Investigating the Possible Relationships Between Narrative Empathy, Neuroticism and Reading Experience a Quantitative Survey Study Among Dutch Adults

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Investigating the Possible Relationships Between Narrative Empathy, Neuroticism and Reading Experience a Quantitative Survey Study Among Dutch Adults Investigating the possible relationships between narrative empathy, neuroticism and reading experience A quantitative survey study among Dutch adults Abstract This research project aimed to investigate the possible relationships between narrative empathy, neuroticism and reading experience. By doing this, it contributes to the literature on the relationship between narrative empathy and readers characteristics, a relatively young area of research (Koopman, 2015a), because most studies investigating narrative empathy have been focused on its relationship with text characteristics (Keen, 2006). Narrative empathy has been defined by Keen (2013) as the sharing of feeling and perspective-taking induced by reading, viewing, hearing, or imagining narratives of another’s situation and condition. Neuroticism, a personality characteristic, reflects the tendency to be highly emotional, impulsive, anxious, and stress reactive (Costa & McCrae, 1992b). Reading experience refers to the amount of literature someone has read in the past. An online survey (N = 92) was used to study these relationships, which included a short narrative and several questionnaires. Based on previous literature, a positive correlation between narrative empathy and neuroticism was hypothesized, which was found by the current study as well. Additionally, the study investigated the possible relationship between narrative empathy and reading experience, which has been proposed as a positive correlation by academics, although this was not supported by results of the current study. In conclusion, the study gathered interesting insights in the relationships between narrative empathy, neuroticism and reading experience useful for both academia and broader contexts. By: Bo van Beuningen (s1008644) Supervisor: L. S. Eekhof Communication- and Information Sciences Bachelor thesis Radboud University 8-6-2020 Table of contents Introduction ................................................................................................................................ 3 Background ............................................................................................................................ 3 Theoretical background .......................................................................................................... 3 Research question and hypotheses ......................................................................................... 8 Method ..................................................................................................................................... 11 Instruments ........................................................................................................................... 11 Participants ........................................................................................................................... 14 Procedure .............................................................................................................................. 15 Statistical treatment .............................................................................................................. 15 Results ...................................................................................................................................... 16 Conclusion and discussion ....................................................................................................... 18 References ................................................................................................................................ 22 Appendices ............................................................................................................................... 