate-reinforcement procedures to liner with yellow squares) disrupted memory, assess age-related changes in long-term memory in infants. For example, in addition to the mobile FIGURE 8.3 Mean baseline ratios for 6-month- task, they developed the train task, which uses old infants in a no-change (control) condition copy,the same logic as the mobile task but is appro- and a context-change condition. The higher ratio priate to use with older babies. In the train task, for infants in the no-change condition reflects greater infants sit in front of a display that includes a retention of the behaviors that were learned 24 hours miniature train set. They can learn to move the earlier. not train around the set by pressing a lever in front of them, and retention is tested as it is in the 2.5 Do mobile task, with infants sitting in front of the - display after a delay and the rate with which 2.0 they press the lever (when it is now not con- nected to the train) being measured. With these two comparable tasks, it is now possible to ask 1.5 how long memories last for infants of different Baseline Ratio ages. Figure 8.4 presents the maximum number Proof of weeks that infants from 2 to 18 months of age 1.0 demonstrated retention on the mobile and train No Change Context Change tasks. As can be seen, the duration of infants’ Source: Adapted from Rovee-Collier, C., Schechter, A., Shyi, memories showed gradual but steady increases C.-W. G., & Shields, P. (1992). Perceptual identification of Draftcontextual attributes and infant memory retrieval. Developmental with age, reflecting a continuously developing Psychology, 28, 307–318. memory system.

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. 308 CHILDREN’S THINKING

FIGURE 8.4 Maximum duration of retention this research are quite striking, showing that from 2 to 18 months of age. Filled circles show infants form long-term memories for these retention on the mobile task, and open circles show novel actions that can last as long as 1 year (see retention on the train task. Six-month-olds were trained and tested on both tasks. P. J. Bauer, 2002, 2007). These results suggest that preverbal infants and toddlers do repre- 14 sent events in their long-term memories and, 13 under the right conditions, can access those 12 memories months later. 11 At what age do infants display deferred imi- 10 tation? Although results vary withdistribute the specific 9 task used, infants as young as 9 months old will 8 imitate simple actions up to 5 weeks later, and 6-month-olds have been shownor to imitate simple 7 Train task 6 behaviors after a 24-hour delay and remember 5 events for up to 8 weeks (see Rovee-Collier & 4 Giles, 2010, for a review). Maximum Retention (weeks) 3 Once infantspost, observe an action, how long 2 do those memories last? The answer depends 1 Mobile task primarily on the age of the infant, with older 0 infants being able to remember more compli- 181512963 cated sets of behaviors over longer periods of Age (months) time. For example, Patricia Bauer and her col- Source: Rovee-Collier, C. (1999). The development of infant mem-copy,leagues (P. J. Bauer, 2002, 2007; P. J. Bauer or y. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 80–85. et al., 2000, 2001) showed infants a series of three-step sequences. For example, the not researcher placed a bar across two posts, hung Deferred Imitation a plate from the bar, and then struck the plate as a Measure of Memory with a mallet (see Photo 8.2). After delays rang- Do ing from 1 to 12 months, the babies were given Another task that has -been used to assess the objects, and their imitation was measured. infants’ long-term memory is deferred imita- About half of the 9-month-olds imitated the sim- tion, which refers to imitating a model after pler two-sequence actions after a 1-month delay, a significant delay. In most deferred-imitation but these infants required at least three expo- experiments, infants watch as an experimenter sures to the events to achieve this level of per- demonstrates some novel behavior with an formance. Rate of deferred imitation increased unfamiliarProof object. At some later time, they substantially for 13-, 16-, and 20-month-old are given the object. If they display the novel infants, with older infants demonstrating higher behavior more than a control group of infants levels of deferred imitation during each delay who had not previously been shown the object, interval than younger infants did (P. J. Bauer Draftit implies that the study group formed a long- et al., 2000). Figure 8.5 shows results of long- term memory for the action. The results of term retention as a function of age of infant

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. CHAPTER 8 MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 309

PHOTO 8.2 Bauer’s three-step sequence as shown by the gong task. Infants watched as a model performed a three- step sequence: placing the bar across two posts, hanging a plate on the bar, and striking the plate with a mallet. Infants were later given the opportunity to reproduce the sequence, demonstrating evidence of deferred imitation, and thus memory.

distribute or

post,

copy, not Do

Thanks to Patricia Bauer -

and length of delay from this study. These find- Is deferred imitation a type of explicit, declara- ings are similar in form to those Rovee-Collier tive, memory? Recall from earlier in this chapter and colleaguesProof reported using the conjugate- that explicit memory is contrasted with implicit reinforcement procedures. They illustrate that memory. The former represents a deliberate infants are able to form long-term memories attempt to remember and is potentially avail- early in life and that the ability to retain these able to conscious awareness, whereas the latter Draftmemories increases gradually during the first is often referred to as memory without aware- 2 years. ness. Most researchers who have investigated

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. 310 CHILDREN’S THINKING

FIGURE 8.5 Percentage of 13-, 16-, and 20-month-old infants displaying deferred imitation of three-step sequences as a function of length of delay.

100

90 13 month

80 16 month

70 20 month 60 50 distribute 40 Ordered Recall Ordered or 30

20 rcentage of Children Demonstrating of Children rcentage Pe 10

0 post, 1 month 3 month 6 month 9 month 12 month Length of Delay

Sources: Data from Bauer, P. J., Wenner, J. A., Dropik, P. L., & Wewerka, S. S. (2000). Parameters of remembering and forgetting in the transition from infancy to early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in , 65(4) (Serial No. 263). Figure from Bauer, P. J. (2002). Long-term recall memory: Behavioral and neuro-developmental changes in the first 2 years of life. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 137–141. copy,

deferred imitation in older infants believe it to be tasks? Just like they do on declarative memory a form of nonverbal explicit memory (P.not J. Bauer, tasks—they fail them (McDonough et al., 1995). Larkina, & Deocampo, 2011; Hayne, 2007). If These findings suggest that deferred-imitation tasks so, it would be using the same type of represen- tap the same memory system as do declarative tasks tational system as that used byDo older children on used with older children and (for example, tasks. - “Tell me what you had for breakfast this morn- How can one tell the difference between explicit ing”) and “that the neurological systems underlying and implicit memory in preverbal children? Perhaps long-term recall are present, in at least rudimentary one can’t definitively, but support for this distinc- form, by the beginning of the second year of life” tion comes from a study of adults with anterograde (Schneider & Bjorklund, 1998, p. 474). amnesia, like the we described earlier who learned to Prooftie complicated knots but had no recol- lection of doing so. People with anterograde amne- Neurological Basis of Infant Memory sia are unable to acquire new declarative memories (memory with awareness) but perform well on The pattern of deferred imitation shown by Draftimplicit memory tasks (memory without aware- infants ages 9 months to approximately 2 years ness). So how do they perform on deferred-imitation is consistent with what is known about brain

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. CHAPTER 8 MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 311

development during this time (see Bachevalier, begin to coalesce, with development continuing 2014; P. J. Bauer, 2009). Long-term memory well into the third year. The relatively gradual requires the integration of brain activity from development of these brain structures correlates multiple sites, including the and the with the relatively gradual improvement in long- and structures within the tempo- term retention of infants during this same period ral lobe. Most parts of the hippocampus develop (P. J. Bauer et al., 2000; Liston & Kagan, 2002). early and are adultlike before birth, although the The neuromaturational model of mem- (part of the hippocampus), which ory development, as we’ve discussed so far, plays an important role in episodic memory, con- is sometimes interpreted as a transition from tinues to develop after birth and into adulthood implicit to explicit memory, controlled by two (Aimone & Gage, 2011; Richmond & Nelson, different neuroanatomical memorydistribute systems 2007). In fact, one layer of the dentate gyrus that mature at different rates. An alternative includes at birth only about 70% of the number ecological model holds thator the basic memory of cells that it will have in adulthood (Seress, process does not change ontogenetically (but 2001), meaning that about 30% of the neurons is emergent, and quite continuous, as depicted in this layer will be generated after birth. As we in Figure 8.4). Rather, what immature infants saw in Chapter 2, it was once believed that no versus adults select to encode for learning does. new neurons were generated after birth. How- Rovee-Collierpost, and Giles (2010) has argued that ever, we now know that neurogenesis continues the lack of long-term, declarative memory in throughout life in the hippocampus, particularly infancy is not a memory deficit or indicator of in the dentate gyrus. Once neurons are generated, immaturity but represents “rapid forgetting . . . synapses between neurons need to be formed, and an evolutionarily selected survival-related strat- this reaches its peak in the dentate gyrus duringcopy, egy that facilitates young infants’ adaptation to the fourth or fifth month after birth. Synapses are their rapidly changing niche and enables them then pruned to levels by about 10 months. to shed the excessive number of recent, rapidly The early developing hippocampusnot presum - formed associations that are potentially use- ably underlies the deferred imitation of simple less, irrelevant, or inappropriate” (p. 203). She actions by 6-month-olds (Collie & Hayne, describes the first 9 months of life as a period 1999), but other brain areas mustDo mature before of “exuberant learning,” when synaptic prun- infants can retain more complicated- information ing occurs, marking a developmental change in for longer periods. what young infants need to learn and remem- The frontal cortex is important in the encod- ber around 10 months, as they transition to a ing and retrieval of declarative memories, and period of perceptual tuning (a phenomenon we this develops more slowly than the hippocampus discussed in Chapter 4). At this point, infants (Monk, Webb, & Nelson, 2001). For instance, have acquired more stable associations between synaptic densityProof reaches its peak in the frontal stimuli in the world. As these associations begin cortex between 15 and 24 months after birth, to crystallize, infants have a firmer knowledge and significant pruning takes place during child- base onto which they can attach new infor- hood (P. Huttenlocher, 1979). Not until the mation, aiding in the retention of long-term, Draftsecond year of life do these and other systems declarative memory. Although the question (hippocampus, prefrontal lobe, ) remains open, the findings we’ve described so

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. 312 CHILDREN’S THINKING

far make it apparent that the memory systems showed immediate imitation similar to that that support implicit and explicit memory are of home-reared children but displayed sig- both present from early in infancy. nificantly poorer levels of deferred imitation. Understanding the nature of infants’ long- As we noted in Chapter 2, postnatal experi- term memory is important for our appreciation ence plays a critical role in getting the brain of the effects of early experience (P. H. Miller, “hooked up” properly, and when infants 2014). For instance, some evidence of the rela- receive less-than-optimal experiences during tion between brain and memory development their first year of life, their brains and memo- in infancy comes from studies of premature ries suffer. infants. For example, Michelle de Haan and Indeed, Rovee-Collier has made the argu- her colleagues (2000) administered deferred- ment that even though youngerdistribute infants ­forget imitation tasks to three groups of 19-month-old more rapidly, how long they can retain infor- children. One group of babies was born full mation is determined by ortheir experience, not term. Another group of infants had been born their level of maturation. She views long-term premature, after 35 to 37 weeks of gestation, memory as an emergent process, whereby but were physiologically healthy. A third group infants’ retention is affected by the number had been born premature after 27 to 34 weeks and strength of associations between the and were physiologically immature. The three target memorypost, and other events, as well as groups of infants showed comparable levels the frequency and rate at which the infant is of immediate imitation; however, they differed repeatedly exposed to the target information. on deferred-imitation tasks, with the preterm Rovee-Collier and Cuevas (2009) have argued infants, especially those who were physiologi- that younger infants forget more rapidly, not cally immature, having significantly lower lev-copy,because they are neurologically less mature els of memory. These results suggest that the but because they have less experience and thus declarative memory system of these preterm fewer associations to which the memory can infants was adversely affected by deprivingnot be linked. them of the last several weeks of their prenatal environment. Other research indicates Dothat the memory abilities (and their underlying- brain struc- Section Review tures; see Kolb et al., 2012) of infants are • Infants display memory in habituation/ impaired as a result of living in deprived dishabituation and preference-for-novelty environments. For example, the deferred- paradigms shortly after birth. Conditioning imitation abilities of 20-month-old techniques, particularly conjugate reinforce- infants, some of whom had been adopted ment procedures, have been used to dem- about 8 Proofmonths earlier from a Romanian onstrate memory in infants as young as orphanage, where they had experienced 3 months for periods as long as 2 weeks, extreme deprivation, were impaired relative to with infants’ memories in these situations being greatly influenced by context effects. typically developing children (Kroupina et al., • Deferred-imitation tasks have shown that Draft2010). Toddlers who had spent their first year older infants can retain information over or so in conditions of extreme deprivation

