RDA Obit Draft W Figs

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

RDA Obit Draft W Figs 1 Obituary: The Nine Lives of Richard D. Alexander 2 3 Richard D. Alexander, T.H. Hubbell Distinguished 4 University Professor of Biology at the University of 5 Michigan, Curator of Insects at the Museum of 6 Zoology there, a member of the National 7 Academy of Sciences, and a pioneer in the 8 study of the evolutionary basis of human 9 behavior, died on the 20th of August, 2018 at age 10 88. Alexander is survived by his wife of 68 years 11 Lorraine Kearnes Alexander; his brother Noel 12 (Donna); his daughters Susan (Sarita) and Nancy; 13 (Photo by Mark O’Brien) his grandchildren Morgan, Lydia, Lincoln, and 14 Winona; and his great-grandson Ezekiel; several nieces and nephews; and “young" 15 Tom Pyle who lived with Alexander’s family for years while growing up. 16 17 Alexander’s life is an iconic American success story. He rose from humble beginnings, 18 having been born on November 18th, 1929 and raised on a small, single-family farm in 19 rural Illinois, without electricity or indoor plumbing. Although his early schooling took 20 place in a one room school house, and he had no thought of attending college, his good 21 grades and keen intellect enabled him to attend Blackburn College and then Illinois 22 State Normal University, obtaining his bachelor’s degree in 1950. He served in the army 23 during the Korean War, stationed at Fort Knox, then went on complete a PhD in 1 24 Entomology at Ohio State University in 1956. As a graduate student he pioneered the 25 use of new acoustic recording technology developed in WWII, to study insect behavior, 26 revolutionizing the study of acoustic communication and speciation in the process. In 27 1957, he was hired by the University of Michigan, where he spent his career. At 28 Michigan Alexander became a national leader in evolutionary biology. His early career 29 awards include the Newcomb Cleveland Prize (1961) from the American Association for 30 the Advancement of Science for his paper "The role of behavioral study in cricket 31 classification” and the Daniel Giraud Eliot medal (1971) from the National Academy of 32 Sciences for “outstanding fundamental work on the systematics, evolution, and behavior 33 of crickets." 34 35 Richard Alexander and friend in 2015 (photo by David Lahti) 2 36 The "nine lives" in the title of this obituary does not refer to a particular fondness for cats 37 on Alexander’s part (in fact, he was partial to horses), or to some series of near death 38 experiences (although he had some of those). Rather, it refers to the many different 39 interests that Alexander pursued during his lifetime. Growing up, he worked as a farmer, 40 rancher and horse trainer, activities he continued throughout his life. He was also a 41 talented poet, songwriter, musician, and wood-carver. In addition to his professional 42 scientific writings, he was a prolific author of many kinds of books, including children’s 43 stories, biographical texts and practical guides, especially on horse training. 44 45 Evolutionary Entomology 46 Alexander began his professional career in the bosom of 47 entomology, with a firm focus on the inter-related fields of 48 acoustic communication, systematics and speciation. It 49 was his expertise in those fields that equipped him to 50 become a major contributor to our understanding of the 51 (cricket by R.D. Alexander) evolution of cooperation in general and a leading thinker on 52 the evolution of the social behavior of that “uniquely unique” species -- humans. His 53 early studies of insect behavior convinced him that behavior and communication are 54 clearly evolved phenomena, underlain by genes as certainly as morphology is, an 55 insight he came to partly because of the utility of behavior in distinguishing species. 56 This understanding led him to realize the importance of behavior in adaptive evolution, 57 leading naturally to further insights into social evolution. The communication systems of 58 crickets had consequences for diversification and speciation, and the social behavior of 3 59 insects like wasps that benefitted offspring, as well as more distant kin, via kin selection 60 and mutualism – were all comprehensible in terms of Darwinian selection and likewise 61 applicable to humans. 62 63 A meeting in 1987 of several of the thinkers who were responsible for the modern unification of 64 evolutionary and behavioral science. Left to right, in the back: David Buss, George C. Williams, Martin 65 Daly, and Mildred Dickemann. In the front, William D. Hamilton, Napoleon Chagnon, and Richard D. 66 Alexander. Three other people who profoundly influenced Alexander’s thinking are Robert Trivers, William 67 Irons, and Margo Wilson. Photo from richarddalexander.com maintained by David Lahti. 