Do Cross-Cultural Psychological Differences Vary with Social Class, Age, and Gender?

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Do Cross-Cultural Psychological Differences Vary with Social Class, Age, and Gender? Do Cross-Cultural Psychological Differences Vary with Social Class, Age, and Gender? A Japan-U.S. Comparison of Cognitive Style and Social Orientation Shinobu Kitayama1 Mayumi Karasawa2 Igor Grossmann3 Jinkyung Na4 Michael E. W. Varnum5 Richard E. Nisbett1 1: University of Michigan; 2: Tokyo Woman’s Christian University; 3: University of Waterloo; 4: Sogang University; 5: Arizona State University Paper (excluding tables, figures, and references): 4343 words Research reported here was supported by the Russell Sage Foundation to R.E.N. and S.K., the International Max Plank Research LIFE Fellowship awarded to I.G., National Institute on Aging Grant 5RO129509-02, and National Science Foundation (NSF) Grant 2007: BCS 0717982 to R.E.N. and S.K. The views presented here are not necessarily those of the NSF. Address correspondence to S.K. ([email protected]), University of Michigan, Department of Psychology, 530 Church Street, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-1109. 1 Abstract Previous work assessing psychological tendencies across cultures suggests that East Asians are more holistic in cognition (e.g., extending attention to the context in object perception) and interdependent in social orientation (e.g., feeling happy when connected with others) than European Americans. However, little is known about the possible moderation of these findings by age and social class because of this literature’s near-exclusive focus on young and highly educated segments of the respective populations. The neglect of these demographic variables has persisted even though different theories predict divergent effects of such variables. Here, we addressed this gap by testing a comprehensive set of eleven holistic cognition measures and six interdependent social orientation measures in random samples of Japanese and Americans who varied in age, educational attainment, and gender (N = 666). The previously reported cultural variation proved highly stable across the subpopulations. Though other demographic factors played a significant role for both cognitive style and social orientation, their combined contribution was 14-18 times smaller than the effects of national culture. We discuss implications for the existing theories of culture. Key words: culture, cognitive style, social orientation, Japan, United States 2 Do Cross-Cultural Psychological Differences Vary with Social Class, Age, and Gender? A Japan-U.S. Comparison of Cognitive Style and Social Orientation Much of the work on culture in psychology has compared East Asians, including Japanese, Koreans, and Chinese, among others, and European Americans(Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Nisbett et al., 2001). This work has focused on habitual psychological tendencies, such as attending context vs. object(Kitayama et al., 2003; Masuda & Nisbett, 2001; Morris & Peng, 1994) and feeling happy when achieving social (rather than personal) goals(Kitayama et al., 2006; Mesquita, 2001). Existing evidence suggests that East Asians are relatively more holistic in cognition and more interdependent in social orientation than European Americans(Kitayama & Uskul, 2011). However, this literature nearly exclusively focused on college students, who are typically young, mostly well-off, and highly educated. It is therefore not clear whether the findings might depend on social class and age, even though different theoretical positions predict such moderations. Here, we report an extensive empirical study of seventeen psychological tasks assessing cognitive style and social orientation in European Americans and Japanese who varied in age, educational attainment, and gender. Cognitive style refers to the habitual tendency to process information either (i) holistically by incorporating context and using intuitive (e.g., similarity-based) inferences or (ii) more analytically by focusing on central objects and employing logical rules(Nisbett et al., 2001). By social orientation, we mean the habitual tendency to seek social interdependence, social embeddedness, and/or social harmony by often downplaying self-interests and personal goals(Markus & Kitayama, 1991). Prior work has shown that compared to European Americans, East Asians are both holistic in cognition and interdependent in social orientation(Kitayama & Uskul, 2011; Miyamoto, 2013), consistent with prior work showing that compared to European 3 Americans, East Asians are less individualistic or more collectivistic(Hofstede, 1984; Triandis, 1995). Although extensive, this work leaves behind one crucial question: How might the cultural variation be moderated by social class and age? Nevertheless, different lines of work suggested that the cultural variation in psychological tendencies