31 2 Introduction Background For a long time, researchers in the field of philosophy and social psychology have been interested in the study of empathy (Unger & Thumuluri, 1997), while only in the recent years, research on the relationship between reading narratives and social cognition has been increased (Eekhof, Van Krieken, & Willems, in preparation). According to many scholars, narrative empathy, the experience of feeling along with story characters, plays a key role in our engagement with narratives (Keen, 2006). Previous research on narrative empathy has mainly been focussed on the relationship between text characteristics and narrative empathy (Keen, 2006) and not so much on the relationship between personal characteristics of the reader and narrative empathy (Koopman, 2015a). This is somewhat paradoxical, because personal characteristics could perhaps be even more influential on narrative empathy than text characteristics (Eekhof et al., in preparation). Hence, the aim of the current project is to gain more insight in this possible relationship. According to scholars, increasing readers narrative empathy or sympathy has several benefits, namely: increased empathy towards an outgroup after reading a story about this particular outgroup (Johnson, 2013), increased pro-social behaviour (Johnson, 2012), higher levels of affective empathy after reading (Bal & Veltkamp, 2013), persuasion (Green & Brock, 2000; Slater & Rouner, 2002; Van Boven & Loewenstein, 2003), and changes in attitudes and beliefs in the direction of character’s opinions or events in stories (Green, 2007; Green & Carpenter, 2011). This emphasizes the practical relevance of understanding the driving forces behind narrative empathy, which is where the current study aims to contribute to. Theoretical background Narrative empathy The word ‘empathy’ is derived from German translation of ‘Einfuhlung’ by Titchener (1909, as cited in Spreng, McKinnon, Mar, & Levine, 2009), which means ‘feeling into’ (Wispe, 1987, as cited in Spreng et al., 2009). ‘Narrative empathy’ has been defined as: ‘the sharing of feeling and perspective-taking induced by reading, viewing, hearing, or imagining narratives of another’s situation and condition’ (Keen, 2013, p. 1). This implies that not only real situations and people, as is the case with so called ‘real-life empathy’, but also narratives are able to evoke empathic reactions (Koopman, 2015a). 3 The narrative empathy vs. real-life empathy distinction is one dimension in the debate on empathy (Keen, 2013). Other two common distinctions are between ‘cognitive’ or ‘cold’ empathy versus ‘affective’ or ‘warm’ empathy (Davis, 1983; Decety & Jackson, 2006; Preston & De Waal, 2002; Vingemont, & Singer, 2006), and ‘trait empathy’ versus ‘state empathy’ (Koopman, 2015a). Empathy is often distinguished as being either cognitive (being able to understand someone else’s perspective; Leverage, Mancing, Schweickert, & William, 2011) or affective (feeling similar emotions as someone else feels; Hatfield, Cacioppo, & Rapson, 1994, as cited in Koopman, 2015a). Cognitive empathy is also known as ‘Theory of Mind’ (Leverage et al., 2011) and affective empathy has been called emotional contagion or emotional empathy (Hatfield et al., 1994, as cited in Koopman, 2015a). Many academics propose that both forms are independent and are mediated by different brain structures (Koopman, 2015a; Nathanson, 2003; Shamay-Tsoory, Aharon-Peretz, & Perry, 2009). The current research project will focus on this second type of empathy. Another well-known classification is the distinction between trait empathy and state empathy. Although the concept of trait empathy has been researched frequently in the past, there is no consensus about a clear definition (Spreng et al., 2009). Trait-empathy can be seen as a personality characteristic, which is more or less fixed, while state empathy is empathy that could be increased temporarily (Koopman, 2015a), e.g., by hearing about another’s emotional state, another’s condition or exposure to narratives (Keen, 2006). The current study will focus on narrative empathy, and therefore on state empathy, because narrative empathy means that temporary empathic reactions are evoked by narratives (Koopman, 2015a). The related concept of ‘sympathy’ was used in literature earlier than empathy, and in the early days, the two terms were even used interchangeably (Keen, 2007, as cited in Koopman, 2015a; Titchener, 1909, as cited in Koopman, 2015a). Furthermore, the two concepts are still considered related (Keen, 2006), because in practice these reactions tend to often co-occur (Kuijpers, 2014). Recently, several scholars (e.g., Busselle & Bilandzic, 2009; Coplan, 2004; Koopman, 2015a; Mar, Oatley, Djikic, & Mullin, 2011) tried to define the difference between the concepts. This resulted in the following distinction: empathy can be used to express the experience of feeling someone else’s feelings, while sympathy means feeling concern for someone, without experiencing their feelings (Busselle & Bilandzic, 2009; Keen, 2006). This can be summarized as ‘feeling with’ (empathy) vs. ‘feeling for’ (sympathy; Keen, 2006). 