Revisiting the Form and Function of Conflict

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Revisiting the Form and Function of Conflict Behavioral and Brain Sciences Revisiting the form and function of conflict: Neurobiological, psychological, and cultural cambridge.org/bbs mechanisms for attack and defense within and between groups Target Article Cite this article: De Dreu CKW, Gross J. (2019) Carsten K. W. De Dreu and Jörg Gross Revisiting the form and function of conflict: Neurobiological, psychological, and cultural Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, 2300 RB Leiden, The Netherlands Center for Research in Experimental mechanisms for attack and defense within and Economics and Political Decision Making (CREED), University of Amsterdam, 1018 WB Amsterdam, The between groups. Behavioral and Brain Sciences Netherlands and Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, 2300 RB Leiden, The Netherlands 42, e116: 1–66. doi:10.1017/ [email protected] https://www.universiteitleiden.nl/en/staffmembers/carsten-de-dreu S0140525X18002170 [email protected] http://www.joerg-gross.net Target Article Accepted: 13 September 2018 Target Article Manuscript Online: 25 Abstract September 2018 Conflict can profoundly affect individuals and their groups. Oftentimes, conflict involves a Commentaries Accepted: 11 February 2019 clash between one side seeking change and increased gains through victory and the other Keywords: side defending the status quo and protecting against loss and defeat. However, theory and behavioral game theory; biobehavioral empirical research largely neglected these conflicts between attackers and defenders, and the – approach avoidance; coevolution of strategic, social, and psychological consequences of attack and defense remain poorly under- prosociality and aggression; conflict; conflict intervention; cultural institutions; intergroup stood. To fill this void, we model (1) the clashing of attack and defense as games of strategy relations; psychological adaptations and reveal that (2) attack benefits from mismatching its target’s level of defense, whereas defense benefits from matching the attacker’s competitiveness. This suggests that (3) attack recruits neuroendocrine pathways underlying behavioral activation and overconfidence, What is Open Peer Commentary? What whereas defense invokes neural networks for behavioral inhibition, vigilant scanning, and hos- follows on these pages is known as a Treatment, in which a significant and tile attributions; and that (4) people invest less in attack than defense, and attack often fails. controversial Target Article is published Finally, we propose that (5) in intergroup conflict, out-group attack needs institutional arrange- along with Commentaries (p. 15) and an ments that motivate and coordinate collective action, whereas in-group defense benefits from Author’s Response (p. 45). See bbsonline. endogenously emerging in-group identification. We discuss how games of attack and defense org for more information. may have shaped human capacities for prosociality and aggression, and how third parties can regulate such conflicts and reduce their waste. 1. Introduction Social conflict has been part and parcel of human history and exerts a range of effects that easily exceed imagination. Conflict is associated with the rise and fall of nations and large-scale migration flows, and interferes with individual life trajectories. Conflict destroys welfare and lives, creates collective imprints and out-group resentments that transcend generations, and can cause famine and the spreading of infectious disease. Conversely, conflict drives techno- logical innovation, inspires art, and creates and destroys hierarchies. Indeed, throughout his- tory, conflicts have revised established structures and divides, introduced new views and practices, and changed the social order of individuals and their groups. Conflict can be about many things such as ownership, territorial access, status and respect, and what is right and wrong (e.g., Blattman & Miguel 2010; Bornstein 2003;DeDreu2010; Deutsch 1973; Gould 1999; Rapoport 1960; Schelling 1960). Sometimes, these conflicts are about something all parties want but that only some can have (Coombs & Avrunin 1988). Examples include politicians competing for the same senate seat, rivaling research laboratories claiming the patent ownership of a potentially lucrative technology, and superpowers seeking world hegemony. Alternatively, conflicts emerge because some parties want something that others try to prevent from happening (Durham 1976; Miller 2009; Pruitt & Rubin 1986). Examples include revisionist states seeking to capture their neighbor’s territory, activist rebels fighting elitist powerholders, companies launching hostile take-over attempts, and terrorists attacking civilian and military targets. Conflicts among nonhuman animals typically have such a structure of attack and defense (Boehm 2009; 2012; Dawkins & Krebs 1979; Sapolsky 2017; Wrangham 2018). Human con- flicts may be no different. In fact, more than two-thirds of the 2,000 militarized interstate dis- © Cambridge University Press 2019 putes that emerged since the Congress of Vienna in 1816 involved a revisionist state and a non-revisionist state (De Dreu et al. 2016a; Gochman & Maoz 1984; Wright 2014). Likewise, 68% of community disputes involved a challenger who desired a revision and a defender who protected the status quo (Ufkes et al. 2014). Finally, people often perceive the other side as the threatening aggressor who leaves them no option but to aggressively defend 2 De Dreu & Gross: Revisiting the form and function of conflict themselves, a psychological bias often fueled by leader rhetoric groups of people, and (coalitions of) nation states (e.g., (Chambers et al. 2006; Plous 1985; Staub 1996). Blattman & Miguel 2010; Bornstein 2003; Cunningham et al. Although conflicts often have an attack-defense structure, the- 2009; Gould 1999; Humphreys & Weinstein 2006; Pietrazewski ory and research have rarely made a clear distinction between 2016; Rusch 2014a). At the same time, there is a growing consen- attack and defense (Lopez 2017; Pruitt & Rubin 1986; Rusch sus to view conflicts as situations in which individuals or groups 2014a; 2014b; Wrangham 2018). We currently lack theory and cannot realize their preferred state when other individuals or research about the ways in which clashes between attackers and groups realize their own preferred state (Coombs & Avrunin defenders evolve, about the neurocognitive mechanisms that scaf- 1988;DeDreu2010; Deutsch 1973; Kelley & Thibaut 1978; fold attack and defense, and about the cultural institutions that Pruitt 1998; Schelling 1960). This conceptualization of conflict groups use to attack their neighbors or to defend against enemy as incompatibility of interests is adopted here as well. attacks. Accordingly, our aim here is to provide a framework of At the core of this unifying approach is behavioral game the- the structure, psychological adaptations, and institutional conse- ory, which offers stylized models of conflict (e.g., Camerer 2003; quences of attacker-defender conflicts. Hirshleifer 1988; Kagel & Roth 1995; Schelling 1960). Indeed, We proceed as follows. Section 2 presents a formal model of game theoretical models of conflict have been used extensively attack-defense conflict and identifies unique properties of games in the study of international tension and interstate warfare of attack and defense that are neither present in nor captured by (Bacharach & Lawler 1981; Huth & Russett 1984; Jervis 1978; canonical games of conflict that have dominated classic and con- Snyder & Diesing 1977) to understand the group dynamics and temporary conflict analysis and research. Section 3 maps these cultural arrangements that create and fuel intergroup conflict properties onto behavioral decision-making and delineates its (Abbink 2012; Bornstein 2003; Colman 2003; De Dreu et al. underlying neurocognitive mechanisms. We show that psycholog- 2014; Lacomba et al. 2014), to investigate the neural networks ical “biases” often viewed as a result of imperfect human cognitive and neuroendocrine pathways involved in cooperation and com- architectures may be functional for either attack or defense. petition (Cikara & Van Bavel 2014; Decety & Cowell 2014;De Section 4 considers games of attack and defense between groups Dreu 2012; Rilling & Sanfey 2011), and to model the evolution of people. We argue that groups face greater difficulty motivating of human prosociality and aggression (Bowles & Gintis 2011; and coordinating a collective attack of out-groups than defending Garcia et al. 2014; Henrich & McElreath 2003;Nowak2006; against outside enemies, and that an out-group attack requires dis- Rusch 2014a; West et al. 2007). tinctly different sociocultural mechanisms and institutional Almost invariably, these lines of inquiry rely on models of con- arrangements than in-group defense. Section 5 shows how games flict in which opposing decision-makers compete for the same of attack and defense, both within and between groups, may reason(s). Accordingly, these models are ill-suited to approach have shaped human capacities for cooperation and aggression. the attacker-defender conflicts between revisionist and non- revisionist states, rebels and elitist powerholders, terrorists and security officers, or progressives and traditionalists. In this sec- 2. The structure of conflict tion, we highlight the structural properties of attack-defense con- Mankind spends great amounts of energy on injuring others, and on pro- flicts that set them apart from the canonical games of conflict that tecting
Recommended publications
  • Stereotypes and Prejudice
    Stereotypes and Prejudice Their Overt and Subtle Influence in the Classroom CONNIE T. WOLFE University of Michigan STEVEN J. SPENCER Hope College The authors examine overt and subtle forms of stereotyping and prejudice. Two theories that explain overt prejudice are reviewed: realistic conflict theory and social identity theory. Although overt prejudice seems to have declined, subtle stereotyping is still pervasive. The authors review one theory, aversive racism theory, that explains this phenomenon. They also discuss two perspectives, attributional ambiguity and stereotype threat, which provide accounts of the impact of subtle racism. Both overt and subtle prejudice present challenges for the classroom. The authors describe one intervention called the jigsaw classroom that encourages work toward common goals and helps reduce the expression and impact of overt discrimination. A second intervention program, wise schooling, is presented, which aims to reduce the impact of subtle stereotypes by reducing stereotype threat. Why do prejudice and discrimination exist? Has overt racism been replaced by more subtle forms of prejudice? How does stereotyping affect its targets? In this article we describe two theories, realistic conflict theory and social identity theory, which provide an answer to the first question. We address the second question by noting that although overt discrimination has decreased, subtle forms of prejudice are still quite common and we describe one theory, aversive racism, that provides a compelling account of this change in the expression of prejudice. Finally, we answer the third question by describing two phenomena, attributional ambiguity and stereotype threat, that result from the pervasive nature of subtle stereotyping. This article is a selective overview of what social psychology has to say about these crucial issues.
    [Show full text]
  • Conflict Sensitivity and Peacebuilding Programming Guide
    CONFLICT SENSITIVITY AND PEACEBUILDING Programming Guide NOVEMBER 2016 N C O FLIC S T EM AN T A S L Y Y S S I E S & M T H E N O O I R T I E A S T N O E F M C E H L A P N M I G E & N P G I R S O E G D R M A CONFLICT 02 SENSITIVITY AND PEACEBUILDING PROGR AMMING GUIDE AcKNOWLEDGEMENTS There are many individuals across UNICEF who have provided valuable insights and contributions to the production of this guide. The guide was produced through a consultative process and informed by practical application of its content through several training workshops at country and regional levels, often tied to UNICEF programme planning processes. Special thanks go to Zachary Metz, Naghmeh Sobhani and Kristoffer Nilaus-Tarp, the three consultants who worked closely with Sharif Baaser and John Lewis (HATIS/ Programmes Division, UNICEF NYHQ) in producing the guide as part of a broader capacity development project. UNICEF would like to thank the Government of The Netherlands and the Swiss Agency for Development and Cooperation (SDC) for their generous funding contributions that made the production of this guide possible. CONFLICT SENSITIVITY AND 03 PEACEBUILDING PROGRAMMING GUIDE SUMMARY, OVERVIEW AND GUIDE TO THE GUIDE SUMMARY AND OVERVIEW The Conflict Sensitivity and Peacebuilding Programming Guide is a tool for UNICEF field staff and leadership to understand, situate and operationalize conflict sensitivity and peacebuilding through UNICEF’s existing work or new initiatives in different contexts and in partnership with other stakeholders.
    [Show full text]
  • The Theory‐Practice Research of Peter J. Carnevale
    Negotiation and Conflict Management Research From the Field to the Laboratory: The Theory-Practice Research of Peter J. Carnevale Linda L. Putnam,1 Mara Olekalns ,2 Donald E. Conlon3 and Carsten K. W. De Dreu 4,5 1 University of California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA, U.S.A. 2 Melbourne Business School, Melbourne, Vic., Australia 3 Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI, U.S.A. 4 Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands 5 University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Keywords Abstract negotiation, emotion, third party, human–computer As colleagues and collaborators, we reflect on the work and legacy of interaction, methods. Peter Carnevale, currently professor at the University of Southern Cali- fornia, and recipient of the 2002 Jeffrey Z. Rubin Theory-to-Practice Correspondence Award of the International Association for Conflict Management Linda L. Putnam, Department of (IACM). We review Carnevale’s main contributions, including his work Communication, University of on time pressure and surveillance, strategies for mediation, emotions in California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA 93106, USA; negotiation, and the use and integration of distinct methods for studying e-mail: conflict and negotiation. We share personal anecdotes from our time as [email protected]. PhD students and collaborators with Peter Carnevale, and we touch on lessons learned for doing science and mentoring the next generation. doi: 10.1111/ncmr.12185 Introduction Trained in traditional experimental research, few scholars have crossed the theory/practice divide with such a strong focus on conceptual development. Whether relying on gaps in experimental research, insights from real-world mediations, or theory development that challenges existing knowledge, Peter J.
    [Show full text]
  • The Psychology of Peace, Conflict, and Reconciliation in Northern Ireland Spring, 2019 PSY386 Tentative Syllabus (Updated 4/6/19)
    The Psychology of Peace, Conflict, and Reconciliation in Northern Ireland Spring, 2019 PSY386 Tentative Syllabus (updated 4/6/19) Professor: Ellen Shupe, Ph.D. 2218 ASH Email: [email protected] Cell: (616) 516-0818 Readings Feeney, B. (2004). A short history of the Troubles. Dublin, Ireland: The O’Brien Press. Additional readings (see list below) Course Overview The Psychology of Peace, Conflict, and Reconciliation in Northern Ireland is a 6-credit study abroad program consisting of 3 weeks of class meetings on the GVSU Allendale campus and three weeks of travel and study in the North of Ireland. The course focuses on the psychology of conflict and on the psychology of peace and reconciliation, two topics that are often studied independently. There is a long history of scholarship on the dynamics, causes, and consequences of conflict and violence, from a variety of perspectives (e.g., social, biological, and clinical) and on interpersonal, intergroup, and international levels. Unlike theory and research on conflict, the specific focus on peace and reconciliation is relatively new to psychology. The emerging field of peace psychology draws on rich theory from clinical, social, political, and community psychology, and is influenced by literature from sociology, international relations, political science and other fields outside of psychology. Thus, both the psychology of conflict and the psychology of peace and reconciliation are broad topics that could easily be studied over the course of a year or more. Although we will also discuss interpersonal and international conflict, PSY386 will focus primarily on the psychological literature related to violent intergroup and ethnopolitical conflict and peace-building efforts, as they are most relevant to Northern Ireland.
    [Show full text]
  • Ebook Download the Psychology of Conflict and Conflict Management
    THE PSYCHOLOGY OF CONFLICT AND CONFLICT MANAGEMENT IN ORGANIZATIONS 1ST EDITION PDF, EPUB, EBOOK Carsten K W De Dreu | 9781136679926 | | | | | The Psychology of Conflict and Conflict Management in Organizations 1st edition PDF Book Newstrom eds. Breaking the bonds of reciprocity in negotiations. These studies report that individuals low on need for affiliation prefer a dominating style, whereas those high on need for affiliation tend to use an obliging style. In forcing, one party aims to achieve his or her goal by imposing a solution onto the other party. Zaheer, S. Conflict within interdependence: Its value for productivity and individuality. Those differences will lead to conflict if the individuals reach different conclusions. Wiley Encyclopedia of Management , 11 , 1—4. Recently viewed 0 Save Search. Help Learn to edit Community portal Recent changes Upload file. The five styles are as follows:. The Academy of Management Review, 23 3 , — Journal of Applied Psychology , 97 2 , — How arbitration works. International Journal of Conflict Management , 5 , — Management of a business. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 66 4 , — The goal is to hold conflict levels in the middle of this range. Conflict coaching is a new and rapidly growing process in the public as well as private sector Brinkert, This style is also the best choice in situations in which one party cannot resolve the conflict alone. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves. Academy of Management Journal, 26 2 , — Types of management. It takes two to tango: An interdependence analysis of the spiraling of perceived trustworthiness and cooperation in interpersonal relationships.
    [Show full text]
  • The Effects of Racial Conflict on Organizational Performance: a Search for Theory
    New Horizons in Adult Education and Human Resource Development 13 Volume 21, Number 1/2, Winter/Spring 2007 THE EFFECTS OF RACIAL CONFLICT ON ORGANIZATIONAL PERFORMANCE: A SEARCH FOR THEORY Marilyn Y. Byrd Instructor, Business Administration and Systems University of Mary Hardin-Baylor Abstract This article addresses the effect of racial conflict on organizational performance as an issue that needs theoretical support in the foundational theories of human resource development (HRD). While the field of HRD recognizes theories from multiple disciplines, the field lacks a theoretical framework to inform leadership in managing racial conflict. In this article literature across multiple disciplines was reviewed to identify research and theory that links racial conflict, racial groups, organizational groups, and performance outcomes. The findings indicate Critical Race Theory (Bell, 1993; Delgado, 1995; Ladson-Billings & Tate, 1995) and Embedded Group Theory (Alderfer & Smith, 1982) provide useful frameworks for addressing inter-group conflict by offering counter discourse through storytelling. This article also suggests a conceptual framework for HRD to begin theory-building research of its own. The purpose of this article is to address the effect of racial conflict on organizational performance as an issue that needs theoretical support in the foundational theories of HRD. This article examines foundational theories of HRD, suggest a conceptual framework for theory- building research addressing racial conflict, racial groups, organizational groups, and performance outcomes, discuss why HRD should be concerned with racial conflict, and review theories and research from other fields that might be useful in addressing the topic. The following research questions are addressed: 1. What foundational theories of HRD inform racial conflict among work groups? 2.
    [Show full text]
  • What Is Racial Domination?
    STATE OF THE ART WHAT IS RACIAL DOMINATION? Matthew Desmond Department of Sociology, University of Wisconsin—Madison Mustafa Emirbayer Department of Sociology, University of Wisconsin—Madison Abstract When students of race and racism seek direction, they can find no single comprehensive source that provides them with basic analytical guidance or that offers insights into the elementary forms of racial classification and domination. We believe the field would benefit greatly from such a source, and we attempt to offer one here. Synchronizing and building upon recent theoretical innovations in the area of race, we lend some conceptual clarification to the nature and dynamics of race and racial domination so that students of the subjects—especially those seeking a general (if economical) introduction to the vast field of race studies—can gain basic insight into how race works as well as effective (and fallacious) ways to think about racial domination. Focusing primarily on the American context, we begin by defining race and unpacking our definition. We then describe how our conception of race must be informed by those of ethnicity and nationhood. Next, we identify five fallacies to avoid when thinking about racism. Finally, we discuss the resilience of racial domination, concentrating on how all actors in a society gripped by racism reproduce the conditions of racial domination, as well as on the benefits and drawbacks of approaches that emphasize intersectionality. Keywords: Race, Race Theory, Racial Domination, Inequality, Intersectionality INTRODUCTION Synchronizing and building upon recent theoretical innovations in the area of race, we lend some conceptual clarification to the nature and dynamics of race and racial domination, providing in a single essay a source through which thinkers—especially those seeking a general ~if economical! introduction to the vast field of race studies— can gain basic insight into how race works as well as effective ways to think about racial domination.
    [Show full text]
  • Ch 12: Aggression
    13 Aggression and conflict What’s it about? (Social Psychology pp. 482–525) Conflict is seen as the perceived incompatibility of goals, where what is wanted by one group may be against the desires of another group. Aggression is defined by social psychologists as a behaviour whose immediate intent is to hurt someone. There are generally two distinct categories of aggression: instrumental aggression and hostile aggression. Group norms often promote aggressive behavior rather than restraining it. Models can reduce aggression, but often also enhance it. Factors that increase the chances of aggression include emotional arousal, alcohol, and time pressure, but similarity reduces aggression. The realistic conflict theory argues that intergroup hostility, conflict, and aggression arise from competition among groups for mastery of scarce but valued material resources. The relative deprivation theory suggests that social comparison, not objective reality, determines how satisfied or dissatisfied people are with what they have. Approaches to reduce aggression and conflict include minimizing or removing aggressive cues; altering perceptions; encouraging cooperation; encouraging careful interpretation and identification with others; trying to find mutually acceptable solutions; or working together toward a shared goal. Negotiation is reciprocal communication designed to reach agreement in situations where some interests are shared, and some are in opposition. Superordinate goals are shared goals that can be attained only if groups work cooperatively
    [Show full text]
  • Methods of Negotiation Research: Introduction
    INER 9.3_f2_340-344 3/8/05 7:58 PM Page 341 International Negotiation 9: 341–344, 2004. 341 © 2004 Koninklijke Brill NV. Printed in the Netherlands. Methods of Negotiation Research: Introduction PETER J. CARNEVALE* Department of Psychology, New York University, 6 Washington Place, Room 577, New York, NY 10003 USA E-mail: [email protected] CARSTEN K.W. DE DREU** Organizational Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Roetersstraat 15, 1018 WB, Amsterdam, The Netherlands E-mail [email protected] Main Entry: method Function: noun 1: a way, plan, or procedure for doing something 2: orderly arrangement from The American Heritage® Dictionary of the English Language, Fourth Edition A close look at the many methodological practices in the study of negotiation reveals a simple fact: there is no one best way, no one best plan, no single orderly arrangement that best produces understanding about negotiation. Indeed, the cornucopia of methods is impressive – as is the strength of the field. There are historical case studies, laboratory experiments, survey studies, archival data analysis, mathematical modeling – the diversity of method is * Peter Carnevale is professor of social and organizational psychology at New York University, moving to NYU in 2001 after many years at the University of Illinois at Urbana- Champaign. He is a past president of the International Association for Conflict Management and past Chair of the Conflict Management Division of the Academy of Management. His research program incorporates multiple methods and techniques – laboratory experiments, surveys of professionals, interviews, archival data analysis, multidimensional scaling, etc., to understand the interplay of cognitive, affective, and motivational processes in negotiation and mediation.
    [Show full text]
  • Nonnative Accents and Conflict Management
    Nonnative Accents and Conflict Management: The Mediating Roles of Stereotype Threat, Regulatory Focus, and Conflict Behaviors on Conflict Outcomes Regina Kim Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy under the Executive Committee of the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences COLUMBIA UNIVERSITY 2017 © 2017 Regina Kim All rights reserved ABSTRACT Nonnative Accents and Conflict Management: The Mediating Roles of Stereotype Threat, Regulatory Focus, and Conflict Behaviors on Conflict Outcomes Regina Kim The proposed study explores the experiences of nonnative speakers when they interact with native speakers in conflict situations. The aim of the study is to test if nonnative speakers experience stereotype threat when interacting with native speakers in conflict situations and, if so, to examine how stereotype threat affects their regulatory focus, conflict behaviors, and outcomes. A serial mediation model with three mediators (stereotype threat, regulatory focus, conflict behaviors) will be tested. This study contributes to the field of organizational psychology and conflict studies by 1) extending stereotype threat literature and examining nonnative speakers as a social identity group that experiences stereotype threat, 2) exploring the effects of stereotype threat in a conflict context, and 3) extending workforce diversity literature and examining language diversity in relation to conflict-related behaviors and outcomes in organizational settings. The findings from the proposed study offer
    [Show full text]
  • The Two Psychologies of Conflict Resolution
    Group Processes & G Intergroup Relations P 1999 Vol 2(2): 99–118 I R The Two Psychologies of Conflict Resolution: Differing Antecedents of Pre-Experience Choices and Post-Experience Evaluations Tom R. Tyler New York University Yuen J. Huo University of California, Los Angeles E. Allan Lind Duke University The literature on the ‘myth of self-interest’ model of perceived human motivation suggests that people believe that both they and others are more motivated by self-interest than is actually the case. Four studies are reported which test one implication of the myth of self-interest: the psychology of pre-experience preferences and post-experience evaluations will differ. We hypothesize that people arrive at pre-experience preferences for decision-making procedures based upon the belief that they want to maximize their self-interest. Further, they will define their self-interest in material terms. Consequently, they choose procedures that they believe promise them the best material outcomes. However, post-experience evaluations are based upon a different factor – the quality of the treatment received during the course of the procedure. The results of all four studies support the suggestion that the psychology of preference and of evaluation differ as predicted. The findings suggest that preference and choice should be viewed as reflecting different psychological processes. keywords choice, satisfaction, self-interest, the relational model Miller and Ratner (1996, 1998) have recently Author’s note suggested that there is a discrepancy between Address correspondence to Tom Tyler, the actual and the assumed power of self-inter- Department of Psychology, New York est in human motivation.
    [Show full text]
  • Revisiting the Form and Function of Conflict: Neurobiological
    Behavioral and Brain Sciences Revisiting the form and function of conflict: Neurobiological, psychological, and cultural cambridge.org/bbs mechanisms for attack and defense within and between groups Target Article Cite this article: De Dreu CKW, Gross J. (2019) Carsten K. W. De Dreu and Jörg Gross Revisiting the form and function of conflict: Neurobiological, psychological, and cultural Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, 2300 RB Leiden, The Netherlands Center for Research in Experimental mechanisms for attack and defense within and Economics and Political Decision Making (CREED), University of Amsterdam, 1018 WB Amsterdam, The between groups. Behavioral and Brain Sciences Netherlands and Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, 2300 RB Leiden, The Netherlands 42, e116: 1–66. doi:10.1017/ [email protected] https://www.universiteitleiden.nl/en/staffmembers/carsten-de-dreu S0140525X18002170 [email protected] http://www.joerg-gross.net Target Article Accepted: 13 September 2018 Target Article Manuscript Online: 25 Abstract September 2018 Conflict can profoundly affect individuals and their groups. Oftentimes, conflict involves a Commentaries Accepted: 11 February 2019 clash between one side seeking change and increased gains through victory and the other Keywords: side defending the status quo and protecting against loss and defeat. However, theory and behavioral game theory; biobehavioral empirical research largely neglected these conflicts between attackers and defenders, and the – approach avoidance; coevolution of strategic, social, and psychological consequences of attack and defense remain poorly under- prosociality and aggression; conflict; conflict intervention; cultural institutions; intergroup stood. To fill this void, we model (1) the clashing of attack and defense as games of strategy relations; psychological adaptations and reveal that (2) attack benefits from mismatching its target’s level of defense, whereas defense benefits from matching the attacker’s competitiveness.
    [Show full text]