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. CHAPTER 8 MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 313

life. We wrote back to the woman, assuring her relatively long periods, with infants’ long- that her son’s inability to remember events much term memory for actions increasing gradu- before his fourth birthday is quite normal and ally over the first 2 years of life. Maturation of an area of the hippocampus, the dentate that just because her child can’t remember his gyrus, as well as of the frontal cortex, is experiences from this age doesn’t mean that they associated with infants’ ability to display didn’t have an impact on him. deferred imitation. It is not just memories from infancy that escape us. For the most part, we are unable to Ask Yourself . . . remember much of anything before the age of 3. How do researchers use infants’ preference 3.5 or 4 years (P. J. Bauer, 2014). Most of us for novelty to assess newborns’ memory? have a few memories for events distributethat happened 4. What role does context in infants’ between the ages of about 3 and 6 years, but memories? How have researchers assessed these memories are very few in comparison with this? or 5. What is a deferred-imitation task? How do what we can remember from after this time we know it is a measure of explicit memory? ­(Pillemer & White, 1989). What we lack spe- cifically are autobiographical memories, which refer to personal and long-lasting memories and are thepost, basis for one’s personal life history (K. Nelson, 1996). If children younger than 2 INFANTILE AMNESIA can form explicit memories, why are they unable to retain those memories as autobiographical The results of Rovee-Collier, P. J. Bauer, and episodes that they can recall later in life? their colleagues indicate that infants do, indeed,copy, JoNell Usher and (1993) studied form long-term memories. This finding, on the this lack of memory for the early years by ques- surface, contradicts the phenomenon of infantile tioning college students about experiences they amnesia—the seeming inability of adultsnot to recall had had early in life—experiences such as the specific events or episodes from early childhood. birth of a younger , a stay in the hospital, Such amnesia is bothersome not only to research- a move, or the death of a family member. ers but also to some parents.Do For example, my To assess recall, a series of questions was asked wife and I (DB) used to -write a column for a about each event the person had experienced parenting magazine and would occasionally get (Who told you your mother was going to the letters from concerned parents. One letter came hospital to give birth? What were you doing from a woman who was worried because her when she left? Where were you when you first 10-year-old son could remember very little from saw the new baby?). The percentage of questions his days. She said that she and her hus- college students could answer increased substan- band hadProof always tried to be good parents but tially the older the person was when he or she thought her son’s inability to remember things had experienced the event. Usher and Neisser from early childhood was an indication that either concluded that the earliest age of any meaningful they hadn’t done a very good job after all or, recall was about 2 years for the birth of a sibling Draftworse yet, they had done a truly terrible job and or a hospitalization and 3 years for the death of her son was repressing this painful period of his a family member or a family move.

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. 314 CHILDREN’S THINKING

Most people cannot remember anything from the first few years of their lives, but this does not mean that this information is being repressed.

distribute or

Source: CALVIN AND HOBBES © Watterson. Reprinted with permission of UNIVERSAL PRESS SYNDICATE. All rights reserved. post, A more recent series of longitudinal studies con- memories but fail to recall them over long delays. ducted by Carol Peterson and colleagues involved Thus, it seems that we should not overlook the asking children 4 to 13 years old about their earli- role that forgetting plays in . est three memories (Peterson, Grant, & Boland, Of course, many people do claim to have memo- 2005). Two years later, researchers followed up copy,ries from infancy or even from birth (or before). with these children (Peterson, Warren, & Short, But are these memories to be trusted? The story 2011). They found that, as children aged, so did that opened this chapter (a sick baby looking out their earliest childhood memory. Thatnot is, older the bars of his crib) is an example of how a vivid children had later ages of first memory than did memory from infancy can be a reconstruction of younger ones, and there was an overall shift of an event and not the recollection of an event that about 1 year for the earliest providedDo memories actually happened to the “rememberer.” Are all from the time of the first interview- to the second. early memories like this? And, perhaps more to There was also some inconsistency in children’s the point, does the inability to retrieve memories earliest memories from one time to the next, par- from infancy contradict the new research find- ticularly in the youngest age groups (former 4- to ings of long-term retention in early infancy? 7-year-olds). However, in the oldest group, almost one quarter of children recalled at least two of the same threeProof memories during both interviews. Why Can’t We Remember Events Thus, children’s recollection of early childhood From Infancy and Early Childhood? memories seems to become more stable with age. These findings are consistent with others (for (1963) was the first to speculate Draftexamples, Scarf et al., 2013; Tustin & Hayne, on the reason for infantile amnesia, proposing 2010) showing that young children form episodic that the events of infancy and early childhood are

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. CHAPTER 8 MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 315

rife with sexual overtones toward one’s mother using language. Perhaps the inability to convert and are just generally so traumatic that they are motor memories into verbal ones prevents chil- actively repressed. We protect our adult egos, dren from recalling events from infancy. claimed Freud, by preventing these disturbing Evidence for this latter interpretation has memories from rising to consciousness. been accumulating over the past 2 decades or so Few scientists today agree with Freud’s inter- (Hayne & Simcock, 2009; Simcock & Hayne, pretation. To the contrary, research shows that 2002). For example, Gabrielle Simcock and we are more likely to remember events from Harlene Hayne (2002) showed children rang- early childhood that are enhanced by emotion, ing in age from 27 to 39 months sequences as well as those more chronologically, themati- of actions and then interviewed them 6 and cally, and contextually coherent (Peterson et al., 12 months later for their verbal distributeand nonverbal 2014). Other more cognitively based interpreta- memory of the events. Despite having the ver- tions propose the possibility that (a) information bal ability to describe theiror previous experience, is not stored for long-term retention before about none of the children did so spontaneously. To 2 years of age, and (b) information is encoded the extent that children did talk about these differently by infants and toddlers than by older prior events, they did so only if they had the children and adults (see P. J. Bauer, 2007; Howe, vocabulary to describe the event at the time of Courage, & Rooksby, 2009; Rovee-Collier & the experiencepost,. That is, children who were more Giles, 2010). The first possibility seems unlikely; verbally sophisticated at the time of initial test- as we’ve seen, research using the deferred-imi- ing tended to verbally recall some aspects of the tation procedure (P. J. Bauer, 2007; Meltzoff, event, but children were seemingly not able to 1995), discussed earlier in this chapter, has translate earlier preverbal experiences into lan- shown that infants from 6 to 16 months of agecopy, guage. According to Simcock and Hayne (2002), can encode and retain a simple experience for as “Children’s verbal reports were frozen in time, long as 1 year. reflecting their verbal skill at the time of encod- The second alternative, that informationnot is ing, rather than at the time of test” (p. 229; see encoded differently during the early and later also a review by Reese, 2014). years of life, is consistent with observations made Such an interpretation can account for our by and others thatDo the nature of rep- inability to retrieve memories from infancy, but resentation changes from -infancy to early child- 3- and 4-year-old children are clearly verbal hood and then again (although less drastically) and would presumably represent information somewhere from age 5 to age 7. The minds that in a symbolic form similar to the way adults resided in our heads when we were infants are do. The fact that 3- and 4-year-old children no longer there, replaced by minds that process can recount verbally events that happened symbols, especially verbal ones. We reconstruct many months or even years ago suggests that memoriesProof through adult schemes and representa- this interpretation cannot be the entire answer tions, which are not suitable for events encoded (Fivush & Hamond, 1990). in infancy and early childhood. For example, the A number of alternative explanations for memories tested in infancy all involve recall of infantile amnesia have been proposed. For exam- Draftaction patterns, whereas the recall assessed in ple, several authors have suggested that for childhood and adulthood involves verbal recall, ­autobiographical memories to be laid down and

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. 316 CHILDREN’S THINKING

later retrieved, there needs to be a sense of self increased at a later stage of development, mice (the auto in autobiographical) (Fivush, 2011; demonstrated an increase in forgetting. These Howe, 2014). Research has shown that the sense results are interesting, but they do not account of self develops gradually during the preschool for the large and somewhat exclusive loss of years (see discussion of the development of self- autobiographical memories, yet not other forms concept in Chapter 10) and that, although young of declarative memory, as we transition from children have memories, the experiences of early infancy to childhood. Thus, shifts in encoding, childhood occurred when the sense of self was representation of the self, and language, as we poorly developed, thus providing no anchor for describe next, provide additional explanation for such events. Unless events can be related to the infantile amnesia. self, they cannot be retrieved later. Other theorists have proposeddistribute changes in Another alternative is that infantile amnesia language and how language is used as explana- is merely a consequence of the child’s developing tions for the phenomenonor of infantile amnesia information-processing system (Leichtman & (Fivush & Hamond, 1990; K. Nelson, 1993, Ceci, 1993). Following the tenets of fuzzy-trace 1996). This was illustrated in Simcock and theory (Brainerd & Reyna, 2014; see discussion Hayne’s (2002) research, which showed that in Chapter 5), Michelle Leichtman and Stephen 2- and 3-year-old children were not able to Ceci (1993) proposed a developmental shift in translate a post,preverbal memory into language how events are represented, with young children 6 months later. But language is not fully devel- encoding events primarily in terms of verbatim oped by 3 years of age, and other language- (precise) memory traces and older children rely- related changes may further influence children’s ing more on gist (less precise) traces. Verbatim ability to remember events from their past. traces are more susceptible to forgetting than gist copy,Young children, in trying to understand and traces are. Thus, the heavy reliance on the highly predict their world, are attentive to routines forgettable verbatim traces makes ­memories and embed novel events in terms of these rou- from infancy and early childhood unavailable.not tines (for example, what happens at breakfast, Gist traces become increasingly available by the what happens on a trip to the grocery store). early school years, about the time when more Such memories, ensconced as they are in rou- memories can be retrieved. Do tines, are not very distinctive and, thus, are Recent neurocognitive- research also points not easily retrieved later on. Even when events to changes in hippocampal neurogenesis as are distinctive, however, young children do not important (Akers et al., 2014). Recall that rates necessarily know how to organize them in a of hippocampal neurogenesis are high early in memorable form. As we’ll discuss in more detail life and decline with age, as we described ear- later in this chapter, children “learn” to remem- lier. These high rates of neurogenesis and sub- ber through interactions with adults. Adults sequent pruningProof may result in our inability to provide the cues and structure for children to access hippocampus-dependent episodic memo- form —stories for embedding events ries later in life. In fact, when neurogenesis was and, later, for remembering them. Only after ­experimentally reduced in mice at an early devel- being guided by adults can children learn to Draftopmental stage, their hippocampus-dependent code memories and realize that language can be memories persisted. But when neurogenesis was used to share memories with others.