may depend on social class and age. According to one time-honored tradition of thought (Bush, 2014; Foucault, 1977), higher status individuals first develop cultural ideologies and associated practices to effectively govern the respective regions. For example, in Western European history, the Roman Christian Church may have played a crucial role in transforming traditional, kin-based social systems into more individualistic ones by discouraging kin-based organizations by a series of decrees (Schulz et al., 2019). Likewise, the value of social harmony, pervasive in East Asia, may have initially been adopted tactically by the landowners to encourage conformity to local norms to increase the production of a crop available, rice, which requires massive coordination of human labor(Talhelm et al., 2014). These socio-ecological and political considerations would suggest we might expect high-status individuals to show stronger evidence of predominant cultural mentalities. Scholars have also investigated culture within a broad scope of life-span development. In cognitive domains, for example, earlier theorizing distinguished between cognitive mechanics (evolution-based hard wares of the mind) and cognitive pragmatics (culture-based softwares of the mind(Baltes, 1993)). Based on this distinction, scholars have suggested that culture can influence cognitive mechanics at least when one is relatively young and the hard wares of the mind are supposedly still plastic. However, this cultural influence may diminish after early adulthood when plasticity of the brain hard wares wanes. Further, it may be overwhelmed by cognitive decline after that (D. C. Park et al., 1999). Hence, cultural variations in cognition may decrease with age in the domains of cognitive mechanics. In contrast, cognitive pragmatics is 4 based on learning of culture, which may accumulate over time. Hence, cultural variations in cognition may increase in the domains of cognitive pragmatics. To address these divergent theoretical possibilities, we tested samples of age- heterogeneous Japanese and Americans, with varying levels of educational achievement. We focused on the potential moderation of the cultural differences by gender by recruiting roughly equal numbers of males and females within each subgroup. They were administered a set of 17 tasks, 11 of which are designed to assess the degree of holistic cognition and the remaining six of which are designed to measure the degree of interdependent social orientation. See Supplementary Information for additional tasks included in the surveys conducted in each of the two countries. This effort enabled us to test whether both holistic cognitive style and interdependent social orientation might vary with social class, age, and gender. Based on prior work, we expected that lower social class(Igor Grossmann & Varnum, 2011), older age (Carstensen et al., 1999), and female gender (Cross et al., 2000) would be associated with relatively more holistic cognition and more interdependent social orientation. Last, but not least, prior cross-cultural work typically reported only a small number of tasks, often only one of them, at a time to study the cultural variation. This practice is a major limitation if one is to test whether the cultural variation can generalize beyond the unique task(s) assessed in a given study. We sought to overcome this limitation by treating the tasks as random factors(Judd et al., 2017). Methods Participants The current work was conducted in 2006-2009. The research protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Michigan (HUM00003112). We recruited age- stratified random community samples of Japanese in the Tokyo Metropolitan area and Americans in Washtenaw county, Michigan. In Japan we randomly selected names from a municipal registry. In the U.S. we randomly sampled names from a telephone directory. This 5 research was conducted in the period 2006-2009, a time at which a vast majority of individuals in Washtenaw County had landlines. As indicated in Table 1, we attempted to include an approximately equal number of participants of both sexes, and of each of three age groups (25- 39, 40-59, 60-79), as well as an adequate number of adults from lower socio-economic strata. We approximated social class by level of education, which has been previously proposed as the key factor that distinguishes different classes . In Japan, a survey company contacted participants first, with several questionnaires included in the mail survey. Participants Table 1 Demographics of the current samples in Japan and the United States. Age Education No Some Post Range Mean Colleg College college Grad e Female (84) 25-40 33.68 17 37 27 3 Young Male (93) 25-40 32.78 22 15 47 9 Middl Female (82) 41-56 47.82 18 33 31 JPN e Male (68) 41-55 48.63 14 5 45 4 Female (49) 60-75 66.59 22 18 9 0 Old
Recommended publications
  • Cultural Distance, Perception of Emotional Display Rules, And
    CULTURAL DISTANCE, PERCEPTION OF EMOTIONAL DISPLAY RULES, AND THEIR INFLUENCE ON SOJOURNER ADJUSTMENT A thesis presented to the faculty of the College of Arts and Sciences of Ohio University In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree Master of Science Nicole L. Gullekson August 2007 2 This thesis titled CULTURAL DISTANCE, PERCEPTION OF EMOTIONAL DISPLAY RULES, AND THEIR INFLUENCE ON SOJOURNER ADJUSTMENT by NICOLE L. GULLEKSON has been approved for the Department of Psychology and the College of Arts and Sciences by ________________________________________________ Jeffrey B. Vancouver Associate Professor of Psychology ________________________________________________ Benjamin M. Ogles Dean, College of Arts and Sciences 3 Abstract GULLEKSON, NICOLE L., M.S., August 2007, Experimental Psychology CULTURAL DISTANCE, PERCEPTION OF EMOTIONAL DISPLAY RULES, AND THEIR INFLUENCE ON SOJOURNER ADJUSTMENT (109 pp.) With increased globalization, more individuals temporarily leave home to work and study in foreign countries. These sojourners are confronted with societal norms different from their home cultures. The present study investigated the extent to which international student sojourners perceive differences in emotional display norms between their home and host cultures, as well as the influence of such perception on adjustment. Although accurate perception of the host culture’s emotional display rules was not related to adjustment, a “guest” effect existed. Specifically, international student participants reported that one should
    [Show full text]
  • Buddhism and Holistic Versus Analytic Thought
    CULTURE, RELIGION AND COGNITION: BUDDHISM AND HOLISTIC VERSUS ANALYTIC THOUGHT by Alain Samson A thesis submitted for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy (PhD) 2007 University of London The London School of Economics and Political Science Institute of Social Psychology UMI Number: U615882 All rights reserved INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. Dissertation Publishing UMI U615882 Published by ProQuest LLC 2014. Copyright in the Dissertation held by the Author. Microform Edition © ProQuest LLC. All rights reserved. This work is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code. ProQuest LLC 789 East Eisenhower Parkway P.O. Box 1346 Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346 Declaration I certify that the thesis I have presented for examination for the MPhil/PhD degree of the London School of Economics and Political Science is solely my own work other than where I have clearly indicated that it is the work of others (in which case the extent of any work carried out jointly by me and any other person is clearly identified in it). The copyright of this thesis rests with the author. Quotation from it is permitted, provided that full acknowledgement is made. This thesis may not be reproduced without the prior written consent of the author. I warrant that this authorization does not, to the best of my belief, infringe the rights of 2 British ity<;(.
    [Show full text]
  • Cultural Neuroscience: a Historical Introduction and Overview Nicholas O
    Unit 9 Biological Psychology, Neuropsychology and Culture Article 1 Subunit 2 Neuropsychology and Culture 8-1-2014 Cultural Neuroscience: A Historical Introduction and Overview Nicholas O. Rule University of Toronto, [email protected] Please address correspondence to: Nicholas O. Rule, Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, [email protected]; this work was supported in part by NSERC 491593. Recommended Citation Rule, N. O. (2014). Cultural Neuroscience: A Historical Introduction and Overview. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture, 9(2). https://doi.org/10.9707/2307-0919.1128 This Online Readings in Psychology and Culture Article is brought to you for free and open access (provided uses are educational in nature)by IACCP and ScholarWorks@GVSU. Copyright © 2014 International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology. All Rights Reserved. ISBN 978-0-9845627-0-1 Cultural Neuroscience: A Historical Introduction and Overview Abstract The integration of cognitive neuroscience with the study of culture emerged from independent ascensions among both fields in the early 1990s. This marriage of the two previously unconnected areas of inquiry has generated a variety of empirical and theoretical works that have provided unique insights to both partners that might have otherwise gone overlooked. Here, I provide a brief historical introduction to the emergence of cultural neuroscience from its roots in cultural psychology and cognitive neuroscience to its present stature as one of the most challenging but rewarding sub-disciplines to have come from the burgeoning growth of the study of the brain and behavior. In doing so, I overview some of the more studied areas within cultural neuroscience: language, music, mathematics, visual perception, and social cognition.
    [Show full text]
  • Migration, Acculturation, and the Maintenance of Between-Group Cultural Variation’
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/234807; this version posted June 13, 2018. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license. 1 Migration, acculturation, and the maintenance of 2 between-group cultural variation ∗ 3 Alex Mesoudi 4 12 June, 2018 5 Abstract 6 How do migration and acculturation (i.e. psychological or behavioral change resulting from migration) 7 affect within- and between-group cultural variation? Here I answer this question by drawing analogies 8 between genetic and cultural evolution. Population genetic models show that migration rapidly 9 breaks down between-group genetic structure. In cultural evolution, however, migrants or their 10 descendants can acculturate to local behaviors via social learning processes such as conformity, 11 potentially preventing migration from eliminating between-group cultural variation. An analysis 12 of the empirical literature on migration suggests that acculturation is common, with second and 13 subsequent migrant generations shifting, sometimes substantially, towards the cultural values of the 14 adopted society. Yet there is little understanding of the individual-level dynamics that underlie these 15 population-level shifts. To explore this formally, I present models quantifying the effect of migration 16 and acculturation on between-group cultural variation, for both neutral and costly cooperative 17 traits. In the models, between-group cultural variation, measured using F statistics, is eliminated by 18 migration and maintained by conformist acculturation. The extent of acculturation is determined 19 by the strength of conformist bias and the number of demonstrators from whom individuals learn.
    [Show full text]
  • An Introduction to Cultural Neuroscience Lyndac.Linandevah.Telzer
    An Introduction to Cultural Neuroscience LyndaC.LinandEvaH.Telzer This chapter’s goal is to serve as a brief introduction to the emerging feld of cultural neuroscience. It does not intend to provide an extensive review but rather a brief overview of the feld. We start by defning cultural neu- roscience and follow by providing important reasons for doing research in this feld. Next we explain some key terms and methodologies. We then highlight important studies in the feld and provide recommendations for designing a study. Finally, we make suggestions for future directions. Throughout this chapter, we choose a few specifcexamplestoillustrate ways in which cultural neuroscience researchers have utilized the concepts and tools described here. What Is Cultural Neuroscience? Cultural neuroscience is an emerging interdisciplinary feld that combines theories and methods from cultural and social psychology, anthropology, and social and cognitive neuroscience to investigate the interactions between culture, psychological processes, brain, and genes at diferent timescales (for reviews, see Chiao, Cheon, Pornpattananangkul, Mrazek, & Blizinsky, 2013; Han et al., 2013; Kim & Sasaki, 2014). Because of the broad interdisciplinary nature of the feld, describing all of the diferent ways in which cultural neuroscientists do research in this area is beyond the scope of this chapter. Here we will be focusing on the most common method, which is representations in the brain through neuroimaging techniques. Because this feld lies at the intersection of many areas of study, we begin by describing how cultural neuroscience derives from each of these The Handbook of Culture and Biology,FirstEdition.EditedbyJose´ M. Causadias, Eva H. Telzer and Nancy A.
    [Show full text]
  • Society and Culture
    03 CHAPTER Society and Culture America is the only culture that went from barbarism to decadence without civilization in between. —Oscar Wilde LEARNING OUTCOMES The request “May I take your order, please?” is familiar to everyone in the world today. Obviously you are in a McDonald’s, Burger King, 01 Define society and culture and Starbucks, or some other fast-food restaurant, where global corpo- give examples of different types of rations have programmed worker greetings, worker routines, and societies. even worker attitudes to a standardized formula. You can be assured that the menu, the food, and the service will be the same no matter 02 List and give examples of the major where the particular restaurant is located, but things are not what they components of culture. seem. Many people believe that McDonald’s, for instance, is such a potent source of American fast-food culture and American culture in 03 Define and give examples of general that when it transplants itself overseas, “American values” ethnocentrism and cultural are widely disseminated. In effect, this thesis holds that no matter relativism. the cultural setting, the arrival of the Golden Arches inevitably leads 04 Explain globalization and cultural to cultural homogenization on the American model, a trend that is diversity. supposedly sweeping the globe (Ritzer, 2018). Once again, however, things may not be what they seem. Although McDonald’ s relies on 05 Discuss society and culture from standardization and emphasizes that a person always knows what each of the major theoretical to expect from McDonald’s anywhere in the world, local franchises perspectives in sociology.
    [Show full text]
  • Cultural Diversity and Conformity
    Cultural Diversity and Conformity Chapter 2: Cultural Diversity and Conformity Case Study: Body Ritual Among the Nacirema Section 1: The Meaning of Culture Section 2: Cultural Variation Section 3: The American Value System Lab: Applying What You’ve Learned Original Content Copyright © Holt McDougal. Additions and changes to the original content are the responsibility of the instructor. Cultural Diversity and Conformity Case Study: Body Ritual Among the Nacirema Ceremonies and rituals are an important part of any culture. In the 1950s, Horace Miner examined some of the rituals of the Nacirema culture. His description of the culture included a portrait of an average Nacirema and his or her daily body ritual. Although the language Miner used made the culture seem exotic and strange, the description was a truthful representation of the American (Nacirema spelled backwards) morning ritual. Original Content Copyright © Holt McDougal. Additions and changes to the original content are the responsibility of the instructor. Cultural Diversity and Conformity Section 1 at a Glance The Meaning of Culture • Culture is made up of the material and nonmaterial products of human groups. • A society is a group of interdependent people who share a common culture and feeling of unity. Society differs from culture in that societies are made up of people and cultures are made up of products. • All cultures share certain elements: technology, symbols, language, values, and norms. Original Content Copyright © Holt McDougal. Additions and changes to the original content are the responsibility of the instructor. Cultural Diversity and Conformity How did the car influence American culture? Original Content Copyright © Holt McDougal.
    [Show full text]
  • Universals and Cultural Variations in Emotional Expression by Daniel Thomas Cordaro a Dissertation Submitted in Partial Satisfac
    Universals and Cultural Variations in Emotional Expression By Daniel Thomas Cordaro A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Psychology in the Graduate Division of the University of California, Berkeley Committee in charge: Professor Dacher Keltner, Chair Professor Iris Mauss Professor Cameron Anderson Spring 2014 1 Abstract Universals and Cultural Variations in Emotional Expression by Daniel Thomas Cordaro Doctor of Philosophy in Psychology University of California, Berkeley Professor Dacher Keltner, Chair One of the most fascinating characteristics of emotions is that they have universal expressive patterns. These expressions, which are encoded through multiple bodily channels, allow us to identify distinct emotions in ourselves and others. After groundbreaking theorizing by Charles Darwin and early empirical work by Ekman and Izard, further research in emotion science revealed evidence in single cultures for more emotional expressions above and beyond the well- studied set comprising of anger, contempt, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness, and surprise. Some of these new emotions were displayed using nonfacial modalities, such as the voice, touch, and posture. Following the logic set forth by these methods and findings, we tested two hypotheses: 1) there are more than seven universal expressions of emotion; 2) expressive universality is not limited to facial expression. Findings from three cross-cultural experimental studies yielded support to the above hypotheses. Our first study collected 5500 expressions of twenty-one emotions from five different cultures. Spontaneous behavioral analysis revealed core facial and bodily patterns for each of the 21 states across all five cultures. Additionally, we uncovered over 100 cultural variations to the universal patterns and hundreds of individual variations.
    [Show full text]
  • Cultural Diversity, Geographical Isolation, and the Origin of The
    Cultural Diversity, Geographical Isolation, and the Origin of the Wealth of Nations Quamrul Ashrafy Oded Galorz November 2011 Abstract This research argues that variations in the interplay between cultural assimilation and cultural di¤usion have played a signi…cant role in giving rise to di¤erential patterns of economic devel- opment across the globe. Societies that were geographically less vulnerable to cultural di¤usion bene…ted from enhanced assimilation, lower cultural diversity, and more intense accumulation of society-speci…c human capital. Thus, they operated more e¢ ciently with respect to their production-possibility frontiers and ‡ourished in the technological paradigm that characterized the agricultural stage of development. The lack of cultural di¤usion and its manifestation in cultural rigidity, however, diminished the ability of these societies to adapt to a new technological paradigm, which delayed their industrialization and, hence, their take-o¤ to a state of sustained economic growth. The theory thus contributes to the understanding of the advent of divergence and overtaking in the process of development. Consistently with the theory, the empirical analysis establishes that (i) geographical isolation prevalent in pre-industrial times (i.e., prior to the advent of airborne transportation technology) has had a persistent negative impact on the extent of contemporary cultural diversity; (ii) pre-industrial geographical isolation had a positive impact on economic development in the agricultural stage but has had a negative impact on income per capita in the course of industrialization; and (iii) cultural diversity, as determined exogenously by pre-industrial geographical isolation, has had a positive impact on economic development in the process of industrialization.
    [Show full text]
  • Menopause As a Social and Cultural Construction Bre'on Kelly
    XULAneXUS Volume 8 | Issue 2 Article 4 4-1-2011 Menopause as a Social and Cultural Construction Bre'on Kelly Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.xula.edu/xulanexus Recommended Citation Kelly, Bre'on (2011) "Menopause as a Social and Cultural Construction," XULAneXUS: Vol. 8 : Iss. 2 , Article 4. Available at: https://digitalcommons.xula.edu/xulanexus/vol8/iss2/4 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by XULA Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in XULAneXUS by an authorized editor of XULA Digital Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Kelly: Menopause as a Social and Cultural Construction ! ! Volume 8, Issue 2, April 2011. Scholarly Note. 29-39. <http://xulanexus.xula.edu/textpattern/index.php?id=114> ! Menopause as a Social and Cultural Construction Bre’on Andrice Kelly, Sociology Faculty Mentor: Dr. Christopher Faircloth Abstract When the word “menopause” is mentioned in this modern age, too often a picture appears of an unstable, mature woman, fragmenting her family with her emotional instability. This essay explores this image as a social and cultural construction. Drawing initially from the medicalization model exposed in sociology, I discuss the social construction of menopause and the related medical versus feminist Bre’on Kelly is a Sociology debate. Lastly, using studies of menopause from various cultures, major with a minor in this literature review explores how women of different cultures Psychology from New Orleans, define, give meaning to, and experience menopause, as well as the LA. Upon graduating from personal, societal, and cultural implications of these.
    [Show full text]
  • Culture and Culture Change
    M02_EMBER1208_13_SE_C02.QXD 10/21/09 9:05 PM Page 14 Culture and Culture Change e all consider ourselves to be unique individuals with our own set of per- sonal opinions, preferences, habits, and quirks. Indeed, all of us are unique; and yet most of us also share many feelings, beliefs, and habits with most of the people who live in our society. If we live in North America, we are likely to have the feeling that eating dogs is wrong, have the belief that bacteria Wor viruses cause illness, and have the habit of sleeping on a bed. Most peo- ple hardly ever think about the ideas and customs they share with other people in their society, assuming them to be “natural.” These ideas and be- haviors are part of what we mean by culture. We only begin to become chapter 2 aware that our culture is different when we become aware that other peo- ples have different feelings, different beliefs, and different habits from ours. So most North Americans would never even think of the possibility of eat- CHAPTER OUTLINE ing dog meat if they did not know that people in some other societies com- Defining Culture monly do so. They would not realize that their belief in germs was cultural if they were not aware that people in some societies think that witchcraft or Cultural Constraints evil spirits causes illness. They might not become aware that it is their cus- Attitudes That Hinder tom to sleep on beds if they were not aware that people in many societies the Study of Cultures sleep on the floor or on the ground.
    [Show full text]
  • Dying the Good Death: Cultural Competence and Variance in Hospice Care
    Tapestries: Interwoven voices of local and global identities Volume 8 Issue 1 Resisting Borders: Rethinking the Limits Article 9 of American Studies 2019 Dying the Good Death: Cultural Competence and Variance in Hospice Care Lydia Koh-Krienke Macalester College, [email protected] Keywords: Death, Hospice, Cultural competence, Culturally relevant, Pluralism, Universalism Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.macalester.edu/tapestries Recommended Citation Koh-Krienke, Lydia (2019) "Dying the Good Death: Cultural Competence and Variance in Hospice Care," Tapestries: Interwoven voices of local and global identities: Vol. 8 : Iss. 1 , Article 9. Available at: https://digitalcommons.macalester.edu/tapestries/vol8/iss1/9 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the American Studies Department at DigitalCommons@Macalester College. It has been accepted for inclusion in Tapestries: Interwoven voices of local and global identities by an authorized editor of DigitalCommons@Macalester College. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Dying the Good Death: Cultural Competence and Variance in Hospice Care Lydia Koh-Krienke nun-chi. While I would still most likely be Introduction dying in my house, it may have a greater What do you want your death to significance to me as an act of “returning look like? home”. In both of these cases, I die a good I am at home, in a familiar bed with death, even though the setting and process may the nostalgic scent of simmering chicken broth differ. wafting up my nose. Four generations of my This paper will interrogate what it family surround me; the throng of means to “die well” by examining the grandchildren, too young to understand, play intersections between death and culture.
    [Show full text]