68 69 From beginnings in evolutionary entomology, Richard Alexander developed multiple, 70 ground-breaking theories concerning the evolution and development of key human 71 social traits, including monogamy, juvenile helplessness (altriciality), parental and 72 alloparental care, incest and cousin-marriage, cooperation in increasingly large social 4 73 groups and the associated problems of warfare, deceit and self-deception, language 74 and scenario-building, music and the arts, humor, religion, and even science as a 75 human endeavor. 76 77 Alexander was instrumental in founding the multi-disciplinary Human Behavior and 78 Evolution Program at the University of Michigan, alumni of which founded the Human 79 Behavior and Evolution Society. Long before "sociobiology" or “evolutionary 80 psychology” were widely discussed, Alexander had already written many profoundly 81 influential works on evolution and social behavior. His review article entitled “The 82 Evolution of Social Behavior”, published in 1974 in the “Annual Review of Ecology and 83 Systematics,” has been cited over 4000 times. He continued to publish significant 84 contributions on the evolution of social behavior on a regular basis throughout the rest 85 of his career. A volume in his honor celebrating and summarizing many of these 86 contributions was published in 2013 (Human Social Evolution: The Foundational Works 87 of Richard D. Alexander (K. Summers and B. Crespi, eds), 2013, Oxford University 88 Press). 89 90 Alexander’s (1974) review of the evolution of social behavior presented a 91 comprehensive theoretical framework for understanding eusociality and social systems 92 in insects, birds, and mammals. The 58 page synthetic review laid out hypotheses that 93 a generation of biologists and anthropologists have spent their lives testing. When 94 asked why it was so frequently cited, Alexander offered an uncharacteristically self- 5 95 deprecating answer: ‘because I got so many things wrong and every hot-shot, young 96 scientist wants to make his reputation by showing me up’. 97 98 One criticism of the review led Alexander to one of his best known discoveries, 99 eusociality – societies with sterile workers -- in the naked mole-rat: why had eusociality 100 evolved many times among the hymenoptera and only once in all other insects? 101 William Hamilton had recently suggested that kin selection might help answer the 102 question since hymenoptera (ants, bees, wasps) are haplodiploid which means sisters 103 are more closely related to each other than they are to brothers or their own offspring, 104 something that favors sisters becoming workers and rearing more sisters instead of 105 their own young. It turned out that the factors driving the evolution of sterile workers are 106 more complicated than simple genetic relatedness, but the insight of this finding is still 107 important. In his 1974 review, Alexander argued that parental care (not asymmetric 108 relatedness) was the essential ancestral character in the evolution of insect eusociality. 109 Unlike Hamilton’s haplodiploid hypothesis, Alexander’s included the Isoptera (termites), 110 a diploid group that evolved from ancestral wood roaches with parental care. One 111 criticism of Alexander’s parental care hypothesis was that there are many other taxa 112 with parental care, particularly within vertebrates, where eusociality has failed to 113 emerge. 114 115 Alexander took this criticism seriously and explored it by posing the related question: if 116 there were a eusocial vertebrate, what would it be like? He speculated that such a 117 vertebrate would have certain characteristics and suggested a hypothetical 6 118 subterranean rodent, living in the African savannah (associated with patchily distributed 119 plants with large storage root storage organs), which fed on these tubers and suffered 120 predation by snakes. Each trait followed from a deep understanding of life history 121 constraints and tradeoffs. Underground burrows would allow expansion for large 122 colonies; roots would allow low risk foraging; snakes would require self-sacrifice to 123 defend a colony. After listening to Alexander present this hypothetical eusocial 124 vertebrate at a seminar at Northern Arizona University, mammologist Terry Vaughn 125 pointed out that it sounded very much like the naked mole-rat and, soon after, 126 Alexander went to Kenya to collect a live colony for observation. This led to an 127 explosion of scientific research on this odd-looking 128 mammal, culminating in the 1990 volume, “The Biology 129 of the Naked Mole Rat” (Sherman, Jarvis and Alexander 130 1990). Like the 1845 discovery of the planet Neptune by 131 Adams and Leverrier, Alexander explored evolutionary 132 space by extrapolation, with a deep understanding of the 133 forces that shape social behavior, and was able to 134 predict eusociality in a taxon where it had not yet been 135 directly observed. 136 137 Human Evolution 138 Exactly one hundred years after Darwin published his great work on The Descent of 139 Man (1871), Alexander published his first paper on the same topic, entitled "The Search 140 for an Evolutionary Philosophy of Man" (Alexander 1971), sketching a quest that would 7 141 occupy most of his attention for the remainder of his career.