4 Dimensions of narrative empathy Because the main concept of the current study is narrative empathy, understanding its dimensions and driving forces is highly valuable. It has been suggested that ‘character identification’ (Keen, 2006), ‘immersion’ (Keen, 2016), ‘transportation’ (Johnson, 2012; Mar & Oatley, 2008), and ‘absorption’ (Kuijpers, Hakemulder, Tan, & Doicaru, 2014) influence the degree of narrative empathy a reader experiences. Those concepts will be outlined below. Character identification has been defined by literary scholars as: ‘the process whereby readers put themselves in the place of a character and experience what the character feels’ (Altenbernd & Lewis, 1969, in Jose & Brewer,
Recommended publications
  • The Art of Staying Neutral the Netherlands in the First World War, 1914-1918
    9 789053 568187 abbenhuis06 11-04-2006 17:29 Pagina 1 THE ART OF STAYING NEUTRAL abbenhuis06 11-04-2006 17:29 Pagina 2 abbenhuis06 11-04-2006 17:29 Pagina 3 The Art of Staying Neutral The Netherlands in the First World War, 1914-1918 Maartje M. Abbenhuis abbenhuis06 11-04-2006 17:29 Pagina 4 Cover illustration: Dutch Border Patrols, © Spaarnestad Fotoarchief Cover design: Mesika Design, Hilversum Layout: PROgrafici, Goes isbn-10 90 5356 818 2 isbn-13 978 90 5356 8187 nur 689 © Amsterdam University Press, Amsterdam 2006 All rights reserved. Without limiting the rights under copyright reserved above, no part of this book may be reproduced, stored in or introduced into a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means (electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording or otherwise) without the written permission of both the copyright owner and the author of the book. abbenhuis06 11-04-2006 17:29 Pagina 5 Table of Contents List of Tables, Maps and Illustrations / 9 Acknowledgements / 11 Preface by Piet de Rooij / 13 Introduction: The War Knocked on Our Door, It Did Not Step Inside: / 17 The Netherlands and the Great War Chapter 1: A Nation Too Small to Commit Great Stupidities: / 23 The Netherlands and Neutrality The Allure of Neutrality / 26 The Cornerstone of Northwest Europe / 30 Dutch Neutrality During the Great War / 35 Chapter 2: A Pack of Lions: The Dutch Armed Forces / 39 Strategies for Defending of the Indefensible / 39 Having to Do One’s Duty: Conscription / 41 Not True Reserves? Landweer and Landstorm Troops / 43 Few
    [Show full text]
  • Vulnerable Narcissism Is (Mostly) a Disorder of Neuroticism
    Journal of Personality 86:2, April 2018 VC 2017 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Vulnerable Narcissism Is (Mostly) a DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12303 Disorder of Neuroticism Joshua D. Miller,1 Donald R. Lynam,2 Colin Vize,2 Michael Crowe,1 Chelsea Sleep,1 Jessica L. Maples-Keller,1 Lauren R. Few,1 and W. Keith Campbell1 1University of Georgia 2Purdue University Abstract Objective: Increasing attention has been paid to the distinction between the dimensions of narcissistic grandiosity and vulnerability. We examine the degree to which basic traits underlie vulnerable narcissism, with a particular emphasis on the importance of Neuroticism and Agreeableness. Method: Across four samples (undergraduate, online community, clinical-community), we conduct dominance analyses to partition the variance predicted in vulnerable narcissism by the Five-Factor Model personality domains, as well as compare the empirical profiles generated by vulnerable narcissism and Neuroticism. Results: These analyses demonstrate that the lion’s share of variance is explained by Neuroticism (65%) and Agreeableness (19%). Similarity analyses were also conducted in which the extent to which vulnerable narcissism and Neuroticism share similar empirical networks was tested using an array of criteria, including self-, informant, and thin slice ratings of personality; interview-based ratings of personality disorder and pathological traits; and self-ratings of adverse events and functional out- comes. The empirical correlates of vulnerable narcissism and Neuroticism were nearly identical (MrICC 5 .94). Partial analyses demonstrated that the variance in vulnerable narcissism not shared with Neuroticism is largely specific to disagreeableness- related traits such as distrustfulness and grandiosity. Conclusions: These findings demonstrate the parsimony of using basic personality to study personality pathology and have implications for how vulnerable narcissism might be approached clinically.