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. CHAPTER 8 MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 317

Katherine Nelson (1993) makes this point cognition. This transition is based on everyday especially clear: adult-child interaction and the developing lan- guage system, and it transforms the child into The claim here is that the initial functional sig- an individual who has a personal past that can nificance of is that of be shared with others. This does not preclude sharing memory with other people, a function that the possibility that changes in the self-system language makes possible. Memories become val- (Howe, 2014) or in the information-processing ued in their own right—not because they predict system (Leichtman & Ceci, 1993) also play the future and guide present action, but because an important role. Our guess is that they do. they are shareable with others and thus serve a The most recent research suggests that infantile social solidarity function. I suggest that this is a distribute universal human function, although one with vari- amnesia reflects important changes occurring able, culturally specific rules. In this respect, it is during early childhood—changes that permit analogous to human language itself, uniquely and autobiographical memoryor and that truly sepa- universally human but culturally—and individu- rate our species from all others. However, even ally—variable. (p. 12) later-developing skills, such as perspective tak- ing and abstract reasoning, may be important Research examining the earliest memories of for helping us organize life experiences along people from different lends some sup- a timeline post,(Habermas & Bluck, 2000). Some port for Nelson’s ideas. For instance, in a series research shows that it is not until age 12 that of studies, White North Americans reported children begin to link single events together earlier memories than Asians, in most cases (Habermas & de Silveira, 2008), although oth- by about half a year (Peterson, Wang, & Hou, ers have shown that even 8-year-olds can nomi- 2009; Q. Wang, 2006, 2014). In addition,copy, nate “chapters” or stages of life that describe American adults’ earliest memories were more their history, and the number and complexity likely to include emotions and less likely to of these periods increases with age (Reese et al., involve family activities than were thenot child - 2010). hood memories of Chinese adults (Q. Wang, 2001). These patterns are correlated with cul- tural differences in parent-childDo interaction. Infantile Amnesia and Hypnotic For example, compared- to Korean mothers, Age Regression American mothers talk about past events more with their 3-year-old children (Mullen & Yi, Are memories from infancy and early child- 1995) and also have earlier memories than hood really gone, or are they just not retriev- Korean adults (Mullen, 1994). (Gender also able by our conscious minds? What about seems to have an effect, with women tending to hypnosis? Don’t psychologists often use hyp- have memoriesProof from earlier in life than men; see nosis to help people remember forgotten or Fivush & Zaman, 2014.) repressed memories, and doesn’t that some- K. Nelson’s (1993) claim suggests that times include remembering events from one’s infantile amnesia might not be so significant in early childhood? But are memories recalled Draftits own right, as Freud believed, but, instead, using hypnosis reliable, and are these mem- reflects an important transition in human ories even real? In general, people under

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. 318 CHILDREN’S THINKING

hypnosis often do recall more details about an event than do people not under hypnosis, Section Review but many of these details turn out to be false • Infantile amnesia refers to the inability to (Erdelyi, 1994). recall information from infancy and early In hypnotic age regression, adults are hyp- childhood. Current theories about the rea- notized and “brought back” to an earlier age. son for infantile amnesia focus on the de- In doing so, will the adults now truly think velopment of self-concept, developmental differences in how information is encoded, like they did as children, and will they remem- and changes in children’s use of language to ber events from their childhood better than communicate their memories to others. nonhypnotized people? Michael Nash (1987) • The use of hypnotic age regression is not suc- reviewed more than 60 years of this literature, cessful in retrieving memoriesdistribute from infancy. and his answer is a definitive no. For example, in research during which adults are regressed Ask Yourself . . . or to the preschool years and then given Piagetian­ 6. How does the emergence of autobio- tasks such as conservation (see Chapter 5), graphical memory correspond to other they act more like adults who are asked to measures of self-awareness, such as mirror pretend to solve the problem like a 4-year-old self-recognition and metacognition? 7. What are some explanations for infantile would than like an actual 4-year-old child. In post, amnesia? What evidence is there to sup- one study, adults were regressed to 3 years of port these theories? age and asked to identify objects such as dolls, blankets, or teddy bears that were of particu- lar importance and comfort to them at that time (Nash et al., 1986). The parents of both copy, hypnotized and control participants were con- tacted to confirm the subjects’ recollections. IMPLICIT MEMORY The hypnotized people were substantiallynot less accurate in identifying favorite objects from The memory infants show in the preference-for- their early childhoods than were the controls. novelty task or in Rovee-Collier’s conditioning The reports of the hypnotizedDo participants experiments does not seem to require conscious matched those of their -parents just 21% of awareness. Typically, conscious awareness has the time, whereas the hit rate for the controls been a prerequisite for explicit, or declarative, was 70%. memory, with the unconscious memories of infants People who experience age regression might typically being classified as a form of implicit have the feeling of recalling a real memory. memory. (See, however, Howe, 2000, and Rovee- However, how confident one is in the veracity Collier & Giles, 2010, for arguments against this of a memory,Proof unfortunately, does not always distinction.) Implicit memory is “memory without predict the truth of the memory. There is no awareness” (see Schacter, 1992), and it is not lim- evidence that hypnotic age regression can suc- ited to preverbal infants and toddlers but occurs ceed in retrieving repressed, or simply forgotten, also in older children and adults. Draftmemories from childhood, despite many people’s The distinction between implicit and explicit claims to the contrary. memory has more than heuristic value, for the

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. CHAPTER 8 MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 319

two types of memories seem to be governed by In this second phase, children were tasked with different brain systems, as revealed by research responding whether they knew the person pic- on people with brain damage (Schacter, 1992; tured. Regardless of age, 5-, 8-, and 11-year-olds Schacter, Norman, & Koutstaal, 2000). For showed implicit effects; they were faster instance, as we mentioned previously, the hip- to respond that they recognized previously pre- pocampus is involved in transferring new explicit sented faces than those of faces they knew that information from the short-term store (the loca- had not been presented earlier in the experiment. tion of immediate awareness) to the long-term To rule out the influence of explicit, declara- store. People with damage to the hippocampus tive memory, Russo and colleagues excluded can acquire a new skill as a result of repeated from their analysis those responses to pictures practice, but they will have no awareness of ever that participants could explicitlydistribute recall having learning such skills. For example, seen during the first phase of the study, instead (1964) reported the case of H.M., a patient with focusing only on those responsesor to pictures that hippocampal brain damage. H.M. was given a children did not remember seeing earlier (even mirror-drawing task over several days, in which though they indeed had). These findings pro- he had to trace figures while watching his hand vide strong evidence for similar priming effects in a mirror. H.M.’s performance was quite poor (implicit memory) across age groups when the initially but improved after several days of prac- influence ofpost, explicit memory is controlled for. tice, despite the fact that he had no recollection A similar finding was reproduced in 4-, 5-, and of ever performing the task before. The enhance- 10-year-olds (Hayes & Hennessy, 1996). In ment of performance as a result of practice is a other words, although children exhibited sub- reflection of intact implicit (procedural) mem- stantial age differences in explicit memory, no ory, whereas H.M.’s failure to recall previouslycopy, developmental differences were found in implicit performing the task is a reflection of a lack of memory. explicit memory. Another interesting study of implicit memory There has been less developmentalnot research again involved showing 9- and 10-year-olds pic- on implicit memory compared to explicit mem- tures of preschool children, including some who ory, but what research has been done presents a had been their classmates 4 and 5 years ear- consistent picture. Although Dosubstantial age dif- lier (Newcombe & Fox, 1994). Children were ferences are found on tests- of declarative memo- asked to determine whether each picture they rization, few age differences are found when were shown was that of a former classmate (an implicit memory is tested (see Finn et al., 2016; explicit recognition memory task); changes in Lloyd & Miller, 2014). For instance, Riccardo the electrical conductance of their skin were Russo and colleagues (1995) adapted a percep- also recorded. Greater changes in skin conduc- tual priming task to assess the implicit memory tance for former classmates’ pictures relative to of children.Proof Children first viewed pictures of pictures of unfamiliar children was used as a classmates and were asked to make decisions reflection of implicit recognition memory (that about the portrayed expressions and gender. is, requiring no conscious awareness). Not sur- After a delay, they were shown another series prisingly, children’s performance was relatively Draftof pictures, half of which had been presented poor on both the explicit and implicit tasks in the earlier phase and half of which had not. (although greater than expected by chance,

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. 320 CHILDREN’S THINKING

indicating some memory of both the explicit and hypothesis (Schneider, 2015). Indeed, Nora implicit types). However, no difference in skin Newcombe and colleagues (2000) suggest that conductance was found between children who we may retain much in the way of implicit mem- performed well on the explicit task and those ories from infancy that can affect our behavior who performed poorly. This suggests that even much later in life. the children whose performance on the explicit It is worth noting, as Lloyd and Miller (2014) memory task was no greater than chance still have, that implicit memory is a broad umbrella recognized, implicitly, as many of their former term, used by memory researchers to describe classmates as those children who had performed a variety of aspects of nondeclarative memory, better. This pattern of data indicates that some from operant and , to children remembered (implicitly) more than they perceptual and conceptual priming,distribute and proce- knew (explicitly). A similar pattern of findings dural memory, to name a few, each of which was demonstrated in the event-related potentials is linked to different brainor areas. As neurosci- (ERPs, a measure of brain response as the direct ence advances, it is likely that we will develop result of exposure to a stimulus) of 6-month- more detailed distinctions between the different old infants when viewing previously experienced forms of implicit memory and their development stimuli. The ERPs recorded when they viewed (Lloyd & Miller, 2014). repeated faces showed greater negativity (in post, other words, a stronger response) than when they viewed new faces, suggesting that infants were sensitive to the previously experienced Section Review stimuli (Webb & Nelson, 2001). • In contrast to explicit memory, few age dif- These findings suggest that implicit memory copy,ferences are observed for implicit memory, is an early developing ability. Performance on when there is no conscious intention to re- implicit memory tasks is associated with the member something. basal ganglia, neocortex (priming), notstriatum Ask Yourself . . . (skill learning), and (conditioning; see 8. What do we know about the development Toth, 2000, for a review), which develop earlier of implicit memory? How does it differ than areas of the brain associatedDo with declara- from the development of explicit memory? tive memory (see Lukowski- & Bauer, 2014). Many believe that nondeclarative, implicit mem- ory is an evolutionarily older memory system in contrast with explicit, declarative memory (see Bjorklund & Sellers, 2014). DEVELOPMENT OF Some theorists have speculated that implicit EVENT MEMORY memory isProof under the control of automatic rather than effortful processes (Jacoby, 1991) and, fol- Much of what we remember is for events, things lowing the theorizing of Lynn Hasher and Rose that happen to us during the course of everyday Zacks (1979), the evidence suggests that these life. Unlike implicit memory, event memory is Draftprocesses show little development across child- explicit. We are aware that we are remembering. hood, supporting the developmental invariance For most aspects of event memory, however, we

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. CHAPTER 8 MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 321

did not specifically try to remember the event children don’t always select the “right” things to when we experienced it. In other words, for most pay attention to. At a baseball game, they might event memories, the encoding of the event was spend more time watching the hot dog ven- unintentional. Because we did not intend to learn dors, the batboys, and the second-base umpire. new information, our unintentional memory is What they remember of the game will thus be not influenced by the use of deliberate encoding very different than what an older child or adult strategies, which accounts for much of the age remembers. differences observed on intentional memory tasks. It is also worth noting that event memory (See discussion of strategic memory in Chap­ ter 7.) is constructive in nature. Event memory does Rather, memory representations can be laid down not involve the verbatim recall of a list of facts involuntarily as part of ongoing activity, and sev- or the memorization of lines, likedistribute an actor in a eral researchers have speculated that, in some play (although children, and adults, do retain cases, such naturalistic learning could actually some specific, or verbatim,or aspects of events; produce higher levels of memory performance see Brainerd & Reyna, 2014). Rather, we recall than would more deliberate memorization the gist of the message, and in the process, we attempts, especially for young children (Istomina, transform what was actually said or done. That 1975; Piaget & Inhelder, 1973). is, we interpret our experiences as a function of The issue at hand is when and how children what we alreadypost, know about the world, and our remember the experiences of their everyday lives. memory for events is colored by previous knowl- How are these memories organized? How long edge (see our discussion of top-down processing do they last? And how is it that children acquire in Chapter 7 and Bartlett, 1932). Memory, in them? general, is not like a tape recorder. True, we A young child must master many aspects ofcopy, sometimes do retain verbatim information and memory if he or she is to remember important use that information to construct stories. But the events. First, an event must be attended to and tales we tell about our lives are best thought of as perceived. Then, the child must make somenot sense constructions, based on our actual experiences, of that event so that it can be represented in our background knowledge of the things we are his or her mind and recalled later on. If a child trying to remember, our information-processing doesn’t attend to the importantDo aspects of an abilities, and the social context in which the event or cannot make sense- of what he or she remembering is being done. experienced, there is really nothing to remem- ber. One important thing to realize is that young children pay attention to different aspects of Script-Based Memory events than adults do, and they do not necessar- ily know which aspects of an event are important What is it that young children remember? One and whichProof are trivial. For example, as adults, thing they tend to remember well is recurring we know that the purpose of a baseball game events—what typically happens on a day-to-day is to watch the players on the field play ball. basis (Hudson & Mayhew, 2009). Katherine We automatically pay less attention to the field Nelson and her colleagues have demonstrated Draftmaintenance staff, the players on the bench, and that preschool children tend to organize events most of the other spectators. However, young in terms of scripts, which are a form of schematic