Recommended publications
  • THE DESCENT of MADNESS: Evolutionary Origins of Psychosis
    THE DESCENT OF MADNESS Drawing on evidence from across the behavioural and natural sciences, this book advances a radical new hypothesis: that madness exists as a costly consequence of the evolution of a sophisticated social brain in Homo sapiens. Having explained the rationale for an evolutionary approach to psych- osis, the author makes a case for psychotic illness in our living ape relatives, as well as in human ancestors. He then reviews existing evolutionary theor- ies of psychosis, before introducing his own thesis: that the same genes causing madness are responsible for the evolution of our highly social brain. Jonathan Burns’ novel Darwinian analysis of the importance of psychosis for human survival provides some meaning for this form of suffering. It also spurs us on to a renewed commitment to changing our societies in a way that allows the mentally ill the opportunity of living. The Descent of Madness will be of interest to those in the fields of psychiatry, psychology, sociology and anthropology, and is also accessible to the general reader. Jonathan Burns is chief specialist psychiatrist at the Nelson Mandela School of Medicine. His main areas of research include psychotic illnesses, human brain evolution and evolutionary origins of psychosis. THE DESCENT OF MADNESS Evolutionary Origins of Psychosis and the Social Brain Jonathan Burns First published 2007 by Routledge 27 Church Road, Hove, East Sussex BN3 2FA Simultaneously published in the USA and Canada by Routledge 270 Madison Ave, New York, NY 10016 Routledge is an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business © 2007 Jonathan Burns This edition published in the Taylor & Francis e-Library, 2007.
    [Show full text]
  • Raising-Darwins-Consciousness.Pdf
    RAISING DARWIN'S CONSCIOUSNESS Female Sexuality and the Prehominid Origins of Patriarchy Sarah Blaffer Hrdy University of California, Davis Sociobiologists and feminists agree that men in patriarchal social systems seek to control females, but sociobiologists go further, using Darwin's theory of sexual selection and Trivers's ideas on parental investment to explain why males should attempt to control female sexuality. From this perspective, the stage for the development under some conditions of patriarchal social systems was set over the course of primate evolution. Sexual selection encompasses both competition between males and female choice. But in applying this theory to our "lower origins" (pre- hominid ancestors), Darwin assumed that choices were made by essen- tially "coy" females. I argue here that female solicitation of multiple males (either simultaneously or sequentially, depending on the breeding system) characterized prehominid females; this prehominid legacy of cy- clical sexual assertiveness, itself possibly a female counter-strategy to male efforts to control the timing of female reproduction, generated fur- ther male counter-strategies. This dialectic had important implications for emerging hominid mating systems, human evolution, and the devel- opment of patriarchal arrangements in some human societies. For homi- nid males who will invest in offspring, there would be powerful selection for emotions, behaviors, and customs that ensure them certainty of pater- nity. The sexual modesty that so struck Darwin can be explained as a recent evolved or learned (perhaps both) adaptation in women to avoid penalties imposed by patrilines on daughters and mates who failed to conform to the patriline's prevailing norms for their sex.
    [Show full text]
  • Iciumv LEADERS in ANIMAL BEHAVIOR the Second Generation
    UO1BJ3U3D iciumv LEADERS IN ANIMAL BEHAVIOR The Second Generation Edited by Lee C. Drickamer Northern Arizona University Donald A. Dewsbury University of Florida CAMBRIDGE UNIVERSITY PRESS 13 Myths, monkeys, and motherhood: a compromising life SARAH BLAFFER HRDY Definition of an Anthropologist: "(Someone) who studies human nature in all its diversity." Carmelo Lison-Tolosana (1966) Maternal effects (1946-64) From a young age, I was interested in why humans do what they do. With little exposure to science, certainly no inkling that there might be people in the world who studied other animals in order to better understand our species, I decided to become a novelist. Born in Leaders in Animal Behavior: The Setond Generation, ed. L. C. Drickamcr & D. A. Dewsbury. Published by Cambridge University Press. CO Cambridge University Press 2010. 344 Sarah Bluffer Hrdy Texas in 1946, right at the start of the postwar baby boom, I was the third of five children - Speedway. Prevailin four daughters and finally the long-awaited son. My father's father, R. L. Blaffer, had come segregation, and pi to Texas from Hamburg via New Orleans in 1901 at the time oil was discovered at interested in the e1 Spindletop. He recognized that fortunes would be made in the oil business. He married inheritance, female Sarah Campbell from Lampasas, whose father was in that business. I was named for her, the women's moven Sarah Campbell Blaffer II. My mother's father's ancestors, the Hardins, French Huguenots Reared by a suo from Tennessee, arrived earlier, in 1825, before Texas was even a state.
    [Show full text]
  • Cinderella Effect Facts
    The “Cinderella effect”: Elevated mistreatment of stepchildren in comparison to those living with genetic parents. Martin Daly & Margo Wilson Department of Psychology, Neuroscience & Behaviour McMaster University Hamilton, Ontario, Canada L8S 4K1 <[email protected]> <[email protected]> Theory Parents commit a huge amount of time, attention and material resources to the care of their children, as well as incurring life-threatening risks to defend them and bodily depletion to nourish them. Why are parents motivated to invest so heavily in their children? From an evolutionary perspective, the answer is surely that natural selection has favoured intensive parental care in our lineage. Those ancestral genotypes and phenotypes that best succeeded in raising children to become reproducing adults were the ones that persisted and proliferated. If the psychological underpinnings of parental care have indeed evolved by natural selection, we may furthermore anticipate that parental feeling and action will not typically be elicited by just any random conspecific juvenile. Instead, care-providing animals may be expected to direct their care selectively towards young who are (a) their own genetic offspring rather than those of their reproductive rivals, and (b) able to convert parental investment into increased prospects for survival and reproduction. This is the kernel of the theory of discriminative parental solicitude, which (notwithstanding some interesting twists and caveats) has been abundantly verified in a broad range of care-giving species
    [Show full text]
  • Steps to an Evolutionary Ecology of Mind and Morality
    This page intentionally left blank Death, Hope and Sex Steps to an Evolutionary Ecology of Mind and Morality By showing how and why human nature is what is is, evolutionary theory can help us see better what we need to do to improve the human condition. Following evolutionary theory to its logical conclusion, Death, Hope, and Sex uses life history theory and attachment theory to construct a model of human nature in which critical features are understood in terms of the development of alternative reproductive strategies contingent on environmental risk and uncertainty. James Chisholm examines the implication of this model for perspectives on concerns associated with human reproduction, including teen pregnancy, and young male violence. He thus develops new approaches for thorny issues such as the nature–nurture and mind–body dichotomies. Bridging the gap between the social and biological sciences, this far-reaching volume will be a source of inspiration, debate, and discussion for all those interested in the evolution of human nature and the potential for an evolutionary humanism. james s. chisholm is Associate Professor in the Department of Anatomy and Human Biology at the University of Western Australia in Perth, Australia. His previous publications include Navajo Infancy: An Ethological Study of Child Development (1983). Death, Hope and Sex Steps to an Evolutionary Ecology of Mind and Morality James S. Chisholm The Pitt Building, Trumpington Street, Cambridge, United Kingdom The Edinburgh Building, Cambridge CB2 2RU, UK 40 West 20th Street, New York, NY 10011-4211, USA 477 Williamstown Road, Port Melbourne, VIC 3207, Australia Ruiz de Alarcón 13, 28014 Madrid, Spain Dock House, The Waterfront, Cape Town 8001, South Africa http://www.cambridge.org © James S.
    [Show full text]
  • The Theoretical Foundations of Evolutionary Psychology
    3GC01 06/09/2015 12:40:42 Page 3 Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (2015). The theoretical foundations of evolutionary psychology. In Buss, D. M. (Ed.), The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology, Second edition. Volume 1: Foundations. (pp. 3-87). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. CHAPTER 1 The Theoretical Foundations of Evolutionary Psychology JOHN TOOBY and LEDA COSMIDES THE EMERGENCE OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY: WHAT IS AT STAKE? HE THEORY OF evolution by natural selection has revolutionary implications for understanding the design of the human mind and brain, as Darwin himself was Tthe first to recognize (Darwin, 1859). Indeed, a principled understanding of the network of causation that built the functional architecture of the human species offers the possibility of transforming the study of humanity into a natural science capable of precision and rapid progress. Yet, more than a century and a half after The Origin of Species was published, many of the psychological, social, and behavioral sciences continue to be grounded on assumptions that evolutionarily informed researchers know to be false; the rest have only in the past few decades set to work on the radical reformulations of their disciplines necessary to make them consistent with findings in the evolutionary sciences, information theory, computer science, physics, the neuro- sciences, molecular and cellular biology, genetics, behavioral ecology, hunter-gatherer studies, biological anthropology, primatology, and so on (Pinker, 1997, 2002; Tooby & Cosmides, 1992). Evolutionary psychology is the long-forestalled scientific attempt to assemble out of the disjointed, fragmentary, and mutually contradictory human disciplines a single, logically integrated research framework for the psychological, social, and behavioral sciences—a framework that not only incorporates the evolu- tionary sciences and information theory on a full and equal basis, but that systemati- cally works out all the revisions in existing belief and research practice that such a synthesis requires (Tooby & Cosmides, 1992).
    [Show full text]
  • States in Mind Anthony C. Lopez, Rose Mcdermott, and Michael
    States in Mind States in Mind Anthony C. Lopez, Rose McDermott, and Evolution, Coalitional Psychology, and Michael Bang International Politics Petersen One of the most com- monly studied puzzles in international politics is the recurrence of coalitional competition and aggression between political groups such as states. Indeed, this pattern constitutes an enduring and central feature of all politics. Yet de- spite the tragic endurance of this leitmotif throughout history, its manifestation varies through time and space. Some wars are fought for honor or revenge, whereas others are ignited for mere opportunism or as a consequence of vari- ous misperceptions, whatever their source. We argue that evolutionary theory provides a theoretical framework that can explain both the stubborn endur- ance and dynamic diversity of coalitional behavior. Debate on the relevance of “human nature” and biological factors for explaining political behavior is not new.1 Yet the comprehensive value of evo- lutionary theory for explaining important aspects of international politics has not been adequately explicated. As we discuss below, this has in part been a consequence of general skepticism about the validity and scope of evolution- ary theory for explaining political behavior. We argue, however, that evolu- tionary psychology can generate falsiªable ex ante predictions that are of central interest to the study of international politics, and we offer several hy- potheses derived from this model to illustrate the depth of this approach. Evo- lutionary psychologists have already generated a large body of work that suggests that the human brain contains webs of psychological mechanisms, or adaptations, each designed to operate in domains relevant to modern politics, and which emerged as a product of natural selection.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolution, Child Abuse and the Constitution Christopher Malrborough
    Journal of Law and Policy Volume 11 | Issue 2 Article 6 2003 Evolution, Child Abuse and the Constitution Christopher Malrborough Follow this and additional works at: https://brooklynworks.brooklaw.edu/jlp Recommended Citation Christopher Malrborough, Evolution, Child Abuse and the Constitution, 11 J. L. & Pol'y (2003). Available at: https://brooklynworks.brooklaw.edu/jlp/vol11/iss2/6 This Note is brought to you for free and open access by the Law Journals at BrooklynWorks. It has been accepted for inclusion in Journal of Law and Policy by an authorized editor of BrooklynWorks. MARLBOROUGHMACROX.DOC 6/25/03 5:10 PM EVOLUTION, CHILD ABUSE AND THE CONSTITUTION Christopher Marlborough* INTRODUCTION The presence of a non-genetic parent in a child’s home is the largest single risk factor for severe child maltreatment yet discovered.1 Professor Owen Jones has used the example of stepparent infanticide to explain how evolutionary analysis in law can serve society’s goals when prevailing theories have failed.2 * Brooklyn Law School Class of 2003; B.A., State University of New York at Purchase, 1991. I would like to thank Professors Jennifer Rosato and Bailey Kuklin for their input and guidance in writing this note and my lovely wife Jennifer for her infinite patience. 1 MARTIN DALY & MARGO WILSON, THE TRUTH ABOUT CINDERELLA: A DARWINIAN VIEW OF PARENTAL LOVE 7 (1998) [hereinafter DALY & WILSON, CINDERELLA]. 2 Owen Jones, Evolutionary Analysis in Law: An Introduction and Application to Child Abuse, 75 N.C. L. REV. 1117 (1997) [hereinafter Jones, Child Abuse]. Professor Jones suggests a four-stage process to determine when evolutionary principles can be helpful to inform legal policy.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolutionary Psychology As a Unifying Framework and Meta-Theory
    Part V | Chapter 1 Chapter 1 Ultimate explanations: Evolutionary psychology as a unifying framework and meta-theory The term ‘evolutionary psychology’ (EP) was coined during “lengthy and intensive debates about how to apply evolution to behavior” (Tooby & Cosmides, 2005, p. 15) between Martin Daly, Margo Wilson, Don Symons, John Tooby, Leda Cosmides, and David Buss in the 1980s. It is a relatively young and developing way of thinking in psychology that can serve as a meta-theoretical framework, as it builds directly on the foundations of biology. More spe- cifically, EP is based on the only scientific explanation for the complexity of earthly life forms, namely evolution by natural selection. In the scientific community, it is largely ac- knowledged that humans are a product of evolution by natural selection too. We are mammals belonging to the branch of the tree of life called primates and our closest living relatives are the chimpanzees and bonobos, with whom we share a common ancestor that lived some 6 to 7 million years ago. Though such long time spans are beyond our ‘natural’ capacity to comprehend, consider this: the process of evolving from a light-sensitive cell to a human eye can happen in fewer than 400,000 years (Nilsson & Pelger, 1994). Researchers named our species homo sapiens and contemporary researchers have deter- mined our ‘start date’ to be at least 300,000 years ago, based on new homo sapiens findings in Morocco (Hublin et al., 2017). ‘Start date’ is a bit of a misleading term, as there is of course no ‘sudden appearance,’ but a very slow, invisible, and gradual change.
    [Show full text]
  • An Evolutionary Solution to Manslaughter Mitigation
    Emory Law Journal Volume 62 Issue 1 2012 Principles for Passion Killing: An Evolutionary Solution to Manslaughter Mitigation D. Barret Broussard Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarlycommons.law.emory.edu/elj Recommended Citation D. B. Broussard, Principles for Passion Killing: An Evolutionary Solution to Manslaughter Mitigation, 62 Emory L. J. 179 (2012). Available at: https://scholarlycommons.law.emory.edu/elj/vol62/iss1/3 This Comment is brought to you for free and open access by the Journals at Emory Law Scholarly Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Emory Law Journal by an authorized editor of Emory Law Scholarly Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. BROUSSARD PROOFS1 10/31/2012 7:49 AM PRINCIPLES FOR PASSION KILLING: AN EVOLUTIONARY SOLUTION TO MANSLAUGHTER MITIGATION† ABSTRACT The law recognizes the frailty of human nature by mitigating murder to manslaughter when committed in the heat of passion or under extreme emotional disturbance. Evolutionary analysis entails the scientific study of the principles of human nature. Yet, the law’s understanding of human nature is not congruent with evolutionary analysis. To be legally provoked under common law for manslaughter mitigation, a homicide must be in response to one of four kinds of provocation: adultery, mutual combat, false arrest, and violent assault. And under adultery, only sexual infidelity counts. Sexual infidelity is not the only type of infidelity that can push a person into a homicidal rage, and while American jurisdictions have started moving away from the rigid categories, sexual infidelity remains a paradigmatic approach for mitigation. The Model Penal Code attempted to make the law more contextual, but it created a new series of adjudications that are expansive and also incongruent with evolutionary analysis.
    [Show full text]
  • International Society for Behavioral Ecology Supplement to Behavioral Ecology
    2 0 1 4 - V O L U M E 2 6 I S S U E 1 International Society for Behavioral Ecology www.behavecol.com Supplement to Behavioral Ecology C O N T E N T S Editorial 1 Conference update 3 Book reviews 6 From the President 2 Conference calendar 4 Books for review 10 The new ISBE Executive 2 Spotlight on young scientists 5 F R O M T H E N E W S L E T T E R E D I T O R With this issue, I become the new ISBE newsletter I look forward hearing from editor. First of all I would like to thank Mariella you - the ISBE members. Herberstein for her excellent work as newsletter This is your newsletter, and editor during the last seven years. It will be a it will be nothing without challenge trying to fill her shoes! contributions from members. I am happy to receive A short introduction to who I am: After finishing a feedback on the content as PhD in Trondheim, Norway, I spent five years in well as ideas on how the Australia doing postdocs at Bob Wong's lab at newsletter can be developed. Monash and at John Endler's lab at Deakin University. Please email me any Since 2012, I am a lecturer at Linnaeus University in suggestions or contributions, Sweden, where I teach mainly Behavioural Ecology, or just grab hold of me at the Ethology and Evolutionary biology. My research New York meeting in August. interests center on behavioural strategies in animals, This is what I look like: using fishes - such as gobies, guppies and sticklebacks - as model systems.
    [Show full text]
  • 8Dissertation-Title-Page Copy
    PERSONALITY AND REPRODUCTIVE SUCCESS IN THE ACHE (PARAGUAY): IMPLICATIONS FOR THE EVOLUTION OF HUMAN INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES _______________________________________ A Dissertation presented to the Faculty of the Graduate School at the University of Missouri-Columbia _______________________________________________________ In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy _____________________________________________________ by DREW H. BAILEY Dr. David C. Geary, Dissertation Supervisor July, 2012 The undersigned, appointed by the dean of the Graduate School, have examined the dissertation entitled PERSONALITY AND REPRODUCTIVE SUCCESS IN THE ACHE (PARAGUAY): IMPLICATIONS FOR THE EVOLUTION OF HUMAN INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES presented by Drew H. Bailey, a candidate for the degree of doctor of philosophy, and hereby certify that, in their opinion, it is worthy of acceptance. Professor David C. Geary Professor Amanda Rose Professor Phillip Wood Professor Mark Flinn Professor Craig Palmer ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I thank my collaborators on this project, Robert Walker, Gregory Blomquist, David Geary, and Kim Hill, and my dissertation committee, David Geary, Amanda Rose, Phillip Wood, Mark Flinn, and Craig Palmer, for improving the project and manuscript significantly throughout the process, and for their support during my graduate work. I also thank the individuals in the Ache communities who participated in the study and those who assisted us in the field, specifically Ricardo Mbekrorogi, Germino Chachugi, and Nemecio Urugi, and thank Keity and Robert Walker for their assistance in the field. For their assistance with translation and adapting surveys, I thank Elizabeth Bailey, Wilmer Fernandez, Kiero Guerra, Marcia Kearns, and Jon Thacker. I thank David Geary for his mentoring during my time at Missouri, and for his support during this long and difficult project.
    [Show full text]