    [Show full text]
  • Ontgonnen Verleden
    Ontgonnen Verleden Regiobeschrijvingen provincie Noord-Brabant Adriaan Haartsen Directie Kennis, juni 2009 © 2009 Directie Kennis, Ministerie van Landbouw, Natuur en Voedselkwaliteit Rapport DK nr. 2009/dk116-K Ede, 2009 Teksten mogen alleen worden overgenomen met bronvermelding. Deze uitgave kan schriftelijk of per e-mail worden besteld bij de directie Kennis onder vermelding van code 2009/dk116-K en het aantal exemplaren. Oplage 50 exemplaren Auteur Bureau Lantschap Samenstelling Eduard van Beusekom, Bart Looise, Annette Gravendeel, Janny Beumer Ontwerp omslag Cor Kruft Druk Ministerie van LNV, directie IFZ/Bedrijfsuitgeverij Productie Directie Kennis Bedrijfsvoering/Publicatiezaken Bezoekadres : Horapark, Bennekomseweg 41 Postadres : Postbus 482, 6710 BL Ede Telefoon : 0318 822500 Fax : 0318 822550 E-mail : [email protected] Voorwoord In de deelrapporten van de studie Ontgonnen Verleden dwaalt u door de historisch- geografische catacomben van de twaalf provincies in Nederland. Dat klinkt duister en kil en riekt naar spinnenwebben en vochtig beschimmelde hoekjes. Maar dat pakt anders uit. Deze uitgave, samengesteld uit twaalf delen, biedt de meer dan gemiddeld geïnteresseerde, verhelderende kaartjes, duidelijke teksten en foto’s van de historisch- geografische regio’s van Nederland. Zo geeft het een compleet beeld van Nederland anno toen, nu en de tijd die daar tussen zit. De hoofdstukken over de deelgebieden/regio’s schetsen in het kort een karakteristiek per gebied. De cultuurhistorische blikvangers worden gepresenteerd. Voor de fijnproevers volgt hierna een nadere uiteenzetting. De ontwikkeling van het landschap, de bodem en het reliëf, en de bewoningsgeschiedenis worden in beeld gebracht. Het gaat over de ligging van dorpen en steden, de verkavelingsvormen in het agrarisch land, de loop van wegen, kanalen en spoorlijnen, dijkenpatronen, waterlopen, defensielinies met fortificaties.
    [Show full text]
  • Gelderse Gaten De Voortgang Van Gelderse Gemeenten Met Het Behalen Van De Doelen Uit Het Gelders Energie Akkoord
    notitie Gelderse Gaten De voortgang van Gelderse gemeenten met het behalen van de doelen uit het Gelders Energie Akkoord datum auteurs maart 2018 Sem Oxenaar Derk Loorbach Chris Roorda Gelderse Gaten De voortgang van Gelderse gemeenten met het behalen van de doelen uit het Gelders Energie Akkoord auteurs Sem Oxenaar Derk Loorbach Chris Roorda over DRIFT Het Dutch Research Institute for Sustainability Transitions (DRIFT) is een toonaangevend onderzoeksinstituut op het gebied van duurzaamheidstransities. DRIFT staat (inter)nationaal bekend om haar unieke focus op transitiemanagement, een aanpak waarbij wetenschappelijke inzichten over transities door middel van toegepast actie-onderzoek worden vertaald in praktische handvatten en sturingsinstrumenten. Inhoud 1. Achtergrond 3 1.1. Opdracht 3 1.2. Opzet 3 1.3. Data 3 2. Waar staan gemeenten nu? 5 2.1. Energie praktijk 5 3. Tien gemeenten nader bekeken 11 3.1. Wat gebeurt er? 11 3.2. Wat valt op? 12 4. Lessen om te versnellen 13 4.1. Opgehaalde lessen 13 4.2. Vanuit Drift 13 4.3. Discussie 14 4.4. Dicht de Gelderse Gaten 15 5. Bijlagen 16 5.1. Bijlage 1: Kanttekeningen 16 5.2. Bijlage 2: Beschrijving 10 gemeenten 16 P. 2 1. Achtergrond 1.1. Opdracht DRIFT is gevraagd om vanuit transitieperspectief te reflecteren op de voortgang van gemeenten bij het behalen van doelen van het Gelders Energie Akkoord (GEA), en om hier conclusies en concrete aanbevelingen aan te verbinden. Centraal staan de hoofddoelen uit het akkoord, waaraan de gemeenten zich gecommitteerd hebben: → Een besparing in het totaal energieverbruik van 1,5% per jaar → Een toename van het aandeel hernieuwbare energieopwekking naar 14% van het totale verbruik in 2020 en 16% in 2023 → Klimaatneutraal in 2050 1.2.