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. 322 CHILDREN’S THINKING

organization with real-world events organized Hamond (1990) asked 2.5-year-old children in terms of their causal and temporal character- specific questions about recent special events, istics (K. Nelson, 1993, 1996; see also Fivush, such as a trip to the beach, a camping trip, or 2008). For example, a fast-food restaurant script a ride on an airplane. Rather than recalling might involve driving to the restaurant, entering the novel aspects of these special events, the the restaurant, standing in line, ordering, paying children were more apt to focus on what adults the cashier, taking the food to the table, eating, would consider routine information. Take, for and then throwing away the trash before leaving. instance, the following conversation reported Children learn what usually happens in a situa- by Fivush and Hamond (1990) between an tion, such as what happens during snack time at adult and a child about a camping trip. The school, a birthday party, or breakfast, and they child first recalled sleeping outside,distribute which is remember novel information in the context of unusual, but then remembered very routine these familiar events. things: or Substantial research demonstrates that even very young children organize information tem- Interviewer: You slept outside in a tent? Wow, porally in a scriptlike fashion (see P. J. Bauer, that sounds like a lot of fun. 2007; Fivush, Kuebli, & Clubb, 1992) and that such schematic organization for events doesn’t Child: post,And then we waked up and eat change appreciably into adulthood (for reviews dinner. First we eat dinner, then of this literature, see Fivush & Hudson, 1990; go to bed, and then wake up and eat breakfast. K. Nelson, 1996). Perhaps even more impres- sive is evidence that even preverbal infants use Interviewer: What else did you do when you temporal order to remember events. For exam-copy, went camping? What did you do ple, Patricia Bauer and Jean Mandler (1989, when you got up, after breakfast? 1992) tested infants ranging in age from 11.5 to Child: Umm, in the night, and went to 20 months on imitation tasks. The toddlers were not sleep. (p. 231) shown a sequence of events (for example, putting a ball in a cup, inverting a smaller cup on top of the larger one, and shaking theDo cups) and, later, It seems strange that a child would talk given the opportunity to interact- with the materi- about such routine tasks as waking up, eating, als again. Bauer and Mandler reported that the and going to bed when so many new and excit- children reenacted the sequence of events in the ing things must have happened on the camp- same temporal order they had been shown. This ing trip. But the younger the child, the more finding argues for the existence of a script-style he or she might need to embed novel events memory organization long before children are into familiar routines. According to Fivush and able to talk.Proof Hamond, everything is new to 2-year-olds, and Young children’s tendencies to organize they are in the process of learning about their information following familiar scripts seem to surroundings. result in their tendency not to remember much Why should young children’s memory be so Draftin the way of specific (that is, nonscript) infor- tied to recurring events? One way to answer this mation. For example, Robyn Fivush and Nina question is to ask what the function of memory

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. CHAPTER 8 MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 323

is for young children. (1996, Disney World. All children recalled a great deal 2005) has taken such a functional view, arguing of information about their trip, even after 18 that memory has an adaptive value of permit- months. The older children recalled more details ting children to predict the likelihood of events and required fewer prompts (cues) to generate in the future. Basically, by remembering the recall than did the younger children, and chil- likelihood of an event’s occurrence in the past, dren who talked more about the trip with their one can predict its likelihood of occurring in parents recalled more information about the trip. the future. From this perspective, some events Nevertheless, recall for this single, special event (recurring ones) are more likely to be remem- was quite good, even though it did not fall nicely bered than are others (single events). According into a familiar routine. to K. Nelson (1996), distribute

Memory for a single, one-time occurrence of some Role of Parents in “Teaching”or event, if the event were not traumatic or life-threat- Children to Remember ening, would not be especially useful, given its low probability. Thus, a memory system might be opti- Children begin to talk about past events not long mally designed to retain information about frequent and recurrent events—and to discard information after they acquirepost, their first words, and their recol- about unrepeated events—and to integrate new lection skills develop rapidly from 2 to 4 years of information about variations in recurrent events age (K. Nelson, 2014). Parents play an important into a general knowledge system. (p. 174) role in this development, as Hamond and Fivush’s research demonstrated. In effect, parents “teach” K. Nelson’s ideas are related to theory-theory children how to remember (Fivush, 2014). As it concepts discussed in Chapters 4 and 5, spe-copy,turns out, parents, especially mothers (but also cifically the Goldilocks effect, Bayesian statisti- sometimes fathers), and their children spend a good cal learning, and causal representation. Nelson deal of time reminiscing about the past, and indi- makes the point that memory for routinenot events vidual differences in how this is done are related allows infants to anticipate events and to take a to how well children remember past events. This part in (and, possibly, control of) these events. suggests that parents can play an important role in There is no such payoff for a novelDo event; thus, it children’s early remembering, a point that has been makes sense to forget it. - made by several theorists (Fivush, 2014; Haden Children do, however, eventually remember & Ornstein, 2009) and that is consistent with the specific events, not just some generalized event theorizing of Lev Vygotsky (1978) and the socio- memory. In fact, although 2- and 3-year-old cultural perspective discussed in Chapter 3 (see children may rely heavily on scripts, they have the discussion of shared remembering; Gauvain, been shown to remember specific information 2001). For example, Judith Hudson (1990) argued for extendedProof periods (Hamond & Fivush, 1991; that children learn how to remember by interact- Howe, 2000). Hamond and Fivush (1991) pre- ing with their parents, that “remembering can be sented research that demonstrates how long viewed as an activity that is at first jointly car- memories for specific events can last for young ried out by parent and child and then later per- Draftchildren. They interviewed 3- and 4-year-old formed by the child alone” (p. 172). In most children 6 or 18 months after they had gone to , ­Hudson proposed, parents begin talking

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. 324 CHILDREN’S THINKING

with young children about things that happened Mother: Tanya, what did we see at the zoo? in the past. They ask questions such as “Where Tanya: Elphunts. did we go this morning?,” “What did we see at the zoo?,” “Who went with us?,” and “What else did Mother: That’s right! We saw elephants. we see?” From these interchanges, children learn What else? that the important facts to remember about events Tanya: [shrugs and looks at her mother] are the whos, whats, whens, and wheres of their experiences. Through these conversations with Mother: Panda bear? Did we see a panda their parents, they are learning to notice the impor- bear? tant details of their experiences and to store their Tanya: [smiles and nods her head] memories in an organized way so that they can be distribute easily retrieved when needed. Parents also often Mother: Can you say panda bear? discuss future events with children (for example, Tanya: Panda bear. or an upcoming trip to Disney World), and when they do, particularly when they include photographs of Mother: Good! Elephants and panda bears. what they may see, children later remember more What else? about the event (Salmon et al., 2008). Tanya: Elphunts. In studying these interchanges between par- post, ents and preschoolers, Hudson found that par- Mother: That’s right, elephants. And also a ents do more than just ask the right questions. gorilla. They also give the right answers when the child Tanya: Go-rilla! can’t remember, showing children how the con- versation should go. Young children generally copy, show low levels of when remembering Hilary Ratner (1984) illustrated the impor- an event, but they can remember much more tance of these parent-child conversations. She when specific cues are presented. In fact,not Fivush observed 2- and 3-year-old children interact- and Hamond (1990) stated that “young children ing with their parents at home and recorded recall as much information as older children do, the number of times the mother asked the child but they need more memory Doquestions in order about past events. She then tested the children’s to do so” (p. 244). By asking- repeated questions memory abilities. The children who showed bet- to children, adults are structuring the conversa- ter memory abilities at that time, and also a tion, showing children how “remembering” is year later, were those whose mothers had asked done. Moreover, by providing the missing infor- them many questions about past events. Other mation, children also learn that their parents will research has shown that mothers who provide help them out when they can’t seem to retrieve their preschool children with more evaluations the informationProof called for. of their memory performance, and who use A good example of this was a conversation more elaborative language when talking about I (DB) overheard while riding on the Metro in memory with their children, have children who Washington, D.C. A young mother and her remember past events better than do children Draft19-month-old daughter, Tanya, were returning with less elaborative mothers (Fivush, Haden, & home after a trip to the zoo. Reese, 2006; McDonnell et al., 2016). That is,

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. CHAPTER 8 MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 325

after making a statement about some previous in the backpack) that were jointly handled and event (for example, “Then we ate the cake”), talked about by the mother and child were bet- elaborative mothers are more likely to provide ter remembered than were features that were comments that confirm or negate a child’s state- handled and talked about only by the mother ment (such as “That’s right,” “Yes,” or “No”) or that were jointly handled by the mother and than less elaborative mothers are. Other research child but not discussed. This result clearly points has even shown that preschool children whose out the important role of joint activity guided mothers used more elaborative language in talk- by the mother, including the use of language, in ing with them about past events had earlier fostering young children’s event memory. memories as adolescents than did children with The results of recent research point to the less elaborative mothers (Jack et al., 2009). interactive role of parents and childrendistribute in the In other research, mothers and their young process of “learning” how to remember. Thus, children engaged in three novel events, one when remembering becomes a culturalor phenomenon, children were 30 months, a second at 36 months, consistent with the ideas of Vygotsky and others and a third at 42 months (Haden et al., 2001; who propose a sociocultural perspective of devel- Hedrick et al., 2009). For example, in one study opment (see Chapter 3; Fivush, 2014). Parents (Haden et al., 2001), children were tested for teach children how to construct narratives (that their memory of each event at 1 day and at is, create stories)post, in which to embed the impor- 3 weeks following each episode. The events were tant things that happen to them. This in turn carried out in children’s homes and involved allows children to share their experiences with the investigator setting up props and asking the others. This is a practice that may vary some- mother and child to carry out an elaborate make- what, however, between cultures (Schröder believe activity. For example, for the campingcopy, et al., 2013). For example, Mary Mullen and event, the mother and child first loaded supplies Soonhyung Yi (1995) examined how frequently in a backpack, hiked to a fishing pond, caught a Korean and American mothers talked to their fish with a fishing rod and net, and thennot moved 3-year-old children about past events and to their campsite, where they found sleeping reported that the American mothers talked about bags, pots, pans, and utensils, which they used the past with their young children nearly 3 times to cook and eat their food. TheDo frequency with as often as the Korean mothers did. This is con- which mothers and children- jointly carried out sistent with reports that American children talk these activities, and the degree to which lan- about past events more than Korean children guage was involved during the execution of the do (Han, Leichtman, & Wang, 1998) and that task, were observed and related to children’s American adults report earlier childhood memo- subsequent memory performance. First, and not ries than Korean adults (Mullen, 1994). This surprisingly, children’s overall memory perfor- suggests that early language experience contrib- mance increasedProof with age and was greater for utes to the onset of autobiographical memory, the 1-day delay than the 3-week delay. Most consistent with the argument made by Katherine pertinent for our discussion here was the rela- Nelson (1996, 2005). tion between mother-child activities during the It is also worth noting that there are dif- Draftevent and children’s later recollections. Fea- ferences in the event memories reported by tures of the events (for example, putting food girls and boys. When asked to remember

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. 326 CHILDREN’S THINKING

information about earlier experienced events, It’s hard to minimize the significance of auto- girls tend to remember more information than biographical memory and to think that there boys do (Reese, Haden, & Fivush, 1996), may have been a time in our lives when it didn’t but not always (see K. D. Lewis, 1999). For exist. Our recollections about what we’ve expe- example, in a study by Elaine Reese, Catherine rienced in the past define for us who we are and Haden, and Robin Fivush (1996), children ages how we interact with others. But autobiographi- 40 to 70 months participated in several ses- cal memory develops over the course of infancy sions during which their mothers, fathers, or and early childhood. In fact, Alison Gopnik an experimenter asked them to recollect about (2009) proposed that although 2- and 3-year-old salient events that had occurred in the recent children can remember specific events, a reflec- past. Regardless of who interviewed the chil- tion of episodic memory, they lackdistribute true autobio- dren (that is, their mothers, fathers, or the graphical memory. According to Gopnik, “They experimenter), girls remembered more details do not experience their livesor as a single timeline about past events than boys did. These gen- stretching back into the past and forward into der differences were related to ways in which the future. They don’t send themselves backward parents conversed with their sons and daugh- and forward along this timeline as adults do. . . . ters about the past. Both mothers and fathers Instead, the memories, images, and thoughts pop tended to be more elaborative with daughters in and out ofpost, consciousness as they are cued by than sons when engaged in parent-child remi- present events, or by other memories, images, niscing, and girls generally received more eval- and thoughts” (pp. 153–154). uations of their memory responses than boys. These findings suggest that the roots of females’ greater event memory lie early in development copy,Section Review and might result, in part, because of the way • Young children’s event memory is based on parents talk to boys and girls during attempts scripts, a form of schematic organization at remembering, with daughters beingnot encour - with real-world events organized by their aged to embellish their memories more than causal and temporal characteristics. sons are. Other research indicates that moth- • Children’s early memories are for general routines and not for specific autobiographi- ers talk to their male and female preschool Do cal experiences. children about different -topics. For example, • Parents “teach” children how to remember Dorothy Flannagan, Lynne Baker-Ward,­ and by interacting with them and providing the Loranel Graham (1995) reported that in their structure for putting their experiences into conversations about school, mothers talked to narratives. their sons more about learning and instruction, Ask Yourself . . . whereas they tended to talk to their daugh- ters more Proofabout social interactions. Thus, there 9. What do we mean when we say that event are differences both in how parents talk to memory is constructive in nature? boys and girls about remembering events and 10. What evidence is there for the social- cultural perspective of event memory? in what they are asked to remember, both of 11. How do children remember and organize Draftwhich seemingly affect what and how well chil- events in memory? dren remember.

