Perceived Movement of the Afterimage During Eye Movements'

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Perceived Movement of the Afterimage During Eye Movements' Perceived movement of the afterimage during eye movements' ARlEN MACK2 AND JANET BACHANT NEW SCHOOL FOR SOCIAL RESEARCH The question ofwhether an afterimage viewed in a dark field appear stationary, for there is neither efferent information about appears to move during eye movement lWlS studied by comparing eye movement nor image displacement.3 recordings of eye movements with recordings of reports of It has recently been demonstrated that abnormal image perceived movement. The correlation was found to be quite good displacement coupled with head movement invariably leads to the even under conditions where the eye movements were perception of movement, at least prior to adaptation to this spontaneous rather than specifically directed. The results were unusual state of affairs (posin, 1966; and described in Rock, taken to support the hypothesis that the behavior of the retinal 1966; Wallach & Kravitz, 1965). If the image displaces at a image is "interpreted" by taking into account information different rate or in a different direction than the head, it will concerning what the eyes are doing. appear to move. Only if it displaces at the same rate and in the normal direction wnI it appear stable. This has not, however, been "When the eyes move, the after-image moves in the same way unequivocally demonstrated with abnormal image displacement [Helmholtz, 1924]." Since Helmholtz, if not before, it has accompanying eye movement. Wallach and Lewis (1965) report generally been assumed that an afterimage would appear to move that a mismatch between image displacement and eye movement if the eyes were moving while it was viewed. The general basis of most often does notlead to reports of movement. By a technique this notion is the phenomenon of position constancy: the fact entailing the perception of the shadow cast by the pupil of the that despite movements of the 0 that cause the image to move eye, these investigators achieved a state of affairs in which the over the retina, a stationary scene will continue to look movement of the shadowed disc was controlled by the movement stationary. It is plausible to believe that position constancy is the of the eye, so that it moved only when the eye moved. The result of a process in which image displacement that is concurrent technique made it possible to produce a situation in which the with movement does not lead to the perceived movement of the target disc moved across the retina at either a faster or slower rate objects whose images are displacing, because under these than the rate at which the eye was moving. This experiment led conditions the displacement is taken into account and movement Wallach and Lewis to conclude, "Our results are incompatible is attributed to the 0 himself. Thus, image displacements with the view that the apparent rest of visual objects whose dependent on 0 movement are taken into account so that the images shift due to eye movements is to be explained by a objects in the visual field appear stationary and the 0 experiences compensating process which takes the eye movement into himself or some part of himself to be moving. Now, if it is the account [po 28]." Instead they argue that any shifting of the case that a correspondence between image displacement and 0 image during movements of the eye is simply disregarded or movement is a necessary condition for position constancy, then suppressed. Yarbus(1967), however, reports that abnormal image the absence of such a correspondence should lead to the loss of shifts coupled with eye movements do, in fact, lead to the position constancy and the appearance of movement in the visual appearance of object movement. field. Since the afterimage does not displace, the afterimage "If the direction of the retinal image shift in step with the should appear to move when the 0 moves. saccade was not directly opposite to the direction of One might summarize this view of position constancy by movement of the retina, the visual object also appeared to saying that retinal image displacements are evaluated in terms of be moving jerkily [po 207] ." the available information about 0 movements so that a Yarbus's results, which differ from the Wallach and Lewis correspondence between image displacement and 0 movements findings, appear to be based on the use of specially designed will always lead to position constancy and conversely a lack of contact lenses that enabled him to vary the direction of image correspondence, retinal displacement in a noncorresponding displacement accompanying eye movements. direction or at a noncorresponding rate, or the absence of any Thus, while an investigation into the relationship between eye retinal displacement at all coupled with 0 movement will always movements and the apparent behavior of the afterimage might lead to the appearance of object movement. The perception of an have seemed like an investigation of the obvious, there now afterimage during eye movement is quite analogous to Emmert's appears to be reason to question whether in fact the absence of Law in the case of size perception. If the diminishing size of the image displacement matched with eye movement necessarily must image is taken into account in perceiving size as constant at lead to the perception of movement. If it is true that there increasing distances, then a nonchanging image must be seen as generally is no perception of movement when the magnitude or changingsize with increasing distance. direction of displacement of the retinal image no longer Some qualifications: The evidence now indicates that the corresponds to the magnitude or direction of movements of the information about eye position and eye movement is derived eye, then one might reasonably predict that a condition of from signals corollary to those going to the eye muscles rather nondisplacement of the image accompanying eye movement than from proprioceptive feedback from the eye muscles might also fail to yield the perception of movement. "No (Brindley & Merton, 1960). Thus, where "there is no efferent displacement" is, after all, a special case of abnormal or information that the eyes have been directed to move and there is nonmatching behavior of the image when coupled with an eye image displacement-as in the case of a passively pushed eye movement. On the other hand, ifthe taking-into-account process viewing a normal scene-no transference of the displacement that is effective in maintaining position constancy during head "energy" occurs-and the scene will appear to jump. An movements also operates during eye movements, we should afterimage, however, viewed under the same condition should predict that an afterimage will be seen to move. Perception & Psychophysics, 1969, Vol. 6 (6A) Copyright 1969, Psychonomic Journals, Inc., Austin, Texas 379 It is, of course, also possible to argue that no displacement of in the base of the box along the arc in which the pointer moved the retinal image accompanying eye movement is a special case at the 0-, 45-, 90-, 130-, and 180-deg marks. The 0 was able to and that it is only necessary that there be some unspecified feel these as he moved the pointer. The zero position of the retinal image displacement for position constancy to obtain. On pointer coincided with the O's straight ahead. The arm could be this view the correlations between eye movement and retinal moved through an arc of about 200 deg (I00 deg either side of image movement signifying position constancy would be straight ahead). A telegraph key was placed to the left of the completely nonspecific so that any direction or rate of image pointer on the base of the box. It was wired to one of the two displacement during an eye movement should be accounted for event markers in the recording system. The 0 was instructed to by the eye movement. The difficulty with this view is that it fails depress the key whenever the light (target or image) appeared to to account for the ability to perceive movement during an eye be stationary. movement. A second potentiometer was secured to the top of the box Since a distinction can be made between the perception of a directly above the center of the bite board. From the shaft of this change in visual direction and the perception of movement per se potentiometer a metal arm extended I ft just beneath and parallel (the hour hand of a clock regularly changes its visual direction to the upper surface of the box. At the end of this arm was a without our normally being aware of any movement), and visual I~-V light bulb behind a milk-glass plate bearing a ~·in.-diam direction certainly must be a function of eye position and retinal hole. The housing of the bulb was light-proof so that, while the location, the operation of a taking-into-account process can only light was on, the 0 saw only a small disc of diffuse light be established by reports ofafterimage movement, not simply by insufficiently bright to illuminate any features of the box that reports of shifts in the visual direction of the afterimage, as the had been painted matte black to minimize reflectance. At the top eyes move. of the shaft and outside the box a knob was installed that allowed The experiment to be described is an attempt to determine the E to move the light. This light, which was very close to the whether in fact the afterimage does appear to move when the O's eye level, was visible through an arc of about 200 deg. This eyes move. The experiment is, to our knowledge, the first light was the target light and was used only during the training actually to investigate this matter.
Recommended publications
  • Eye-Movement Studies of Visual Face Perception
    Eye-movement studies of visual face perception Joseph Arizpe A dissertation submitted for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy of the University College London Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience University College London 2015 1 Declaration I, Joseph Arizpe, confirm that the work presented in this thesis is my own. Where information has been derived from other sources, I confirm that this has been indicated in the thesis. 2 Abstract This thesis investigates factors influencing eye-movement patterns during face perception, the relationship of eye-movement patterns to facial recognition performance, and methodological considerations impacting the detection of differences in eye-movement patterns. In particular, in the first study (chapter 2), in which the basis of the other-race effect was investigated, differences in eye- movement patterns during recognition of own- versus other-race (African, Chinese) faces were found for Caucasian participants. However, these eye- movement differences were subtle and analysis-dependent, indicating that the discrepancy in prior reports regarding the presence or absence of such differences are due to variability in statistical sensitivity of analysis methods across studies. The second and third studies (chapters 3 and 4) characterized visuomotor factors, specifically pre-stimulus start position and distance, which strongly influence subsequent eye-movement patterns during face perception. An overall bias in fixation patterns to the opposite side of the face induced by start position and an increasing undershoot of the first ordinal fixation with increasing start distance were found. These visuomotor influences were not specific to faces and did not depend on the predictability of the location of the upcoming stimulus.
    [Show full text]
  • Eye Fields in the Frontal Lobes of Primates
    Brain Research Reviews 32Ž. 2000 413±448 www.elsevier.comrlocaterbres Full-length review Eye fields in the frontal lobes of primates Edward J. Tehovnik ), Marc A. Sommer, I-Han Chou, Warren M. Slocum, Peter H. Schiller Department of Brain and CognitiÕe Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, E25-634, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA Accepted 19 October 1999 Abstract Two eye fields have been identified in the frontal lobes of primates: one is situated dorsomedially within the frontal cortex and will be referred to as the eye field within the dorsomedial frontal cortexŽ. DMFC ; the other resides dorsolaterally within the frontal cortex and is commonly referred to as the frontal eye fieldŽ. FEF . This review documents the similarities and differences between these eye fields. Although the DMFC and FEF are both active during the execution of saccadic and smooth pursuit eye movements, the FEF is more dedicated to these functions. Lesions of DMFC minimally affect the production of most types of saccadic eye movements and have no effect on the execution of smooth pursuit eye movements. In contrast, lesions of the FEF produce deficits in generating saccades to briefly presented targets, in the production of saccades to two or more sequentially presented targets, in the selection of simultaneously presented targets, and in the execution of smooth pursuit eye movements. For the most part, these deficits are prevalent in both monkeys and humans. Single-unit recording experiments have shown that the DMFC contains neurons that mediate both limb and eye movements, whereas the FEF seems to be involved in the execution of eye movements only.
    [Show full text]
  • Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science
    APRIL 1978 Vol. 17/4 Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science A Journal of Clinical and Basic Research Articles Training of voluntary torsion Richard Balliet and Ken Nakayama By means of a visual feedback technique, human subjects were trained to make large conjugate cyclorotary eye movements at will. The range of movement increased with training at a rate of approximately 0.8° per hour of practice, reaching 30° at the end of training. Photographs recorded the ability to make voluntary cyclofixations at any amplitude within the subject's range. Cyclotorsional pursuit was also trained, with ability increasing with greater amounts of visual feedback. In addition, torsional saccadic tracking was trained, showing a magnitude vs. peak velocity relationship similar to that seen for normal saccades. Control experiments indi- cate that all of these movements were voluntary, with no significant visual induction. With extended practice, large torsional movements could be made without any visual stimulation. The emergence of voluntary torsion through training demonstrates that the oculomotor system has more plasticity than has generally been assumed, reopening the issue as to whether other movements could also be trained to alleviate the symptoms of strabismus. Key words: eye movements, torsion, saccades, slow pursuit, fixation, orthoptics, oculomotor plasticity c' yclorotations are defined as rotations the eye can undergo a cyclorotation as it about the visual axis of the eye. These rota- moves from one tertiary position of gaze to tions are considered to be reflexive, with no another, but the amount of this cyclorotation indication of voluntary control. For example, is fixed, being dictated by Listings law.11 In addition, involuntary cyclovergence has been 1 From the Smith-Kettlewell Institute of Visual Science, reported to occur during convergence, and Department of Visual Sciences, University of the reflexive cycloversions have been demon- Pacific, San Francisco, Calif.
    [Show full text]
  • The Effect of Eye Movements and Blinks on Afterimage Appearance and Duration
    Journal of Vision (2015) 15(3):20, 1–15 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/15/3/20 1 The effect of eye movements and blinks on afterimage appearance and duration School of Psychology, Cardiff University, # Georgina Powell Cardiff, Wales, UK $ School of Psychology, Cardiff University, # Petroc Sumner Cardiff, Wales, UK $ Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, # Aline Bompas Centre Hospitalier Vinatier, Bron, Cedex, France $ The question of whether eye movements influence argued that afterimage signals are inherently ambigu- afterimage perception has been asked since the 18th ous, and this could explain why their visibility is century, and yet there is surprisingly little consensus on influenced by cues, such as surrounding luminance how robust these effects are and why they occur. The edges, more than are real stimuli (Powell, Bompas, & number of historical theories aiming to explain the Sumner, 2012). Helmholtz (1962) identified another cue effects are more numerous than clear experimental that is important to take into account when conducting demonstrations of such effects. We provide a clearer afterimage experiments: characterization of when eye movements and blinks do or do not affect afterimages with the aim to distinguish For obtaining really beautiful positive after- between historical theories and integrate them with a images, the following additional rules should be modern understanding of perception. We found neither saccades nor pursuit reduced strong afterimage observed. Both before and after they are devel- duration, and blinks actually increased afterimage oped, any movement of the eye or any sudden duration when tested in the light. However, for weak movement of the body must be carefully avoided, afterimages, we found saccades reduced duration, and because under such circumstances they invariably blinks and pursuit eye movements did not.
    [Show full text]
  • Cine Magnetic Resonance Imaging of Eye Movements
    CINE MAGNETIC RESONANCE IMAGING OF EYE MOVEMENTS C. c. BAILEY\ J. KABALA2, R. LAITT2, M. WESTON2, P. GODDARD2, H. B. HOHl, M. J. POTTSl and R. A. HARRADl Bristol SUMMARY Images were obtained using the FISP 2D sequence (fast Cine magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) is a technique in imaging with a steady state progression). The patient was which multiple sequential static orbital MRI films are positioned within the head coil in the magnet. A strip of taken while the patient fixates a series of targets across paper with six separate marks set 30° apart was suspended the visual field. These are then sequenced to give a in front of the patient and aligned either transversely or graphic animation to the eyes. The excellent soft tissue sagittally depending on the eye movement of interest. The differentiationofMRI, combined with the dynamic imag­ patient was asked to look sequentially at each mark on the ing, allows rapid visualisation, and functional assessment paper, and a scan was performed in each position. Each of the extraocular muscles. Good assessment of contrac­ image took 15 seconds to acquire. After the patient had tility can be obtained, but the technique does not allow fixated all six points the procedure was repeated in reverse study of saccadic or pursuit eye movements. We have order. The images generated were then displayed as a used this technique in 36 patients with a range of ocular continuous video loop. The total time required to generate motility disorders, including thyroid-related ophthalmo­ a video loop for each patient was only 6 minutes.
    [Show full text]
  • Pseudogaze in Afterimages
    Journal of Vision (2016) 16(5):6, 1–10 1 Where are you looking? Pseudogaze in afterimages Caltech Brain Imaging Center, Division of Humanities and Social Sciences, California Institute of Technology, # Daw-An Wu Pasadena, CA, USA $ Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH, USA Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, Universite´ # Patrick Cavanagh Paris Descartes, Paris, France $ How do we know where we are looking? A frequent gaze seems exceptionally well defined and we seldom assumption is that the subjective experience of our feel that we don’t know where we are looking within a direction of gaze is assigned to the location in the world visual scene. The conceptual equivalence of mental and that falls on our fovea. However, we find that observers physical gaze is ingrained to the point that the two can shift their subjective direction of gaze among concepts share almost all their terminology, even in different nonfoveal points in an afterimage. Observers scientific and technical usage. Terms such as ‘‘gaze,’’ were asked to look directly at different corners of a ‘‘look at,’’ ‘‘fixate,’’ etc. do not distinguish between the diamond-shaped afterimage. When the requested corner subjective sense of visual targeting and the physical was 3.58 in the periphery, the observer often reported pointing of the eyes at something. that the image moved away in the direction of the Whereas a large body of work exists regarding the attempted gaze shift. However, when the corner was at perceived direction of visual targets in egocentric space 1.758 eccentricity, most reported successfully fixating at the point.
    [Show full text]
  • Types of Eye Movements
    Types of Eye Movements The information given here is deliberately non-technical and is based for a general Background audience. With apologies to eye movement research colleagues for liberties taken in Objective simplification. Challenges Visual Search & Paintings In our everyday lives we regularly make various types of eye movements, usually without The Exhibit being aware of them. Such movements occur as a result of the activity of the three pairs of Updates antagonistic muscles that support each eye. There are several ways to record eye Press Reports movements and more information can be found here. Links FAQ In looking at paintings, or other two-dimensional scenes, only saccadic eye movements and their associated fixations, in the main, are of research interest. Miniature eye People movements also occur, and these overlay the fixations causing many small movements which, to a large extent, can be regarded as ‘system noise’ (unless the researcher is Home specifically interested in these movements). Main types of eye movements:- [this list of links all link to subsequent sections on this page] Saccade Miniature Pursuit Smooth Compensatory Vergence Nystagmus Saccade A saccade is a rapid eye movement (a jump) which is usually conjugate (i.e. both eyes move together in the same direction) and under voluntary control. Broadly speaking the purpose of these movements is to bring images of particular areas of the visual world to fall onto the fovea. Saccades are therefore a major instrument of selective visual attention. It is often convenient (but somewhat inaccurate) to consider both that a saccadic eye movement always occurs in a straight line and also that we do not ‘see’ during these movements.
    [Show full text]
  • Optical Illusion Art As Radical Interface
    Perceptual Play: Optical Illusion Art as Radical Interface Julian Oliver, 2008 In 1992 Joachim Sauter and Dirk Lüsebrink presented a work called Zerseher ('Iconoclast') at Ars Electronica, where they were awarded a prize in the Interactive Art category. In this work, audiences were invited to destroy a (digital) copy of an antique painting just by looking at it. Zerseher provides a context for direct manipulation of the visible work. The position and orientation of the viewer's eyes are tracked using a camera which, in turn, directs a digital brush over the surface of the image, smearing pixels around. It's not hard to see why the piece was awarded; here the premise explored in philosophy and poetry, that the gaze might have a mutating or productive power of its own, was explicitly manifested: the mere act of looking had the power to alter (or destroy) the artefact itself. Historically speaking, the idea that the gaze might be a primally destabilising force may have arisen from the fact that seeing itself is not always reliable; that the act of seeing – and perceiving what is seen – is something to be suspicious of. Many works of philosophy and poetry have asserted the primary fragility of perception, particularly as related to the ocular sense. This can be traced back as far as Plato in Western thought, who was early in identifying that what is seen propagates as an object of thought – a mental image – separated from the world and that it is there that interpretation occurs: "The image stands at the junction of a light which comes from the object and another which comes from the gaze".1 In recent times, the study of perception2 has been as active in scienctific thought as it has in philosophy.
    [Show full text]
  • A Robust Method of Eye Torsion Measurement for Medical Applications Nan Jin, Sébastien Mavromatis, Jean Sequeira, Stéphane Curcio
    A Robust Method of Eye Torsion Measurement for Medical Applications Nan Jin, Sébastien Mavromatis, Jean Sequeira, Stéphane Curcio To cite this version: Nan Jin, Sébastien Mavromatis, Jean Sequeira, Stéphane Curcio. A Robust Method of Eye Torsion Measurement for Medical Applications. Information, MDPI, 2020, 11 (9), pp.408. 10.3390/info11090408. hal-02919784 HAL Id: hal-02919784 https://hal-amu.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-02919784 Submitted on 24 Aug 2020 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Distributed under a Creative Commons Attribution| 4.0 International License information Article A Robust Method of Eye Torsion Measurement for Medical Applications Nan Jin 1,2,* ,Sébastien Mavromatis 1 , Jean Sequeira 1 and Stéphane Curcio 2 1 Aix Marseille Univ, Université de Toulon, CNRS, LIS, 13000 Marseille, France; [email protected] (S.M.); [email protected] (J.S.) 2 SYNAPSYS, 13013 Marseille, France; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +33-4-9111-7682 Received: 8 July 2020; Accepted: 19 August 2020; Published: 21 August 2020 Abstract: The detection of eye torsion is an important element for diagnosis of balance disorders, although it is rarely available in existing eye tracking systems.
    [Show full text]
  • Cortical Regions Involved in Eye Movements, Shifts of Attention, and Gaze Perception
    ᭜ Human Brain Mapping 25:140–154(2005) ᭜ Cortical Regions Involved in Eye Movements, Shifts of Attention, and Gaze Perception Marie-He´le`ne Grosbras,1* Angela R. Laird,2 and Toma´s Paus1,3 1Cognitive Neuroscience Unit, Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada 2Research Imaging Center, University of Texas Health Science Center, San Antonio, Texas 3Brain and Body Center, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, United Kingdom ᭜ ᭜ Abstract: Human vision is an active process that involves shifting attention across the visual scene, with or without moving the eyes. Such shifts of attention can be generated at will (endogenously) or be triggered automatically, i.e., generated in response to exogenous stimuli including socially relevant cues such as someone else’s gaze. What are the common and distinct brain mechanisms involved in these processes? To address this question, we carried out a quantitative effect-location meta-analysis of 59 brain-imaging experiments whose results were published using standardized coordinates. For each condition of interest, namely voluntary and visually triggered eye movements, voluntary and visually triggered (covert) shifts of attention, and perception of someone else’s gaze, we computed activation likelihood estimation (ALE) maps. Those maps represent at each voxel of the brain the probability of reporting a signal change related to the condition of interest. For eye movements, this analysis confirmed the spatial location of the frontal eye fields, supplementary eye fields, and parietal saccade-related regions. The map of covert shifts of attention demonstrated highest similarity with the map of saccadic eye movements. Gaze perception showed common activation likelihood with the other conditions in the right intraparietal sulcus and in the lateral precentral gyrus.
    [Show full text]
  • Exploring the Human Visual Perception Process to Foster Mindful Seeing (Finding the Angels and Devils in the Details)
    Exploring the Human Visual Perception Process to Foster Mindful Seeing (Finding the Angels and Devils in the Details) by David T. E. Foster A thesis exhibition presented to OCAD University in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER of DESIGN (MDes) in DIGITAL FUTURES. Location: Toronto Media Arts Centre (TMAC), April 12 – 14, 2019 32 Lisgar St., Toronto, Ontario, Canada © David T. E. Foster - 2019 AUTHOR’S DECLARATION I hereby declare that I am the sole author of this thesis. This is a true copy of the thesis, including any required final revisions, as accepted by my examiners. I authorize OCAD University to lend this thesis to other institutions or individuals for the purpose of scholarly research. I understand that my thesis may be made electronically available to the public. I further authorize OCAD University to reproduce this thesis by photocopying or by other means, in total or in part, at the request of other institutions or individuals for the purpose of scholarly research. Signed: _____________________________, Date: _________________ ii ABSTRACT David T. E. Foster ‘Exploring the Human Visual Perception Process to Foster Mindful Seeing (Finding the Angels and Devils in the Details)’ Master of Design, Digital Futures OCAD University, 2019 Using the principles of Iterative and Human Centred Design as methodologies, gaze-tracking equipment and techniques and analysis of writings in such areas as medical, neurological and psychological studies in the visual perception process, the principles of mindfulness and mindful meditation, Trompe L’oeil painting and camouflage, this project will observe the saccade/fixation patterns of study participants viewing multiple different photographic images in an attempt to build a database of saccade/fixation patterns.
    [Show full text]
  • Eye and Mind*
    Eye and Mind* Maurice Merleau-Ponty What I am trying to convey to you is more mysterious; it is entwined in the very roots of being, in the impalpable source of sensations. J. Gasquet, Cézanne Science manipulates things and gives up living in them.1 Operating within its own realm, it makes its constructs of things; operating upon these indices or variables to effect whatever transformations are permitted by their definition, it comes face to face with the real world only at rare intervals. It is, and always has been, that admirably active, ingenious, and bold way of thinking whose fundamental bias is to treat everything as though it were an object-in-general—as though it meant nothing to us and yet was predestined for our ingenious schemes. But classical science clung to a feeling for the opaqueness of the world, and it expected through its constructions to get back into the world. For this reason it felt obliged to seek a transcendent or transcendental foundation for its operations. Today we find—not in science but in a widely prevalent philosophy of the sciences—an entirely new approach. Constructive scientific activities see themselves and represent themselves to be autonomous, and their thinking deliberately reduces itself to a set of data-collecting techniques which it has invented. To think is thus to test out, to operate, to transform—the only restriction being that this activity is regulated by an experimental control that admits only the most "worked-up" phenomena, more likely produced by the apparatus than recorded by it. Whence all sorts of vagabond endeavors.
    [Show full text]