    [Show full text]
  • Indeling Van Nederland in 40 COROP-Gebieden Gemeentelijke Indeling Van Nederland Op 1 Januari 2019
    Indeling van Nederland in 40 COROP-gebieden Gemeentelijke indeling van Nederland op 1 januari 2019 Legenda COROP-grens Het Hogeland Schiermonnikoog Gemeentegrens Ameland Woonkern Terschelling Het Hogeland 02 Noardeast-Fryslân Loppersum Appingedam Delfzijl Dantumadiel 03 Achtkarspelen Vlieland Waadhoeke 04 Westerkwartier GRONINGEN Midden-Groningen Oldambt Tytsjerksteradiel Harlingen LEEUWARDEN Smallingerland Veendam Westerwolde Noordenveld Tynaarlo Pekela Texel Opsterland Súdwest-Fryslân 01 06 Assen Aa en Hunze Stadskanaal Ooststellingwerf 05 07 Heerenveen Den Helder Borger-Odoorn De Fryske Marren Weststellingwerf Midden-Drenthe Hollands Westerveld Kroon Schagen 08 18 Steenwijkerland EMMEN 09 Coevorden Hoogeveen Medemblik Enkhuizen Opmeer Noordoostpolder Langedijk Stede Broec Meppel Heerhugowaard Bergen Drechterland Urk De Wolden Hoorn Koggenland 19 Staphorst Heiloo ALKMAAR Zwartewaterland Hardenberg Castricum Beemster Kampen 10 Edam- Volendam Uitgeest 40 ZWOLLE Ommen Heemskerk Dalfsen Wormerland Purmerend Dronten Beverwijk Lelystad 22 Hattem ZAANSTAD Twenterand 20 Oostzaan Waterland Oldebroek Velsen Landsmeer Tubbergen Bloemendaal Elburg Heerde Dinkelland Raalte 21 HAARLEM AMSTERDAM Zandvoort ALMERE Hellendoorn Almelo Heemstede Zeewolde Wierden 23 Diemen Harderwijk Nunspeet Olst- Wijhe 11 Losser Epe Borne HAARLEMMERMEER Gooise Oldenzaal Weesp Hillegom Meren Rijssen-Holten Ouder- Amstel Huizen Ermelo Amstelveen Blaricum Noordwijk Deventer 12 Hengelo Lisse Aalsmeer 24 Eemnes Laren Putten 25 Uithoorn Wijdemeren Bunschoten Hof van Voorst Teylingen
    [Show full text]
  • The Push and Pull of Network Mobility: How Those High in Trait-Level Neuroticism Can Come to Occupy Peripheral Network Positions
    behavioral sciences Article The Push and Pull of Network Mobility: How Those High in Trait-Level Neuroticism Can Come to Occupy Peripheral Network Positions Eric Gladstone 1,*, Kathleen M. O’Connor 2 and Wyatt Taylor 3 1 Institute for Analytical Sociology, LINKS Center for Social Network Analysis, Gatton College of Business and Economics, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY 40507, USA 2 London Business School, University of London, London NW1 4SA, UK 3 Gatton College of Business and Economics, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY 40506, USA * Correspondence: [email protected] or [email protected]; Tel.: +1-803-463-6266 Received: 8 May 2019; Accepted: 5 June 2019; Published: 28 June 2019 Abstract: Field research shows that people’s network positions are determined, at least in part, by their traits. For instance, over time, actors higher in trait-level neuroticism move out to the network periphery. What is unknown is how this happens. Drawing on personality and social psychological theory, we generated a model that could explain the movement of actors who are higher in neuroticism. Our aim is to add to the existing empirical literature on the interplay of actor level traits and social networks, and do so using methods that can establish possible causal pathways. In four experiments, we tested two explanatory mechanisms—aversion on the part of alters and avoidance on the part of focal actors. Results showed that potential alters indeed perceived actors higher in neuroticism as aversive, leading them to block these actors from well-connected spots. Specifically, low perceived levels of likability prevented actors from being nominated to better positions.
    [Show full text]
  • The Role of Personality Traits in Posttraumatic
    Psychiatria Danubina, 2012; Vol. 24, No. 3, pp 256-266 Review © Medicinska naklada - Zagreb, Croatia THE ROLE OF PERSONALITY TRAITS IN POSTTRAUMATIC STRESS DISORDER (PTSD) Nenad Jakšić1, Lovorka Brajković1, Ena Ivezić2, Radmila Topić1 & Miro Jakovljević1 1Department of Psychiatry, University Hospital Centre Zagreb, Croatia 2Psychiatric Hospital “Sveti Ivan”, Zagreb, Croatia received: 28.6.2012; revised: 10.8.2012; accepted: 22.8.2012 SUMMARY Background: A number of studies have shown that although exposure to potentially traumatic events is common, development of PTSD is relatively rare, which is one of the reasons PTSD still remains a controversial psychiatric entity. The aim of this article was to provide an overview of the research on the role of personality traits in the vulnerability, resilience, posttraumatic growth and expressions associated with PTSD. Personality based approach represents a dimensional aspect of the transdisciplinary integrative model of PTSD. Methods: We conducted a systematic search on PubMed, PsycINFO, and Academic Search Complete from 1980 (the year PTSD was first included in the DSM) and 2012 (the year the literature search was performed). Manual examination of secondary sources such as the reference sections of selected articles and book chapters were also conducted. Results: Most of the reviewed studies dealing with personality traits as vulnerability and protective factors for PTSD examined the relationship between basic personality dimensions and severity of symptoms of PTSD. These studies have applied three types of methodological designs: cross-sectional, post-trauma and pre-trauma longitudinal studies, with latter being the least common option. Conclusion: Finding that appears relatively consistent is that PTSD is positively related to negative emotionality, neuroticism, harm avoidance, novelty-seeking and self-transcendence, as well as to trait hostility/anger and trait anxiety.
    [Show full text]
  • Social and Emotional Skills Well-Being, Connectedness and Success
    Social and Emotional Skills Well-being, connectedness and success ©OECD FOREWORD Contents Foreword Foreword 3 Education systems need to prepare students for continuous effort to create the kind of binding social their future, rather than for our past. In these times, capital through which we can share experiences, ideas Introduction 4 digitalisation is connecting people, cities and continents and innovation and build a shared understanding among to bring together a majority of the world’s population in groups with diverse experiences and interests, thus 01. Measuring Social and Emotional Skills 5 ways that vastly increases our individual and collective increasing our radius of trust to strangers and institutions. potential. But the same forces have made the world also 02. Social and emotional skills drive critical life outcomes 10 more volatile, more complex, and more uncertain. And Over the last years, social and emotional skills have when fast gets really fast, being slow to adapt makes been rising on the education policy agenda and in the 03. The impact of specific social and emotional skills on life outcomes 17 education systems really slow. The rolling processes of public debate. But for the majority of students, their automation, hollowing out jobs, particularly for routine development remains a matter of luck, depending on ○ Conscientiousness – getting things done, as required and in time 17 tasks, have radically altered the nature of work and life whether this is a priority for their teacher and their and thus the skills that are needed for success. For those school. A major barrier is the absence of reliable metrics ○ Openness to experience – exploring the world of things and ideas 20 with the right human capacities, this is liberating and in this field that allow educators and policy-makers to exciting.
    [Show full text]
  • Toespraak Commissaris Van De Koning Beediging Burgemeester
    1: N.B.: De uitgesproken toespraak kan afwijken van deze tekst. Meneer Van Maaren, mevrouw Van Maaren, kinderen, leden van de raad, leden van het college, dames en heren, Meneer Van Maaren, u bent niet de eerste burgemeester die ik mag beëdigen als commissaris van de Koning. Maar u bent wel de eerste burgemeester van wie ik de gehele benoemingsprocedure - van begin tot eind - zelf heb ‘gedaan’. Van de profielschetsvergadering op 28 maart, tot uw beëdiging en installatie vandaag. Daarom begin ik graag met een woord van dank aan de leden van de vertrouwenscommissie. Ik wil u complimenteren en danken voor de prettige en zorgvuldige manier waarop we dit benoemingsproces met elkaar hebben doorlopen. De heer Rehwinkel ben ik zeer erkentelijk voor de wijze waarop hij de afgelopen tweeënhalf jaar waarnemend burgemeester in Zaltbommel is geweest. In de profielschetsvergadering heb ik de fracties gevraagd in één woord te benoemen wat zij de belangrijkste kwaliteit voor de nieuwe burgemeester vonden. Dat was gemakkelijker gevraagd dan gedaan, want politici zitten doorgaans niet om woorden verlegen. Ik heb toen genoteerd: een échte bestuurder, iemand met levenservaring, stevig en betrouwbaar, ondernemend, mensgericht en daadkrachtig, Toespraak bij beëdiging burgemeester Zaltbommel, Pieter van Maaren, 19 september 2019 2: verbindend en empathisch, authentiek, communicatief en een stevige persoonlijkheid. Een mooi palet aan kwaliteiten, waar ik er graag nog één aan toevoeg: integer. Goed voorgaan doet goed volgen. In de Gemeentewet is vastgelegd dat de burgemeester de hoeder van de bestuurlijke integriteit is. Dertig kandidaten solliciteerden naar het burgemeesterschap van de gemeente Zaltbommel. De vertrouwenscommissie zag in u, meneer Van Maaren, de persoon die het best tegemoet kwam aan de profielschets.
    [Show full text]
  • Neuroticism and Fear of COVID-19. the Interplay Between Boredom
    ORIGINAL RESEARCH published: 13 October 2020 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.574393 Neuroticism and Fear of COVID-19. The Interplay Between Boredom, Fantasy Engagement, and Perceived Control Over Time Barbara Caci1*, Silvana Miceli1, Fabrizio Scrima2 and Maurizio Cardaci1 1Department of Psychology, Educational Science and Human Movement, University of Palermo, Palermo, Italy, 2Département de Psychologie, Université de Rouen, Moint Saint-Aignan, France The Italian government adopted measures to prevent the spread of coronavirus 2019 (COVID-19) infection from March 9, 2020, to May 4, 2020 and imposed a phase of social Edited by: distancing and self-isolation to all adult citizens. Although justified and necessary, Joanna Sokolowska, University of Social Sciences and psychologists question the impact of this process of COVID-19 isolation on the mental Humanities, Poland health of the population. Hence, this paper investigated the relationship between Reviewed by: neuroticism, boredom, fantasy engagement, perceived control over time, and the fear of Cinzia Guarnaccia, University of Rennes 2 – Upper COVID-19. Specifically, we performed a cross-sectional study aimed at testing an Brittany, France integrative moderated mediation model. Our model assigned the boredom to the mediation Martin Reuter, role and both the fantasy engagement and perceived control of time to the role of University of Bonn, Germany moderators in the relationship between neuroticism and the fear of COVID-19. A sample *Correspondence: Barbara Caci of 301 subjects, mainly women (68.8%), aged between 18 and 57 years (Mage = 22.12 years; [email protected] SD = 6.29), participated in a survey conducted in the 1st-week lockdown phase 2 in Italy from May 7 to 18, 2020.
    [Show full text]
  • Can Neuroticism Predict Parkinson's Disease?