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. CHAPTER 8 MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 327

CHILDREN AS EYEWITNESSES Lindberg, Keiffer, & Thomas, 2000) has sug- gested three major categories of factors that we One topic in event memory that has attracted sub- should consider in evaluating studies of children’s stantial research attention concerns the reliability and suggestibility (see Figure of children as eyewitnesses. How reliable is chil- 8.6). The first category in Lindberg’s scheme is dren’s testimony? How much do they remember, memory processes, and these concern the differ- and for how long? How suggestible are children? ent memory operations of encoding, , and Can a persuasive interviewer make children report retrieval. Encoding refers to children’s represen- things that didn’t really happen, and can faulty tation of an event and how children respond to interviewing techniques actually result in children information they receive before distributethe event. For believing they were victims of (or witnesses to) a example, how are children influenced by being told that someone they are about to meet is a crime when they were not? These are questions “bad boy” or prone to breaking things? Storage not only for the justice system but also for psy- or refers to information provided to participants chologists because they deal with the nature of after witnessing an event. This may be in the children’s developing memory systems and the form of suggestive questions (for example, “He construction of a particular kind of event memory. spilled chocolate milk over the book, didn’t he?”) In the following sections, we review research or posteventpost, information, which includes any and theory into children as eyewitnesses and experiences that intervene between witnessing the degree to which their testimonies and their an event and recollecting it. Retrieval refers to memories are subject to change. In the first sec- manipulations at the time of testing. For instance, tion, we review age differences in children’s eye- how is memory tested? With open-ended, free- witness reports when no one is trying to changecopy, recall questions (for example, “Tell me every- their minds. That is, what do children remem- thing you can remember about what happened at ber, and what factors influence their memory, the park last week”), cued-recall questions (such when they are asked to report what notthey wit - as “Tell me what color was the boy’s T-shirt”), or nessed or experienced? In the second section, we recognition (for instance, “Look at these pictures. examine the large literature on age differences in Was one of these the boy who took the bike?”)? suggestibility. How susceptibleDo are children to How are these questions posed? suggestion, and to what extent- will they change Lindberg refers to the second category of their answers or their memory representations his taxonomy as focus of the study, by which as a result of suggestive questioning? he means the type of information that is being First, however, let us provide a general frame- assessed. For example, is the interviewer con- work for making sense of this research literature. cerned with psychologically and legally central Children’s eyewitness testimony and suggestibil- (or focal) information (who did what to whom, ity, like eventProof memory in general, are influenced which is critical in determining innocence or guilt by a host of interacting factors. Which factors are in court), or is peripheral (incidental) informa- most important? Can we specify how the various tion (for example, “What color pants was the factors will interact to predict performance? And wearing?” or “How tall was the girl?”) also Draftcan we be confident enough in our conclusions to important? Similarly, is the memory for gist infor- inform the legal system? Marc Lindberg (1991; mation or for details, or verbatim information (as

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. 328 CHILDREN’S THINKING

FIGURE 8.6 Three major interacting classes of variables in interpreting children’s eyewitness memory and suggestibility.

Memory Processes Focus of Study

Encoding • Organization • Perceptual set • Other information in consciousness • Expectancies Storage • Intentions • Leading questions distribute • Interference from external or internal stimuli Details Incidental stimuli or Retrieval • Recognition • Recall Focal stimuli • Accuracy of communication Incidental stimuli

post,Connections

Gist Participant Characteristics

• Attentional focus • Organizational strategies • Knowledge base • Arousal level • Inferential ability • Egocentrismcopy, • Desire to conform • Reality and source- • Interpretations of validity monitoring ability Source: Lindberg, M. A. (1991). An interactive approach to assessing the suggestibility and testimony of eyewitnesses. In J. Doris (Ed.), The suggestibility of children’s recollections: Implications for noteyewitness testimony (pp. 47–55). Copyright © 1991 by the American Psychological Association. Reprinted with permission. Do in fuzzy-trace theory’s description- of memory from this taxonomy, but it is worthwhile to keep traces; see Brainerd & Reyna, 2014)? these categories in mind if for no other reason And, finally, one must consider participant than to remember that understanding children’s factors. Here, developmental level and the asso- eyewitness memory is not child’s play. ciated social, emotional, and cognitive skills of children are important. Also of potential impor- tance are Proofpersonality characteristics of the par- Age Differences in Children’s ticipants, their level of at the time of the Eyewitness Memories event (or at the time of retrieval), their past experiences with a situation, and their more Although there is much variability in methods from Draftgeneral knowledge of the things they witness. study to study, most investigations of ­children’s No single study will include all relevant factors eyewitness memory begin by showing children a

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. CHAPTER 8 MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 329

video of some event, having them observe some Larus, 1992; Poole & White, 1995). How much activity in their school, or involving them person- is remembered differs from study to study, but ally in an activity. Usually, children are not told preschool children typically recall only a small that they will be asked to remember what they proportion of information from an event in view. Later, often minutes after the event but in answer to free-recall questions. Although young some cases days or weeks later, children are asked children remember very little information, what what they remembered (for example, “Tell me they do recall is highly accurate and central to what happened in the video you saw” or “Tell me the event—if there are no suggestions or coaching what happened in your classroom yesterday morn- (G. S. Goodman, Aman, & Hirschman, 1987; ing”). This is essentially a request for free recall. Poole & White, 1995). For example, in a video Typically, children will then be asked some more involving a boy and a girl in a park,distribute with the boy specific recall questions (for example, “What was stealing a bike, young children typically recall the girl in the video wearing?” or “What did the the bike theft but are muchor less apt than older man who came into your class yesterday morn- children or adults to mention in their free recall ing do?”), which constitute cued recall. Often, descriptions of the participants, characteristics of children will be asked some recognition memory the bicycle, or things about the setting (Cassel & questions (for example, “Was the girl wearing a Bjorklund, 1995; Cassel, Roebers, & Bjorklund, white T-shirt?” or “Did the man play with the 1996). Thus,post, young children’s free recall is typi- teddy bear?”). In some studies, the same or similar cally low, accurate, and about central aspects of questions may be repeated, and in others, ques- an event. tions are often intentionally suggestive, sometimes When children are provided general cues (for directing children to a “correct” answer (such as example, “Tell me what the girl looked like”), “Did the man play nicely with the teddy bear?”)copy, they recall more information, as you would and sometimes leading to an “incorrect” answer expect. However, in addition to remembering (such as “Did the man rip the book?”). There more correct facts, they also tend to remem- are, of course, many variations, dependingnot on the ber some incorrect “facts” as well, reducing the purpose of the study, but in most cases, children’s overall accuracy of their recall (Bjorklund et al., memories for specific events are probed, often 1998; Lindberg et al, 2000). with the purpose of seeing howDo likely children are When children do falsely remember informa- to change their answers or -to be swayed by leading tion to cues, does this actually change their mem- questions posed by an interviewer (see Brainerd & ory representations? Will these children, when Reyna, 2014; Bruck, Ceci, & Principe, 2006). interviewed later, remember this misinformation again? The answer seems to depend on several How Much Do Children Remember, factors, including the amount of time between the initial and later interviews. With delays of and HowProof Accurate Are They? only several weeks or less, children seem not First, how much do children of different ages to recall their earlier false memories (Cassel & remember shortly after witnessing an event? Bjorklund, 1995). But when delays are longer When examining immediate (that is, within the (Poole, 1995) or when children are merely asked Draftsame experimental session) free recall, substantial to recognize rather than to recall information age differences are found (Ornstein, Gordon, & (Brainerd & Reyna, 1996), these false memories

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. 330 CHILDREN’S THINKING

not only tend to persist, they might even be First, with delays of about 1 month or less, more resistant to forgetting than true memories children of all ages and adults remember about ­(Brainerd & Mojardin, 1999). the same proportion of accurate and inaccurate These counterintuitive findings have been inter- information as they did originally (Baker-Ward preted in terms of fuzzy-trace theory (Brainerd­ & et al., 1993; Cassel & Bjorklund, 1995). Age dif- Reyna, 2014; see also Chapter 5). According to ferences in recall accuracy are found with longer fuzzy-trace theory, correct recognition is based on delays, however. For example, Rhona Flin and literal, or verbatim, memory traces, which are more her colleagues (1992) reported that both 6-year- susceptible to forgetting than are less-exact fuzzy, olds and adults recalled as much correct informa- or gist, traces. In contrast, false recognition must tion after a 5-month delay as they had originally be based on gist traces because there are no verba- but that recall by the 6-year-oldsdistribute was less accu- tim traces for false memories. Gist traces are more rate than recall by the adults. Thus, the ratio of resistant to forgetting than verbatim traces are, so incorrect-to-correct recall becameor higher for the the gist-based false memories become more likely children than for the adults during the 5-month to be remembered over long delays than the more period. The conclusion from this and similar easily forgettable, verbatim-based true memories. studies is that age differences in accuracy are found, but only when memory is assessed after extended delays.post, Fuzzy-trace theory (Brainerd­ & How Long Do Memories Last? Reyna, 2014; see also Chapter 5) explains these Although most studies have not assessed the long- findings by the greater rate of decay of verbatim term recollections of children, several have inves- (exact) traces relative to gist traces. Verbatim tigated children’s memories of specific events for traces, favored by younger children, deterio- periods ranging from several weeks to 2 years copy,rate more rapidly than the gist, or fuzzy, traces (Flin et al., 1992; Salmon & Pipe, 1997). This favored by older children, resulting in greater loss is important for the legal system, given that chil- of information over delays and corresponding dren are sometimes asked in forensic interviewsnot to increases in erroneous recall. These are the same remember events they witnessed weeks, months, arguments that have been used to explain the or even years earlier (Paz-Alonso et al., 2009; phenomenon of infantile amnesia (see earlier dis- Pipe & Salmon, 2009). AlthoughDo the results of cussion). The verbatim traces favored by infants these studies are not always- consistent, a picture and young children are especially susceptible to emerges of greater age differences in the accuracy deterioration, making it highly unlikely that these of recall with increasing delays. Accuracy, as used memories would be available years after their here, does not refer to how much was remem- original encoding (Leichtman & Ceci, 1993). bered but, rather, to the ratio of incorrect-to- correct information remembered. Children who Factors Influencing Children’s recall veryProof little, for example, but correctly recall what they do remember, have perfect accuracy. In Eyewitness Memory contrast, a child who recalls a substantial amount A host of factors other than age and length of of both correct (accurate) and incorrect (inaccu- delay have been found to influence the amount Draftrate) information might demonstrate more recall and accuracy of children’s eyewitness memories. but less accuracy. For instance, children with high IQs show higher

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. CHAPTER 8 MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 331

levels of eyewitness recall than do their peers invasive medical procedures are related to their with lower IQs (Roebers & Schneider, 2001); knowledge of the procedures; children who know children given incentives to be accurate in their more about the procedure remember more accu- recall are, indeed, more accurate than children rate information (Clubb et al., 1993; Ornstein et not given incentives (Roebers, Moga, & Schnei- al., 2006) and recall less inaccurate information der, 2001); intermediate levels of stress (when (G. S. Goodman et al., 1994). Yet although know- experiencing the event) seem to facilitate recall ing a lot about an event (that is, how actions in of an event relative to overly high or low levels an event are supposed to go) is usually associated of stress (Bahrick­ et al., 1998); children hav- with increased memory accuracy, knowledge can ing emotionally supportive­ mothers who discuss be a double-edged sword. For example, in a study upcoming medical procedures with them recall of 4- and 6-year-old children’s recalldistribute of a mock less inaccurate information about the procedure physical examination, Peter Ornstein and his than ­children having less sympathetic or talkative colleagues (1998) includedor some typical, script- mothers (G. S. Goodman et al., 1994; 1997); and consistent features in the exam (for example, the children whose parents score as more avoidant doctor listened to the children’s hearts with a are less accurate after experiencing higher dis- stethoscope and looked into their ears), but they tress levels during a medical procedure, whereas also included some atypical, unexpected features children’s parents who are less avoidant are (for example,post, the doctor measured children’s head more accurate after experiencing higher distress circumference and used alcohol to wipe their (Chae et al., 2014). Two sets of factors that have belly buttons). In addition, some expected fea- substantial influences on children’s eyewitness tures were omitted from the exams (for example, reports are children’s background knowledge for measuring blood pressure and looking in chil- the event (Ornstein et al., 2006) and the charac-copy,dren’s mouths). Children were interviewed about teristics of the interview (Ceci, Bruck, & Battin, the exam both immediately and after a 12-week 2000), each of which we discuss in greater detail delay. They were first asked open-ended questions next. not (for example, “Tell me what happened during your checkup” and “Tell me what the doctor did Role of knowledge. We saw in Chapter 7 that to check you”), followed by increasingly specific knowledge has a potent role inDo children’s working questions (for instance, “Did the doctor check any memory and strategic memory,- so we should not parts of your face?” and “Did the doctor check be surprised that it also plays an important role your eyes?”). In addition to questions about what in eyewitness memory (Elischberger, 2005; Orn- really did happen during the exam, children were stein & Greenhoot, 2000). Indeed, some research- also asked specific questions about things that did ers (see Howe, Wimmer, Gagnon, & Plumpton, not happen. For example, children for whom the 2009) have proposed an alternative approach to doctor did not check their ears would be asked, fuzzy-traceProof theory in explaining children’s false “Did the doctor look into your ears?” Some of memories, the associative activation theory, argu- the questions referred to events that are likely to ing that age differences in knowledge base and occur in an exam, such as looking into children’s automatic processing are the most important ears, whereas others were for events that were Draftfactor underlying false-memory production. For unlikely to occur (for example, “Did the doctor example, children’s recollections of stressful and give you some stitches?”).

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. 332 CHILDREN’S THINKING

How did the children do? The results for the happens in such an exam. As a result, features that 12-week assessment of this study are shown in are typically found in medical exams, based on Figures 8.7 and 8.8. The first figure reports the children’s past experiences, were more likely to be percentage of correct responses for the 4- and remembered than atypical features. But there was 6-year-old children for the present-typical and a negative side to knowledge, and this is reflected present-atypical features (that is, aspects of the in the results displayed in Figure 8.8. Correct deni- exam that children actually experienced) for the als (the dark portion of the bars in Figure 8.8) open-ended questions and for the specific ques- refer to children correctly stating that an event did tions. As can be seen, children of both ages recalled not happen (“Did the doctor give you stitches?” more typical features correctly than atypical fea- “No.”). As can be seen, children were more likely tures, for both types of questions. This reflects to correctly reject these nonevents distributefor the atypical the positive effects of knowledge base. This was features. False alarms (the light portion of the bars not the first medical exam for any of the children, or and they presumably had a script for what usually FIGURE 8.8 Percentage of absent-typical and absent-atypical features to which children FIGURE 8.7 Percentage of present-typical and responded with correct denials and false present-atypical features recalled correctly in alarms for 4-post, and 6-year-old children, 12-week response to open-ended and specific questions assessment. for 4- and 6-year-old children, 12-week assessment. False alarms Correct denials 100 100 Specific recall Open-ended recall copy, 80 80

60 not 60

40 40

Do of Responses rcentage Pe 20 rcentage of Features Recalled of Features rcentage 20 - Pe

0 0 Typical Atypical Typical Atypical Typical Atypical Typical Atypical 4-Year-Olds 6-Year-Olds 4-Year-Olds 6-Year-Olds Sources: Ornstein, P. A., & Greenhoot, A. F. (2000). Remembering Sources: Ornstein, P. A., & Greenhoot, A. F. (2000). Remembering the distant past:Proof Implications of research on children’s memory the distant past: Implications of research on children’s memory for the recovered memory debate. In D. F. Bjorklund (Ed.), False- for the recovered memory debate. In D. F. Bjorklund (Ed.), False- memory creation in children and adults: Theory, research, and implica- memory creation in children and adults: Theory, research, and implica- tions. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. © 2000 Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, tions. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. © 2000 Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Based on data in Ornstein, P. A., Merritt, K. A., Baker-Ward, Inc. Based on data in Ornstein, P. A., Merritt, K. A., Baker-Ward, L., Furtado, E., Gordon, B. N., & Principe, G. F. (1998). Children’s L., Furtado, E., Gordon, B. N., & Principe, G. F. (1998). Children’s Draftknowledge, expectation, and long-term retention. Applied Cognitive knowledge, expectation, and long-term retention. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 12, 387–405. Psychology, 12, 387–405.

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. CHAPTER 8 MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 333

in Figure 8.8) refer to children incorrectly agreeing of anatomically correct dolls when questioning that an event happened when it did not (“Did the children who are suspected of being victims of doctor look into your ears?” “Yes.”). Here, both . Does the use of such dolls increase the 4- and 6-year-old children were more likely to the accuracy of children’s reports? Maggie Bruck erroneously say that these events did, indeed, hap- and her colleagues (1995) interviewed 3-year-old pen when they were typical rather than atypical children following a routine medical exam (these features of a physical exam. Knowing what usu- children were not suspected child-abuse victims). ally happens caused children to falsely remember Half of the children received a genital exam by what did happen, at least when their memory was the doctor, and half did not. Immediately after tested 12 weeks after the event. the examination, children were shown an ana- A similar finding was produced by Henry tomically correct doll and were asked,distribute with the Otgaar and colleagues (2010) with slightly older interviewer pointing to the genital area of the doll, children, 7 to 11 years old. Regardless of age, “Did the doctor touch you here?”or Only about half children developed more false memories after lis- of the children who did receive the genital exam tening to false narratives describing events about answered correctly, whereas about half of those which they had a lot of knowledge (for exam- who did not receive a genital exam also said yes. ple, getting their fingers caught in a mousetrap) When simply asked to “show on the doll” how versus little knowledge (for example, receiving a the doctor hadpost, touched their genitals or buttocks, rectal enema). These studies provide convincing only 25% of the children who had received the evidence that script knowledge boosts children’s genital exam responded correctly, and 50% of the false memories. children who were not given such an exam falsely showed anal or genital touching. Similar results Characteristics of the interview. Not surpris-copy,have been reported by other researchers (see Poole ingly, how children are interviewed can greatly & Bruck, 2012). Other research indicates that affect what they remember and the accuracy of interviewers who use dolls and other objects to their ­recollections (D. A. Brown & Lamb,not 2015; aid recall ask fewer open-ended questions and are Bruck et al., 2006). The type of questions asked, less likely to stay on topic than interviewers who for example, influences what children remember. do not use such objects (Melinder et al., 2010). Young children tend to recallDo relatively little to Findings such as these call into question the use open-ended, free-recall questions- (“Tell me every- of anatomically correct dolls, at least with young thing that happened when the man came into your children, indicating that the dolls themselves may classroom”), but what they do recall tends to be cause children to “make accusations” of abuse highly accurate. Children recall more when given when no abuse occurred. neutral cues, but the accuracy of their recall declines How warm or supportive an interviewer is (that is, they also recall more false information). can also influence the accuracy of children’s rec- InterviewerProof characteristics, such as whether the ollections. For instance, children remember more interviewer is warm and supportive or high sta- correct information, and less incorrect infor- tus (for example, a police officer), influences the mation, when they are questioned by a warm accuracy of children’s memory, as does the use of and supportive interviewer (Bush et al., 2014; Draftany special recall-facilitating technique or props. Quas, Bauer, & Boyce, 2004). In one study, 4- to For example, many forensic interviews make use 6-year-old children who had high levels of stress

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. 334 CHILDREN’S THINKING

showed increased levels of recall accuracy when (leading) questions. William Cassel and I (DB) they were questioned by an emotionally support- (1995) showed groups of 6- and 8-year-old chil- ive interviewer but reduced levels of accuracy dren and college adults a brief video of a boy and when questioned by a nonsupportive interviewer a girl in a park, with the boy eventually taking (Quas et al., 2004). the girl’s bike without permission. Participants were interviewed 15 minutes after viewing the video and then again 1 week and 1 month later. Age Differences in Suggestibility During these later interviews, participants were given either sets of misleading questions, suggest- Perhaps the single most investigated area of eye- ing things that did not happen (for example, “The witness testimony in both the adult and child lit- girl said it was okay for the boy todistribute take her bike, eratures concerns suggestibility (for a review, see didn’t she?”), or positive-leading questions, sug- Ceci, Hritz, & Royer, 2016). To what extent are gesting things that did, indeed,or happen (for exam- children susceptible to suggestion? Research has ple, “The girl told the boy not to take the bike, shown that people of all ages report more inac- didn’t she?”). Figures 8.9 and 8.10 present some curate information when misleading questions of the results for the 1-week interview. As can be are posed (that is, questions suggesting incorrect seen, the 6- and 8-year-olds tended to follow the “facts”). The questions for developmentalists, and lead of the post,interviewer, agreeing both with the for the legal profession, include are children more misleading questions (see Figure 8.9), thus getting suggestible than adults, what factors influence their suggestibility, and how can we maximize memory accuracy and minimize suggestibility? FIGURE 8.9 Percentage of correct and incorrect responses by age to misleading questions. The general consensus regarding the question copy, of whether children are more suggestible than 100 adults seems to be yes. In an extensive review of the early literature, Stephen Ceci andnot Maggie Correct Bruck (1993) concluded, “There do appear to be 80 Incorrect significant age differences in suggestibility, with preschool children being disproportionately more vulnerable to suggestion thanDo either school-age 60 children or adults” (p. 431).- Most investigators looking for age differences in suggestibility have 40 found it, although in varying degrees and some- times only under certain circumstances (Ackil & of Responses rcentage Pe Zaragoza,Proof 1995; Bruck et al., 1995). 20 0 How Do Children Respond to 6-Year-Olds 8-Year-Olds Adults Misleading Questions? Source: Based on data from Cassel, W. S., & Bjorklund, D. F. (1995). Developmental patterns of eyewitness memory and suggest- DraftLet us provide an example from research that ibility: An ecologically based short-term longitudinal study. Law & asked children different types of suggestive Human Behavior, 19, 507–532.

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. CHAPTER 8 MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 335

more wrong than the adults did, and with the 71% of the 6-year-olds who had agreed with the positive-leading questions (see Figure 8.10), thus suggestion of the first interviewer later changed getting more right than the adults did. In fact, as their answers to comply with the suggestion of can be seen in Figure 8.9, both the 6- and 8-year- the second interviewer. The corresponding per- olds had more incorrect than correct responses to centages for the 8-year-old children and adults the misleading questions, whereas the reverse was were 53% and 35%, respectively. For the more true for the adults. critical central question—that is, whether the In the 1-month interview, participants were girl had given the boy permission to take her first asked sets of leading questions by one exam- bike—42% of the 6-year-old children changed iner suggesting one interpretation (either mis- their answers in response to the second inter- leading or positive leading) and then immediately viewer, whereas only 7% of 8-year-oldsdistribute and thereafter were asked a second set of questions by 12% of adults did so. These results make it clear a second examiner, asking for the opposite inter- that younger children areor highly susceptible to pretation (misleading if positive leading had been the suggestion of an adult interviewer, modify- asked first, and vice versa). How would children ing their answers, it seems, to suit the desires of respond to the same question they had just been whomever is interviewing them. asked, but with an opposite spin? The answer is Yet despite the ease with which young chil- that children often changed their answers. For dren can bepost, led to give answers consistent with example, when asked about the color of the bike, the suggestion of any adult, are their memo- ries actually changed as well? Research that FIGURE 8.10 Percentage of correct and incorrect has asked children misleading questions over responses by age to positive-leading questions. repeated interviews indicates that such repetition copy,will, indeed, cause some children to report the 100 incorrect information in later tests of free recall Correct and recognition, particularly information that is not peripheral, or incidental, to the event. Such find- 80 Incorrect ings indicate that suggestive questioning changes not only children’s answers but also their memo- 60 Do ries. However, in many other cases, children who - follow the lead of an interviewer fail to incorpo- rate that misinformation, especially information 40 that is central to the event, in their subsequent free recall of the event, suggesting that it is far rcentage of Responses rcentage

Pe easier to change children’s answers with such 20 questions than it is to change their minds (see Proof Bjorklund, Brown, & Bjorklund, 2002). 0 In an early investigation, Gail Goodman and 6-Year-Olds 8-Year-Olds Adults Allison Clarke-Stewart (1991) illustrated the Source: Based on data from Cassel, W. S., & Bjorklund, D. F. (1995). extent to which young children’s reports of a Developmental patterns of eyewitness memory and suggestibility: DraftAn ecologically based short-term longitudinal study. Law & witnessed event can be swayed and the extent Human Behavior, 19, 507–532. to which they will stick to their interpretation.