    4/21/2021 Can neuroticism predict Parkinson's disease? Parkinson's disease: Does neuroticism increase risk? Written by James Kingsland on April 20, 2021 — Fact checked by Hannah Flynn, MS Could neuroticism increase the risk of Parkinson’s disease? Jasmin Merdan/Getty Images People who score high on neuroticism in personality tests tend to experience more negative emotions and are more vulnerable to the effects of psychological stress. A study has found that such individuals had over 80% greater risk of Parkinson’s disease in the following decade compared with those who scored low on neuroticism. The association remained significant even after the researchers accounted for other risk factors for the disease, such as anxiety, depression, smoking, and low levels of physical exercise. Neuroticism is one of the so-called Big Five personality traits that remain relatively stable throughout a person’s lifetime. The other four personal characteristics are extroversion, agreeableness, openness, and conscientiousness. People who score high on neuroticism in personality tests are more prone to negative emotionalADVERTISEMENT states, such as worrying, mood swings, and irritability, and the effects of stress. https://www.medicalnewstoday.com/articles/parkinsons-disease-does-neuroticism-increase-risk#Some-limitations 4/21/2021 Can neuroticism predict Parkinson's disease? There is strong evidence that neuroticism is associated with major depression and brain disorders, such as Alzheimer’s disease and other types of dementia. However, few studies have investigated possible links with Parkinson’s disease, which is a degenerative brain disorder characterized by tremors, rigidity, and problems with movement and balance. It is estimated that around 60,000 new cases of Parkinson’s disease are diagnosed in the United States each year.
    [Show full text]
  • Tiel NIJMEGEN Oss ARNHEM Wageningen Zaltbommel
    Fietsroutenetwerk Noordoost-Brabant en Rivierenland In Noordoost-Brabant vindt u veel afwisselend natuurschoon en het gebied is uitstekend geschikt om te verkennen met de U bepaalt zelf uw route en het aantal kilometers dat u wilt fietsen. U kunt in twee richtingen fietsen, van knooppunt naar fiets of te voet. Van Lith tot aan Grave kunt u vanaf de dijk genieten van de prachtige uitzichten over de Maas. Een stukje Veer Informatie knooppunt, via (wit-groene) borden. Op elk knooppunt vindt u een handig informatiepaneel, waarop u kunt zien waar u zich op zuidelijker ligt het natuurgebied De Maashorst en Herpenduin, met een oppervlakte van maar liefst 4000 ha één van de Westbroek Amersfoort Kootwijkerbroek AarkanaaNoordeindel Achterwetering dat moment bevindt. Ontbrekende bordjes in Noordoost-Brabant kunt u doorgeven op www.routesinbrabant.nl, in Rivierenland grootste aaneengesloten natuurgebieden van Brabant. Bos, heide en cultuurgrond worden hier afgewisseld met stuifduinen, Kockengen Soestduinen Harskamp Maarssen www.uiterwaarde.nlMusschendorp op www.uiterwaarde.nl beekdalen en vennen. Zutphen Download de gratis overzetveren app! Barneveldsche Beek Aarlanderveen Maarssenbroek Bilthoven Wandel of struin eens ‘AndersBarnevel langsd de Maas’ Cultuur A Hoven Nieuwe Wetering Bilthoven Den Dolder Leusden-Centrum Liniepont (over de Lek) Vlasakkers Achterveld Heerlijk uitwaaien langs de Maas in de zomer en winter! Het ‘Anders langs de Maas’-struinpad staat garant voor urenlang wandel- Op zoek naar culturele uitstapjes in Rivierenland, bezoek www.rivierenland.nl voor een volledig overzicht. In Noordoost- Vaartijden: zomerdienst. Zuidhoek Teckop Leusden April t/m september volgens vaarschema. Maarn Laagnieuwkoop N230 plezier. Deze middellange afstandsroute tussen ‘s-Hertogenbosch en Cuijk van circa 70 km kent een zomer- én wintertraject, BrabantHarsk maaktamp u kennis met de eeuwenoude vestingstadjes Megen, Ravenstein en Grave.
    [Show full text]