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. 336 CHILDREN’S THINKING

Preschool children watched a man, posing as a Stone is always getting into accidents and break- janitor, who either cleaned and arranged some ing things!”). Children in the suggestion condi- toys, including a doll, or played with the toys tion were interviewed several times after Sam in a somewhat rough and inappropriately sug- Stone’s visit and given misinformation about the gestive manner. About an hour later, the jani- visit (“Sam ripped a book and soiled a teddy bear tor’s “boss” interviewed the children about what when he visited”). Children in the stereotype- they had seen. Of primary concern here is the plus-suggestion condition received both the nega- situation in which children watched the janitor tive stereotype before Sam Stone’s visit and the merely cleaning the toys, although it was sug- misinformation afterward, and children in the gested to them that he had actually been playing control condition received neither the stereotyped with the toys improperly instead of doing his job. information nor the misinformationdistribute about Sam. If a child in this situation initially did not agree Ten weeks after the visit, the children were with the interviewer’s suggestion, subsequent given an open-ended interviewor about what hap- leading questions were asked, with each question pened the day Sam Stone visited the classroom. becoming increasingly stronger in its suggestion Leichtman and Ceci (1995) reported that, rela- (that is, more explicitly suggesting misbehavior). tive to children in the control condition, chil- Two thirds of these children eventually followed dren who had been given the stereotypes made the interviewers’ suggestions, even though the a modest numberpost, of false statements about Sam suggestions did not correspond to what they in the interview and that children in the sugges- had seen. Moreover, when the children were tion condition made a substantial number of questioned by their parents at the end of the ses- false reports. The highest levels of false reports sion, all stuck with the story that they had given about Sam’s visit, however, came from children the interviewer. In sum, when suggestions and copy,who had received both the stereotyped infor- accusations were strong and persistent, young mation before and the misinformation after the children were easily led and did not alter their visit: 46% of 3- and 4-year-old children and newfound interpretations when later questionednot 30% of 5-and 6-year-old children said that Sam by their parents. had either ripped a book, soiled a teddy bear, In other research, Leichtman and Ceci (1995) or both. The percentage of erroneous responses assessed the effects of negativeDo stereotyping and increased to 72% and 44% for the younger and suggestion on preschool children’s- recollections older preschoolers, respectively, when children of an event that happened at their school. An were asked specific follow-up questions concern- unfamiliar person, Sam Stone, came into chil- ing whether Sam had ripped a book or soiled a dren’s classrooms, talked to the teacher, sat with teddy bear. the children during the of a story, made Why are younger children often more sus- a comment about the story (“I know that story; ceptible to the effects of misinformation and it’s one ofProof my favorites!”), walked around the suggestion than older children are? One explana- classroom, and finally left the room, waving tion comes from fuzzy-trace theory, as discussed good-bye to the children. Children in the ste- earlier (Brainerd & Reyna, 2014). Because ver- reotype condition were given information about batim traces deteriorate rapidly, they may not DraftSam Stone before his visit that depicted him as be available when postevent information is pro- accident prone and irresponsible (“That Sam vided or when suggestive questions are asked.

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. CHAPTER 8 MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 337

Thus, the erroneous information has an excellent the bike something I saw or something someone chance of being incorporated with “real” memo- told me? Research has shown that preschool and ries and becoming indistinguishable from them. early school-age children often have difficulty Similarly, it would seem that young children’s monitoring the source of their memories (see elevated rates of erroneous information to unbi- discussion in Chapter 5). For example, children ased cues (that is, questions that ask for more sometimes have difficulty determining whether information but do not attempt to bias a child’s they actually performed an act or just imagined answer one way or the other) might be the result it (Foley, Santini, & Sopasakis, 1989), and they of their greater reliance on verbatim traces. often incorrectly remember that an action car- Many factors influence suggestibility in chil- ried out by another person during a joint activ- dren. For example, social factors, such as a desire ity was actually performed by themdistribute (Ackil & to comply with adult requests, surely play a role ­Zaragoza, 1995; Foley, Ratner, & Gentes, in children’s greater suggestibility (Bjorklund 2010). When young childrenor make errors in such et al., 1998). Children are more likely to comply situations, they are much more likely to attribute with the suggestion of a high-status versus a low- an action to themselves that someone else actu- status person (Ceci, Ross, & Toglia 1987). Chil- ally did than vice versa. Findings such as these dren’s background knowledge for the witnessed have caused some researchers to propose that event also influences their performance ­(Ornstein young children’spost, increased susceptibility to sug- et al., 2006), as does the number of times an gestion might be caused largely by their difficulty event is experienced (Powell et al., 1999). Much in monitoring the source of what they know research has investigated how other aspects of (Ceci & Bruck, 1995). Some research supports children’s cognitive development influence sug- these speculations, showing that 6-year-olds who gestibility. For example, developmental andcopy, are poor at source monitoring are more prone individual differences in and to the effects of suggestion (Mazzoni, 1998). inhibitory control are related to suggestibility Moreover, children who are given some source- in children (Ruffman et al., 2001). notPreschool monitoring training (for example, training in children who perform better on theory-of-mind distinguishing between events they experienced tasks tend to be less suggestible than are children directly versus events they heard about in a pre- with poorer theory-of-mindDo abilities (Welch, vious interview) make fewer false statements to 1999), and children and adults- with better meta- misleading questions (Thierry & Spence, 2002), cognitive skills are more accurate in suggestive although some researchers have found this effect interviews than are their less-metacognitively only for older (7- and 8-year-old) children and sophisticated peers (Roebers, 2002). not for preschoolers (Poole & Lindsay, 2002). One topic that has received considerable Another reason contributing to young chil- attention is that of source monitoring, which dren’s suggestibility might be their beliefs refers to beingProof aware of the source of informa- that their memory is invulnerable to sugges- tion one knows or remembers. For example, tion, or poor . For example, Julia did a particular experience happen to them, to O’Sullivan, Mark Howe, and Tammy Marche a friend, or did they see it on TV? This can be (1996) interviewed preschool, first-grade, and Draftparticularly important in cases of eyewitness tes- third-grade children about factors that might timony. Was the information that the boy stole influence their memories. Although children of

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. 338 CHILDREN’S THINKING

all ages believed that central aspects of a story event) for the 3- and 4-year-old children and for would be more likely to be remembered than the 5- and 6-year-old children in this study over peripheral details, only the third-grade children the 11-week period is shown in Figure 8.11. believed that their memories would be suscep- Although few children admitted to experiencing tible to suggestion. The preschool and first-grade these false events in the initial interviews, by the children were confident that suggestion from a conclusion of the study more than 50% of the parent or sibling would not affect their recollec- 3- and 4-year-old children and about 40% of tion of an event. 5- to 6-year-olds assented that these events did, indeed, happen—and often provided substan- tial detail about the events. Moreover, many False-Memory Creation children continued to believe thatdistribute these events How easy is it to create a in a person? actually happened even after being told by the Asking children misleading questions about an interviewers and their parentsor that the events event they have seen can cause children to confuse were just made up. It seems that false memories the source of the information, thinking that the misinformation was actually something they expe- rienced and not just something they heard someone else say (Ackil & Zaragoza, 1995). But how easy FIGURE 8.11 post,Percentage of false reports over is it to actually get children, or adults, to believe an sessions for 3- and 4-year-olds and 5- and 6-year-olds. event happened to them that never really did? and her colleagues first inves- 60 tigated this with adults (Loftus & Pickrell, 1995). 3–4-year-olds College students were interviewed about four copy, events that had supposedly happened to them in 50 5–6-year-olds childhood (based on reports of parents and older ). One event, being lost in a mallnot at age 5, 40 never actually occurred. The students were asked to write as much about each event as they could 30 remember. They remembered,Do on average, 68% of the true events; they also- “remembered” 25% of the false events, sometimes vividly. 20 rcentage of False Assents of False rcentage

It seems that preschool children are even more Pe susceptible to creating false memories than are 10 adults. Ceci and his colleagues (1994) used a sim- ilar technique to that used by Loftus and Pickrell 0 (1995) andProof interviewed children throughout an 108642 11-week period about events that might have Sessions happened to them. For example, children were Source: Ceci, S. J., Loftus, E. F., Leichtman, M., & Bruck, M. (1994). asked if they remember ever getting their finger The role of source misattributions in the creation of false beliefs among preschoolers. International Journal of Clinical and Draftcaught in a mousetrap. The percentage of false Experimental Hypnosis, 62, 304–320. © 1994 by SAGE Publications, reports (that is, recalling something about the Inc. Reprinted by permission.

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. CHAPTER 8 MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 339

of plausible but extraordinary events are rela- real memory traces or just compliance. They pre- tively easy to put into young children’s minds. sented 8-year-olds with a about a true Other research has examined how rumors (first day at school) and false event (hot air bal- may influence children’s memory of events. loon ride). Across two interviews, the majority of For example, in research by Gabrielle Principe children developed at least a partial false mem- and her colleagues (Principe et al., 2006, 2010, ory of the balloon ride. To analyze whether the 2012), preschool children witnessed a magic false memories were guided by children’s com- show by Magic Mumfrey, during which, toward pliance with the experimenter or actual memory the end of the show, Mumfrey tried to pull a traces, the researchers examined how quickly rabbit out of his hat but failed. After the show, children responded yes or no to statements that some children heard an unfamiliar adult tell their were either consistent or inconsistentdistribute with the teacher that she knew why the trick failed: The true and false events. On some trials, children rabbit had escaped and was in the classroom were instructed to tell theor truth, and on other eating carrots. Other children didn’t hear the trials they were instructed to lie. So, for example, rumor from an adult but were classmates of the when instructed to tell the truth, a yes response children who did, and they heard the rumor as to a statement consistent with the real narrative they interacted with children in their classroom. would be correct. When instructed to lie, a no Finally, some children saw the show but were response wouldpost, be correct. The manipulation not exposed to any rumor. One week later, chil- becomes a bit more complicated when we look dren were interviewed about the show. A major- at the false event. For children who correctly ity of children in both rumor conditions reported rejected the false narrative as never occurring, that the rabbit had gotten loose in the classroom, a yes response would be the correct response and many of these children actually professedcopy, for the statement, “I have been on a hot air bal- seeing the runaway bunny. Five- and six-year-old loon ride” when instructed to lie. However, for children who were warned that there was a false children who indicate they have a false memory rumor going around about what had nothappened of the balloon ride, the expected response when in the school made fewer such claims when they instructed to lie would be no. heard the rumor from their classmates, although The researchers first looked at response times such a warning did not affectDo 3- to 4-year-old to the true events (school-related trials) and children’s reports. Moreover,- children who found that children took longer to respond when said they saw the rabbit in the classroom pro- they had to lie versus when they had to tell the vided more vivid descriptions of the (non)event truth. Next, they compared these response times than children who did not, suggesting that the to those of children with false memories who implanted rumor produced a true false memory, were responding to statements about the false complete with perceptual details, and was not event (hot air balloon ride). The rationale was simply theProof result of children wanting to conform that if children’s false memories were caused by with what other children had witnessed (Principe faulty memory traces, then denying that the false et al., 2010). event took place would actually feel like a lie. Henry Otgaar and colleagues (Otgaar et al., These trials should then demonstrate elevated Draft2012) were further interested in the question of response times and error rates relative to trials in whether children’s implanted memories represent which they have to confirm that the event took

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. 340 CHILDREN’S THINKING

place. However, if implanted false memories are world, developing paradigms where researchers based on compliance (in other words, they never have some control over the situation but where believed the ride took place, but they said it did children remember “real” events that may have to please the experimenter), then the pattern legal implications, which some researchers have should be the opposite: Denying that the false done. For example, researchers have assessed chil- event occurred would be a truthful response, and dren’s recollections of events surrounding natural response times should be relatively quick. The disasters (Ackil, Van Abbema, & Bauer, 2003; pattern of results confirmed that false memories Bahrick et al., 1998). Other real-life research par- were indeed treated like true memories, with adigms involve assessing children’s recollections children taking longer when they had to “lie” of invasive medical procedures—some traumatic about these events, suggesting they were based (such as trips to the emergency room)distribute and others in actual faulty memory traces, not just a desire not (such as well-child pediatric exams). In general, to conform. older children remember moreor than younger chil- dren, and prior knowledge of the exam procedure is positively related to amount of initial (but not Final Thoughts on Children delayed) recall of the events (see Peterson, 2012). as Eyewitnesses Other studies attempt to construct forensic inter- views similarpost, to those that abused children would Children do not have perfect memories, making experience (D. A. Brown & Lamb, 2015; Pipe & them less than ideal witnesses. Of course, adults’ Salmon, 2009). Such studies afford control of vari- memories are also fallible, so how different are ables that are difficult, if not impossible, to control children from adults when it comes to providing in real life. However, there is always the question reliable testimony? As we’ve seen, young chil-copy,of the ecological validity of the studies. How do dren’s spontaneous recall is typically sparse, but we know that children who experience child abuse it also tends to be accurate and about psychologi- behave in the same way? This is where field studies, cally and legally central aspects of an event.not They interviewing children who are suspected victims of are also more prone to suggestion than adults, at child abuse, become important (D. A. Brown & least in most contexts (but see Brainerd & Reyna, Lamb, 2015). Of course, field studies lack control 2012, for important exceptions).Do Since the 1980s, of important factors, and one does not know for when issues about the accuracy- of children’s tes- certain what children actually experienced. But timony became of increasing interest to the legal the combination of laboratory and field studies community (Ceci & Bruck, 1995), developmental can provide greater insight into the reliability and psychologists have investigated children’s event accuracy of children as witnesses. memories for forensically relevant information This research has made significant contribu- and have discovered important things about the tions to the legal system, affecting how chil- emotional,Proof social, and cognitive factors that influ- dren are interviewed by police officers, social ence children’s reports and memories. workers, and lawyers (Bruck et al., 2006; Howe, However, most of the investigations described 2013) and the development of a number of pro- in this section were laboratory studies. Because tocols to help those in the legal profession get Draftchildren’s testimony may have significant legal con- the most accurate information possible when sequences, it is important to do research in the real interviewing children. For example, the National

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. CHAPTER 8 MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 341

TABLE 8.1 Sequence of phases recommended by the NICHD guidelines.

1. Introduction of parties and their roles 2. The “truth and lie ceremony” (warning the child of the necessity to tell the truth) 3. Rapport building 4. Description of a recent salient event 5. First narrative account of the allegation 6. Narrative accounts of the last incident (if the child reports multiple incidents) 7. Cue question (for example, “You said something about a barn. Tell me about that”) 8. Paired direct-open questions about the last incident 9. Narrative account of first incident 10. Cue questions distribute 11. Paired direct-open questions about the first incident 12. Narrative accounts of another incident that the child remembers or 13. Cue questions 14. Paired direct-open questions about this incident 15. If necessary, leading questions about forensically important details not mentioned by the child 16. Invitation for any other information the child wants to mention

Source: Adapted from Poole, D. A., & Lamb, M. E. (1998). Investigative interviews of children:post, A guide of helping professionals (pp. 98–99). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Institute of Child Health and Human Develop- ment (NICHD) protocol incorporates research • The accuracy of children’s event memory is influenced by a host of factors, including findings from both laboratory and field settingscopy, their knowledge for the event they expe- (Lamb, Sternberg, & Esplin, 1998), and an out- rienced and the characteristics of the in- line of the protocol is presented in Table 8.1. terview, such as the use of an anatomically We may never know with 100% notcertainty correct doll. the veracity of a child’s recollections (or that • Young children are generally more suscep- of an adult, for that matter), but developmen- tible to misleading questions (suggestions) tal science has made great leapsDo in understand - and misinformation than are older chil- ing children’s memory and factors that affect its dren and can easily be caused to form false - memories. accuracy. • Many factors influence children’s suggest- ibility, including deficits insource monitoring, with children confusing the source of infor- mation they know or remember. Section Review Proof Ask Yourself . . . • Age differences are found in the amount of information children remember, but 12. What are the major age differences found what young children do recall tends to be in children’s eyewitness testimony? accurate and for central components of an 13. How suggestible are children? What fac- Draftevent. tors affect the accuracy of their memories?

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. 342 CHILDREN’S THINKING

REMEMBERING As you can see, even the 2-year-olds were very good at reminding their parents when the task TO REMEMBER was one of high interest and over short delays. Performance for all children dropped sharply, To this point, our discussion of memory has however, for the low-interest and long-delay focused on what is called , tasks. In fact, none of the 2-year-olds spontane- or remembering something that happened in ously remembered to remind their parents about the past. But another type of memory, one that a low-interest task over a long delay. Children we engage in every day, is prospective memory, improved when given a clue (“Was there some- or remembering to do something in the future thing you were supposed to remind me to do?”), (Einstein & McDaniel, 2005). For example, if increasing their performance by distributeabout a third, you need to pick up a bottle of wine tonight on average. These results suggest that motivation on your way home from school or work for plays an important role in orwhen children display that special dinner and remember to do so, you prospective memory, as does the length of time are demonstrating good prospective memory. they have to wait. If you neglect to do so, you are displaying a More recent research using controlled labo- failure of prospective memory (and, perhaps, a ratory experiments has shown that 2-year-olds less successful evening than you had hoped for). post, Prospective memory involves what some people refer to as —anticipating the FIGURE 8.12 Proportion of children who future and planning for it (Suddendorf & Cor- reminded their parents to perform some task ballis, 2007; Tulving, 2005). This requires a for 2-, 3-, and 4-year-old children for high- and sophisticated cognition/memory system. copy,low-interest tasks over short and long delays. Researchers have paid less attention to the development of prospective memory relative 0.9 to retrospective memory, but this is notgradually 0.8 changing (see Mahy, Moses, & Kliegel, 2014, 0.7 for a review). One of the first studies to examine 0.6 prospective memory in young children was per- 0.5 formed by Susan Somerville Doand her colleagues (1983), who asked the parents- of 2-, 3-, and 0.4 4-year-old children to remind them to perform 0.3 some task in the future, some of which were 0.2 of low interest to the children (for example, 0.1 “Remind me to buy milk when we go to the 0 store tomorrow”)Proof and others of which were of 2 years 3 years 4 years high interest (for instance, buying candy at the High interest/Short delay Low interest/Short delay store). Figure 8.12 shows the performance of High interest/Long delay Low interest/Long delay the children for the high- and low-interest tasks Source: Adapted from Somerville, S. C., Wellman, H. M., & Cultice, for short (5 minutes or less) and longer (for J. C. (1983). Young children’s deliberate reminding. Journal of Draftexample, from morning to afternoon) delays. Genetic Psychology, 143, 87–96.

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. CHAPTER 8 MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 343

rarely perform as well as Somerville’s youngsters reported a moderate but significant negative did (Kliegel & Jager, 2007) and that prospec- correlation (r = –0.29) between executive-func- tive memory continues to improve over the pre- tion measures and the number of times children school (Quon & Atance, 2010) and school years ran out of gas. Subsequent research reported sig- (R. E. Smith, Bayen, & Martin, 2010; Yang, nificant relations between measures of strategic Chan, & Shum, 2011). time monitoring (checking a clock to indicate Many factors influence age differences in the passage of every 5 minutes while watch- prospective memory performance, including ing a film), prospective memory success (Voigt the type of task. Somerville’s tasks involved et al., 2015), and metacognition (Causey, 2010). event-based prospective memory, a type that Other research has shown that executive func- researchers distinguish from time-based pro- tion predicts 3-year-olds’ performancedistribute on a spective memory. Whereas event-based tasks low-interest event-based prospective memory require us to remember to do something task (“Remind me to removeor the sign on the after some specific circumstances occur (for door when we leave”) but not for high-interest instance, being at the store), time-based tasks tasks (“Remind me to get you a prize when require that we act at a particular point in time we finish”) (Causey & Bjorklund, 2014). These or after the passage of a specific duration (for patterns of findings suggest that the relation instance, attending a meeting at 2:00). Chil- between executivepost, function and prospective dren’s performance on these tasks varies as a memory is complex and highly task dependent. function of age, with children remembering Prospective memory involves a host of cog- event-based tasks at a younger age than time- nitive skills, among them a symbolic system based tasks, but the general trend is improve- that can represent the self in the future, which ment across development. For instance, in onecopy, is the essence of episodic memory, and exec- study, 7- to 12-year-old children played a com- utive function (Causey & Bjorklund, 2014; puter game called CyberCruiser, a time-based Ford et al., 2012). In fact, some people have task, in which they used a joystick to maneuvernot referred to this type of memory as episodic around obstacles. They also had to occasion- future thought (Atance, 2015; Nigro et al., ally check the fuel gauge to make sure they 2014). Relatedly, self-awareness in the form did not run out of gas and toDo fill up when they of metacognition seems important in ensur- had less than a quarter of- a tank. Perhaps not ing children implement relevant strategies surprisingly, younger children ran out of gas (like clock checking) to succeed in prospec- more often than older children (Kerns, 2000). tive memory tasks (Causey, 2010; Mahy & Like performance on some retrospective Moses, 2011). Apparently, under some limited memory tasks (Lehmann & Hasselhorn, 2007), conditions with delays of 5 minutes or less, children’sProof performance on prospective memory this is something that 2-year-old children can tasks is influenced by individual differences in do. However, the tendency to travel through executive function (Causey, 2010; Causey & time and to remember to do something in the Bjorklund, 2014; Mahy & Moses, 2011). For future develops with age, as children’s repre- example, in the study by Kimberly Kerns (2000) sentational abilities, memories, and executive Draftdescribed in the previous paragraph, the author functions improve.

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. 344 CHILDREN’S THINKING

Section Review ­motivation, executive function, representa- tional ability, and metacognition. • Developmental differences are found in pro- spective memory, which refers to remem- Ask Yourself . . . bering to do something in the future. 14. How are time-based tasks different from • Researchers distinguish between event- event-based tasks? How might children’s based and time-based prospective strategies differ across each type? memory. 15. How is the development of prospective • Age differences in performance on these memory influenced by executive function? tasks are influenced by many extrinsic What about representational ability? Meta- and intrinsic factors, including task type, cognition? distribute or KEY TERMS AND CONCEPTS

autobiographical memories event memory post,procedural memory conjugate-reinforcement explicit memory prospective memory procedure implicit memory scripts declarative memory infantile amnesia semantic memory deferred imitation nondeclarative memory source monitoring dentate gyrus preference-for-novelty episodic memory paradigmscopy, not SUGGESTED READINGS Do Scholarly Works the extent to which they remember falsely or can be - misled. The book also provides an excellent account Bauer, P., & Fivush, R. (2014). The Wiley handbook on of fuzzy-trace theory. A concise summary of this the development of children’s memory. New York: work can be found in Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. Wiley. This two-volume edited handbook includes up- (2002). Fuzzy-trace theory and false memory. Current to-date reviews written by leading researchers in the Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 164–169. field, reviewing specific topics in children’s memory Principe, G. F., Ceci, S. J., & Bruck, M. (2014). Chil- development.Proof dren’s memory: Psychology and the law. New Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (2005). The science York: Wiley. This book presents the latest research of false memory. Oxford, UK: Oxford University and theory on children’s eyewitness memory and sug- Press. This book, by the originators of the fuzzy-trace gestibility, including children’s recollections of trau- theory, provides a detailed look at the research and matic events and autobiographical memory. theory pertinent to false-memory creation in general, Schneider, W. (2015). Memory development from Draftincluding research on children as eyewitnesses and early childhood through emerging adulthood.

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. CHAPTER 8 MEMORY DEVELOPMENT 345

Cham, ­Switzerland: Springer International. This investigative interviews of children victims and wit- book, by one of the leading researchers in children’s nesses. West Sussex, UK: Wiley. Child forensic sci- memory, provides an up-to-date review of research ence is one of the fields in which cognitive development and theory on memory development. research has had a great practical impact. This book, written by leading researchers in the field, provides a recent summary of how professionals (for example, Reading for Personal Interest psychologists, police officers, social workers) should Lamb, M. E., Hershkowitz, I., Orbach, Y., & Esplin, approach children suspected of abuse and child wit- P. W. (2008). Tell me what happened: Structured nesses to obtain reliable accounts of what happened. distribute or

post,

copy, not Do -

Proof

Draft

Copyright ©2017 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher.