High-Resolution, in Vivo Imaging of the Human Cone Photoreceptor Mosaic

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

High-Resolution, in Vivo Imaging of the Human Cone Photoreceptor Mosaic High-resolution, in vivo imaging of the human cone photoreceptor mosaic Alex R. Wade, B.A. A thesis submitted for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy at University College London, U.K. Institute of Ophthalmology University College London November 1998 ProQuest Number: 10015876 All rights reserved INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. uest. ProQuest 10015876 Published by ProQuest LLC(2016). Copyright of the Dissertation is held by the Author. All rights reserved. This work is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code. Microform Edition © ProQuest LLC. ProQuest LLC 789 East Eisenhower Parkway P.O. Box 1346 Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346 Abstract This thesis describes a project to image the human retina in vivo using a modified confocal laser scanning ophthalmoscope (cLSO). An optical attachment to an existing prototype cLSO was designed and constructed. The attachment reduced the field of view of the cLSO to approximately three degrees of visual angle at the retina but also reduced the amount of light returning to the imaging detector and consequently lowered the signal to noise ratio. The video output of the cLSO was digitised using a high-speed, low-noise video frame grabber. Many individual frames from a single imaging sequence were aligned and averaged using novel image processing software to improve the signal to noise ratio. Arrays of point-like structures could then be identified in the final, processed images. Experiments were undertaken to confirm the point structures corresponded to cone photoreceptor apertures. • The point sizes and spaeings were compatible with those found in anatomical studies and it was shown that point spacing increased with increasing retinal eccentricity - a fact which was also predicted from anatomical studies. • By fitting an additional, high-intensity fixation target to the cLSO it was shown that the point structures were located at the focal plane at which light is trapped into the visual system. The above results were evidence that the point sources were produced by light reflecting from the cone photoreceptor apertures: the LSO/MSA combination was imaging the cone mosaic. An additional experiment was conducted to study the time course of reflectivity changes in individual photoreceptors. It was shown that at least one factor contributing to the variation in imaged photoreceptor brightness was a signal correlated to the arterial pulse. This project has shown that it is possible to routinely image the cone photoreceptor mosaic in vivo at retinal eccentricities of between two and four degrees using a cLSO. It is also possible to use this device to obtain physiological imaging data from the cone mosaic with a temporal resolution of 1/25 second. Declaration I declare that this thesis submitted for the degree of PhD is my own composition and the data presented herein is my own original work, unless otherwise stated. Alex R. Wade, B.A. Acknowledgements I have been fortunate to have conducted this PhD in a well-equipped lab under the excellent supervision of Prof. Fred Fitzke. It has also been pleasure to work with other members of the department of Visual Science: Dr. Glen Jeffery has provided valuable advice on retinal anatomy and supplied the biological samples used in chapter 4. Adrian Williams, Tony Halfyard, Roger Chan and Chris Jubb have provided both technical support and constant encouragement. The prototype cLSO was donated by Carl Zeiss GmbH who also made technical information and full ZEMAX models of the device available. The technical staff at Matrox Electronic Imaging also provided details of their pattern-matching software routine and valuable hardware support. The lab is funded by Marks and Spencer, Fight for Sight and the Foundation Fighting Blindness along with the MRC who also supported my PhD. Dr. Michael Scholz of the Technische Universitat in Berlin was kind enough to devote computer time to perform blind deconvolution on some of my images and Dr. Austin Roorda gave permission to use the unpublished image in figure 1.10. I would like to thank my many excellent teachers throughout the years: John Biffin and Dr. Andrea Brand for communicating the excitement of biological science. Prof. John Mollon who guided me onto the one true path of vision research and Dr. Rhodri Cusack who has made learning an adventure for many years. Finally, I must thank my family for their unwavering support over the years and Isabel for proof-reading this thesis and helping me to maintain my focus. Table of contents ABSTRACT 2 DECLARATION 4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS 5 TABLE OF CONTENTS 6 LIST OF FIGURES 10 ABBREVIATIONS, SYMBOLS AND ACRONYMS 13 PUBLICATIONS 15 CHAPTER 1 - INTRODUCTION 16 Overview of PhD - description of aims and accomplishments. 17 Overview of photoreceptor imaging: 19 Background 19 Physiological optics 21 The model eye 21 The pupil and the MTF 22 Wavefront aberration 26 Photoreceptor biology 27 Photoreceptor types 31 Waveguide properties 36 Spacing and mosaic 41 High-resolution in vivo retinal imaging 45 Ocular speckle interferometry 48 Adaptive optics 49 The confocal LSO 51 Image processing 58 Summary 61 FIGURE 1.10 62 CHAPTER 2 - METHODS AND MATERIALS 63 Hardware 63 Zeiss confocal LSO 63 Beam profile and power 67 Micro scanning attachment (MSA) 68 Fixation and stabilisation 68 Pupil dilation 69 Image acquisition 69 Software 70 Image averaging 70 CHAPTER 3 - IMAGE ALIGNMENT 72 Noise and the SNR 72 Normalised cross-correlation 78 Fourier domain convolution 79 Hierarchical resolution search 80 MIL pattern matching routine 81 Low-pass filtering 82 Estimates of image signal to noise ratios 88 Signal-dependent noise 90 Averaged images 93 Test target 96 Autofluorescence images 96 High-magnification images 97 Discussion 99 CHAPTER 4 - PRELIMINARY STUDIES 101 Optics design - optimisation and modelling in ZEMAX 101 Results 105 Variation in detected power 105 Axial resolution 108 Lateral resolution 110 Scan geometry 116 Biological samples 116 Ferret retina 118 Post-mortem human retina 119 7n Vi VO human retinal imaging 121 Discussion 127 CHAPTER 5 - PHOTORECEPTOR IMAGING 129 Introduction 129 Mosaic spacing 129 Mosaic focal plane 134 Results 136 Images and montages 136 Cone spacing 140 Image focal plane 145 Discussion 154 CHAPTER 6 - CONE REFLECTIVITY CHANGES OVER TIME 159 Introduction 159 Analysis methods 164 Image acquisition 164 Image signal analysis 167 Results 169 Discussion 177 CHAPTER 7 - CONCLUSIONS 179 APPENDIX A 182 COMPARISON OF CONVENTIONAL AND CONFOCAL IMAGING SYSTEMS 182 Single lens imaging system image formation 182 Confocal scanning imaging system 185 The MTF 189 REFERENCES 192 List of figures FIGURE Descriotion 1.1 Cross section of eye 1.2 MTFs of two model eyes 1.3 Diagrams of photoreceptors 1.4 Rod density map 1.5 Cone density map 1.6 The Stiles-Crawford effect 1.7 Cone packing - spacing and density 1.8 Speckle interferometry 1.9 Confocal microscope 1.10 Example of blind deconvolution 2.1 Zeiss confocal LSO 2.2 Beam expansion by the MSA 3.1 Example of image averaging 3.2 Adding noise to a test image 3.3 2-dimensional cross-correlation 3.4 Filtered cross-correlation 3.5 Mean errors in peak location 3.6 Raw image frames 3.7 Autofluorescence - variance against intensity. 3.8 High-magnification - variance against intensity 3.9 Decrease in image variance - test target 3.10 Decrease in image variance - AF image 3.11 Decrease in image variance - high magnification 3.12 Fourier domain filtering 4.1 MSA point spread functions 4.2 Scan angle point spread functions 4.3 Fourier analysis of power variation 10 4.4 Axial response 4.5 SEM image of pinhole 4.6 LSO images of pinhole 4.7 Response to a 5 micron line 4.8 Images of calibrated graticule 4.9 Images of post-mortem ferret retina 4.10 Images of post-mortem human retina 4.11 in vivo retinal image with MSA 4.12 Locating high-magnification images 4.13 High magnification images from 8 subjects 5.1 Yellott’s ring 5.2 Irregularities in the cLSO scan 5.3 High-resolution images taken with the LSO/MSA 5.4a Montage of high magnification images from subject AW 5.4b Montage of high-magnification images from subject RC 5.5 Images from 3 retinal locations 5.6 Array spacing at 3 locations 5.7 Power spectra - real data and simulations 5.8 Diagram of fixation target system 5.9 Shift in retinal focal plane with target position 5.10 Raw high magnification images from different focal planes 5.11 Use of Fourier transforms to find low-frequency details 5.12 Plot of image power against retinal focal plane 5.13 Comparison with images from (Miller, Williams et al. 1996) 5.14 Comparison with AO system 6.1 Change in photoreceptor array appearance over time 6.2 Example of cross-correlation peak heights 6.3 Signal detection 6.4 Cross-correlation matrix for blank target 6.5 Cross-correlation matrix for resting retina 11 6.6 Fourier analysis of cross-correlation data 6.7 Fourier analysis of raw intensity data A1 Schematic imaging system A2 Schematic confocal imaging system A3 Comparison of conventional and confocal PSFs A4 Optical sectioning power of conventional and confocal systems 12 Abbreviations, symbols and acronyms X Wavelength of imaging light AF Auto-fluorescence AO Adaptive optics bpm Beats per minute cGMP Cyclic guanosine mono phosphate cLSO Confocal LSO cpd Cycles per
Recommended publications
  • What's the Connection?
    WHAT’S THE CONNECTION? Sharon Winter Lake Washington High School Directions for Teachers 12033 NE 80th Street Kirkland, WA 98033 SYNOPSIS Students elicit and observe reflex responses and distinguish between types STUDENT PRIOR KNOWL- of reflexes. They then design and conduct experiments to learn more about EDGE reflexes and their control by the nervous system. Before participating in this LEVEL activity students should be able to: Exploration, Concept/Term Introduction Phases ■ Describe the parts of a Application Phase neuron and explain their functions. ■ Distinguish between sensory and motor neurons. Getting Ready ■ Describe briefly the See sidebars for additional information regarding preparation of this lab. organization of the nervous system. Directions for Setting Up the Lab General: INTEGRATION Into the Biology Curriculum ■ Make an “X” on the chalkboard for the teacher-led introduction. ■ Health ■ Photocopy the Directions for Students pages. ■ Biology I, II ■ Human Anatomy and Teacher Background Physiology A reflex is an involuntary neural response to a specific sensory stimulus ■ AP Biology that threatens the survival or homeostatic state of an organism. Reflexes Across the Curriculum exist in the most primitive of species, usually with a protective function for ■ Mathematics animals when they encounter external and internal stimuli. A primitive ■ Physics ■ example of this protective reflex is the gill withdrawal reflex of the sea slug Psychology Aplysia. In humans and other vertebrates, protective reflexes have been OBJECTIVES maintained and expanded in number. Examples are the gag reflex that At the end of this activity, occurs when objects touch the sides students will be able to: or the back of the throat, and the carotid sinus reflex that restores blood ■ Identify common reflexes pressure to normal when baroreceptors detect an increase in blood pressure.
    [Show full text]
  • A Model of Accommodative-Pupillary Dynamics Stanley Gordon Day Iowa State University
    Iowa State University Capstones, Theses and Retrospective Theses and Dissertations Dissertations 1969 A model of accommodative-pupillary dynamics Stanley Gordon Day Iowa State University Follow this and additional works at: https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/rtd Part of the Electrical and Electronics Commons Recommended Citation Day, Stanley Gordon, "A model of accommodative-pupillary dynamics " (1969). Retrospective Theses and Dissertations. 4649. https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/rtd/4649 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Iowa State University Capstones, Theses and Dissertations at Iowa State University Digital Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Retrospective Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Iowa State University Digital Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. This dissertation has been microâhned exactly as received 69-15,607 DAY, Stanley Gordon, 1939- A MODEL OF ACCOMMODATIVE-PUPILLARY DYNAMICS. Iowa State University, Ph.D., 1969 Engineering, electrical University Microfilms, Inc., Ann Arbor, Michigan ®Copyright by STANLEY GORDON DAY 1969 A MODEL OF ACCOMMODATIVE-PUPILLARY DYNAMICS by Stanley Gordon Day A Dissertation Submitted to the Graduate Faculty in Partial Fulfillment of The Requirements for the Degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Major Subject : Electrical Engineering Approved: Signature was redacted for privacy. In Charge of Major Work Signature was redacted for privacy. Head of Major Department Signature was redacted for privacy. Dea^ of Gradulate College Iowa State University Of Science and Technology Ames, Iowa 1969 il TABLE OF CONTENTS Page DEDICATION iii INTRODUCTION 1 REVIEW OF LITERATURE 4 EQUIPMENT AND METHODS 22 RESULTS AND DISCUSSION 47 SUÎ4MARY AND CONCLUSIONS 60 BIBLIOGRAPHY 62 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS 68 APPENDIX 69 i iii DEDICATION This dissertation is dedicated to Sandra R.
    [Show full text]
  • Binocular Vision
    BINOCULAR VISION Rahul Bhola, MD Pediatric Ophthalmology Fellow The University of Iowa Department of Ophthalmology & Visual Sciences posted Jan. 18, 2006, updated Jan. 23, 2006 Binocular vision is one of the hallmarks of the human race that has bestowed on it the supremacy in the hierarchy of the animal kingdom. It is an asset with normal alignment of the two eyes, but becomes a liability when the alignment is lost. Binocular Single Vision may be defined as the state of simultaneous vision, which is achieved by the coordinated use of both eyes, so that separate and slightly dissimilar images arising in each eye are appreciated as a single image by the process of fusion. Thus binocular vision implies fusion, the blending of sight from the two eyes to form a single percept. Binocular Single Vision can be: 1. Normal – Binocular Single vision can be classified as normal when it is bifoveal and there is no manifest deviation. 2. Anomalous - Binocular Single vision is anomalous when the images of the fixated object are projected from the fovea of one eye and an extrafoveal area of the other eye i.e. when the visual direction of the retinal elements has changed. A small manifest strabismus is therefore always present in anomalous Binocular Single vision. Normal Binocular Single vision requires: 1. Clear Visual Axis leading to a reasonably clear vision in both eyes 2. The ability of the retino-cortical elements to function in association with each other to promote the fusion of two slightly dissimilar images i.e. Sensory fusion. 3. The precise co-ordination of the two eyes for all direction of gazes, so that corresponding retino-cortical element are placed in a position to deal with two images i.e.
    [Show full text]
  • On Voluntary Ocular Accommodation
    Perception & Psychophysics IPS, Vol. 17 (2),209-212 On voluntary ocular accommodation ROBERT R. PROVINE University ofMaryland Baltimore County, 5401 Wilkins A venue, Baltimore, Marylq,nd 21228 and JAY M. ENOCH Department ofOphthalmology, University of Florida CoUege of Medicine, GainesviUe, Florida 32610 Young observers were challenged to induce a marked monocular accommodative response to a relatively weak accommodative stimulus by placing a-9 diopter contact lens on the eye. At first, observers could not produce the desired response, but with training, three of four subjects achieved criterion. Both a voluntary accommodative response and a response to an adequate accommodative stimulus were apparently involved. The voluntary component of the response could be demonstrated by having the observers repeat the task in total darkness. Accommodation is a change in the curvature and a monocularly presented stimulus pattern which consisted of two thickness ofthe crystalline lens which is made in order. horizontal rectangles of light (each measuring 2"12' x 1°SO' and to bring light from near objects into focus on the separated by 44' measured at the entrance pupil of the eye). A small tixation point was centered between the two rectangles. The field retina. At this time we have limited understanding of was focused at intinity by the optical system. that is, the target array the nature of the adequate stimulus for was located at the focal point of a field lens. The system was a accommodation (Campbell & Westheimer, 1959) and classical Badal optometer (Ogle. 1%1) incorporated in a the extent to which the response is innate (reflexive) or Stiles-Crawford (S-C) apparatus (Enoch & Hope, 1972) that was moditied for SoC peak tinding determinations (Blank, Provine, & learned (Fincham, 1951; Heath, 1956; Campbell, Enoch.
    [Show full text]
  • The Role of Focus Cues in Stereopsis, and the Development of a Novel Volumetric Display
    The role of focus cues in stereopsis, and the development of a novel volumetric display by David Morris Hoffman A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Vision Science in the GRADUATE DIVISION of the UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA, BERKELEY Committee in charge: Professor Martin S. Banks, Chair Professor Austin Roorda Professor John Canny Spring 2010 1 Abstract The role of focus cues in stereopsis, and the development of a novel volumetric display by David Morris Hoffman Doctor of Philosophy in Vision Science University of California, Berkeley Professor Martin S. Banks, Chair Typical stereoscopic displays produce a vivid impression of depth by presenting each eye with its own image on a flat display screen. This technique produces many depth signals (including disparity) that are consistent with a 3-dimensional (3d) scene; however, by using a flat display panel, focus information will be incorrect. The accommodation distance to the simulated objects will be at the screen distance, and blur will be inconsistent with the simulated depth. In this thesis I will described several studies investigating the importance of focus cues for binocular vision. These studies reveal that there are a number of benefits to presenting correct focus information in a stereoscopic display, such as making it easier to fuse a binocular image, reducing visual fatigue, mitigating disparity scaling errors, and helping to resolve the binocular correspondence problem. For each of these problems, I discuss the theory for how focus cues could be an important factor, and then present psychophysical data showing that indeed focus cues do make a difference.
    [Show full text]
  • Forward and Inward Movement of the Ciliary Muscle Apex with Accommodation in Adults
    Forward and Inward Movement of the Ciliary Muscle Apex with Accommodation in Adults THESIS Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Science in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University By Trang Pham Prosak Graduate Program in Vision Science The Ohio State University 2014 Master's Examination Committee: Melissa D. Bailey, OD, PhD, Advisor Donald O. Mutti, OD, PhD Marjean Kulp, OD, PhD Copyright by Trang Pham Prosak 2014 Abstract Purpose: to study the inward and forward movement of the ciliary muscle during accommodation and to investigate the effects of one hour of reading on the ciliary muscle behavior in young adults. Methods: Subjects included 23 young adults with a mean age of 23.7 ± 1.9 years. Images of the temporal ciliary muscle of the right eye were obtained using the Visante™ Anterior Segment Ocular Coherence Tomography while accommodative response was monitored simultaneously by the Power-Refractor. Four images were taken at each accommodative response level (0, 4.0 and 6.0 D) before and after one hour of reading. Ciliary muscle thickness was measured at every 0.25 mm posterior to the scleral spur. SSMAX, which is the distance between scleral spur and the thickest point of the muscle (CMTMAX), was also measured. The change in the ciliary muscle thickness and SSMAX with accommodation from 0 to 4.0 D and 0 to 6.0 D was calculated. Paired t-tests were used to determine if the ciliary muscle thickness and SSMAX for the 4.0 and 6.0 diopters of accommodative response were different after one hour of reading.
    [Show full text]
  • Binocular Functions Arvind Kumar Morya, Kanchan Solanki, Sahil Bhandari and Anushree Naidu
    Chapter Binocular Functions Arvind Kumar Morya, Kanchan Solanki, Sahil Bhandari and Anushree Naidu Abstract Binocular single vision is the ability to use both eyes simultaneously so that each eye contributes to a common single perception. Normal binocular single vision occurs with bifoveal fixation and normal retinal correspondence in everyday sight. There are various anatomical and physiological factors concerned in the develop- ment of Binocular vision. The development of binocular function starts at 6 weeks and is completed by 6 months. Any obstacles, sensory, motor, or central, in the flex pathway is likely to hamper the development of binocular vision. The presence of these obstacles gives rise to various sensory adaptations to binocular dysfunction. Clinically the tests used can be based on either of the two principles: (A) assessment of relationship between the fovea of the fixing eye and the retinal area stimulated in the squinting eye, viz. Bagolini striated glasses test, red filter test, synoptophore using SMP slides for measuring the objective and subjective angles, and Worth 4-dot test; and (B) Assessment of the visual directions of the two foveae, viz. after image test (Hering Bielschowsky); and Cuppers binocular visuoscopy test (foveo- foveal test of Cuppers). Anomalies of binocular vision results in confusion, diplopia, which leads to suppression, eccentric fixation, anomalous retinal correspondence, and amblyopia. Keywords: accommodation, binocular vision, stereopsis 1. Introduction Binocular single vision is the ability of both eyes to contribute to simultaneous perception by contemporaneous use of each of them. Normal binocular single vision results due to the presence of bifoveal fixation and normal retinal correspondence and vice versa.
    [Show full text]
  • Eye Anatomy & Function
    Eye Anatomy & Function Dr Jie Zhang PhD Senior Research Fellow Associate Professor Bruce Hadden LLD, FRACS, FRANZCO External ocular appearance Key Eye Functions • Transmits and refracts light from the front to the back of the eye • Transparent light path • Includes structures that bend light (refract) • Converts light energy into action potentials transmitted to brain Layers and chambers of the eye Anterior Chamber Fibrous Tunic Posterior Chamber Vascular Tunic Nervous Tunic Vitreous Chamber Defining ocular segments Anterior Posterior segment: segment: Structures in front of Vitreous, vitreous: retina, Cornea, choroid, iris, optic nerve ciliary body, and lens Tear film layers and functions • Oil layer: • Meibomian glands • Prevents evaporation • Water layer: • Lacrimal glands • Lubricates • Allows blinking • Washes away debris • Forms smooth surface • Mucin layer: • Goblet cells of conjunctiva • Attaches tear film to eye • Spreads water evenly Cornea functions • Transmits light, transparent • Collagen and matrix • Aligned • Spacing • Relative dehydration is maintained by endothelial cells • No blood vessels • Refracts light +40-44 dioptres • Curvature • Has different refractive index from air Corneal anatomy Epithelium: Barrier to fluid loss and pathogen penetration Stroma: Collagen, ECM, keratocytes Endothelium: Maintains relative dehydration Dense innervation: Most sensitive organ in the body Immune privilege Rapid tearing reflex The cornea: Transmits light Refracts lights Protects ocular interior Structure of the crystalline lens Crystalline Lens: Structure and function • Composed of α, β, and γ crystallins (water soluble proteins) • Transmission of light • Refraction of light. +17 dioptres • Variable refraction of light - accommodation Cornea 2/3rd and lens 1/3rd refracting power Accommodation Far objects: Near objects: Ciliary muscle relaxed Ciliary muscle contracts ( diameter) (↓ diameter) Zonules tight Zonules relaxed Lens↑ flatter i.e.
    [Show full text]
  • Accommodation Without Feedback Suggests Directional Signals Specify Ocular Focus PHILIP B
    Vision Res., Vol. 37, No. 18, pp. 2511-2526, 1997 Pergamon © 1997 Elsevier Science Ltd. All fights reserved PII: S0042-6989(97)00056-4 Printed in Great Britain 0042-6989/97 $17.00 + 0.00 Accommodation Without Feedback Suggests Directional Signals Specify Ocular Focus PHILIP B. KRUGER,*qt STEVEN MATHEWS,t MILTON KATZ,* KARAN R. AGGARWALA,* SUJATA NOWBOTSING* Received 4 September 1996; in revised form 27 February 1997 Accommodation was monitored continuously under open-loop conditions while subjects viewed a sinusoidally oscillating sine-wave grating (0.2 Hz; _+1 D; 2.7 c/d; 0.56 contrast) in a Badal optometer. The target was illuminated by monochromatic fight (590 nm) or white light (3000 K) with longitudinal chromatic aberration (LCA) normal, doubled, neutralized and reversed. Subjects (12) accommodated well in white fight with LCA normal and doubled (mean gains = 0.85 and 0.94), gain was reduced in the neutralized condition (0.54), in monochromatic light (0.43), and especially when LCA was reversed (0.30). The results suggest that accommodation responds to changes in the relative contrast of spectral components of the retinal image and perhaps to the vergence of fight. © 1997 Elsevier Science Ltd Accommodation Chromatic aberration Focus Open-loop Stiles~Crawford INTRODUCTION movements (<10 arc sec) and assumes that the peak of Reflex accommodation is modeled routinely as a closed- the Stiles-Crawford function is at the center of the pupil. loop negative feedback system that operates to maximize An effect similar to that described by Fincham occurs or optimize luminance contrast of the retinal image without the need for off-axis eye movements if the peak (Heath, 1956; Alpern, 1958; Fender, 1964; Toates, 1972; of the Stiles-Crawford function is decentered toward one Owens, 1980; Manny & Banks, 1984; Raymond et al., side of the pupil (Westheimer, 1968).
    [Show full text]
  • UC Berkeley UC Berkeley Electronic Theses and Dissertations
    UC Berkeley UC Berkeley Electronic Theses and Dissertations Title The role of focus cues in stereopsis, and the development of a novel volumetric display Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/2nh189d9 Author Hoffman, David Morris Publication Date 2010 Peer reviewed|Thesis/dissertation eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California The role of focus cues in stereopsis, and the development of a novel volumetric display by David Morris Hoffman A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Vision Science in the GRADUATE DIVISION of the UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA, BERKELEY Committee in charge: Professor Martin S. Banks, Chair Professor Austin Roorda Professor John Canny Spring 2010 1 Abstract The role of focus cues in stereopsis, and the development of a novel volumetric display by David Morris Hoffman Doctor of Philosophy in Vision Science University of California, Berkeley Professor Martin S. Banks, Chair Typical stereoscopic displays produce a vivid impression of depth by presenting each eye with its own image on a flat display screen. This technique produces many depth signals (including disparity) that are consistent with a 3-dimensional (3d) scene; however, by using a flat display panel, focus information will be incorrect. The accommodation distance to the simulated objects will be at the screen distance, and blur will be inconsistent with the simulated depth. In this thesis I will described several studies investigating the importance of focus cues for binocular vision. These studies reveal that there are a number of benefits to presenting correct focus information in a stereoscopic display, such as making it easier to fuse a binocular image, reducing visual fatigue, mitigating disparity scaling errors, and helping to resolve the binocular correspondence problem.
    [Show full text]
  • Correlation Between AC/A Ratio and Ciliary Muscle Morphology in School-Age Children
    Correlation Between AC/A Ratio and Ciliary Muscle Morphology in School-Age Children THESIS Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Science in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University By Haind Fadel, M.D. Graduate Program in Vision Science The Ohio State University 2011 Master's Examination Committee: Melissa D. Bailey, OD PhD and Jeffrey J. Walline, OD PhD, Advisors Lisa A. Jones-Jordan, PhD Copyright by Haind Fadel, MD 2011 Abstract Purpose: To examine how the morphology of the ciliary muscle varies as a function of AC/A ratio and to investigate the difference between the thickness of the ciliary muscle during accommodation and during cycloplegia. Methods: Measurements were performed on the right eye only of thirty children aged 6 to 12 years. Height and weight were measured. Accommodative response was measured by autorefraction through habitual correction. Axial length was measured with the IOLMaster. Temporal ciliary muscle images from anterior segment Vistante OCT at 1mm (CMT1), 2mm (CMT2), and 3mm (CMT3) posterior to the scleral spur at 0D and 4D stimulus levels and under cycloplegia were measured four times each. Accommodative response and pupil size data were simultaneously recorded by the PowerRefractor during ciliary muscle measurements. Result: There were no significant correlations between AC/A ratio and the changes in ciliary muscle thicknesses, CMT1 (P= 0.8), CMT2 (P= 0.8), CMT3 (P=0.9), or CMT MAX (P=0.9). Axial length has an inverse correlation with the changes in ciliary muscle thicknesses at CMT1 (P=0.004), CMT2 (P=0.04), and CMT MAX (P= 0.03), but not with CMT3 (P= 0.07).
    [Show full text]
  • Comparative Neuro-Ophthalmology
    Comparative Neuro-Ophthalmology Michael Davidson, D.V.M. Diplomate, American College of Veterinary Ophthalmologists Department of Clinical Sciences College of Veterinary Medicine North Carolina State University Raleigh, North Carolina, USA Suggested Reading Webb A, Cullen C. Neuro-ophthalmology. In Veterinary Ophthalmology. (5th ed) Gelatt KN ed. Wiley-Blackwell, Ames, Iowa, 1820-1896, 2013. *Shamir MH, Ofri R. Comparative Neuro-ophthalmology. In Veterinary Ophthalmology, (4th ed). Gelatt KN ed. Blackwell Publishing, Oxford, UK, 1406-1469, 2007. Visual System. In Veterinary Neuroanatomy and Clinical Neurology. Delahunta and Glass ed. Saunders and Elsevier. 2009, p 389. Walsh and Hoyt’s Clinical Neuro-Ophthalmology: The Essentials. Miller NR, Newman NJ, eds. 7th Edition Williams and Wilkins, Philadelphia, 2007. Scagliotti, RH. Comparative Neuro-ophthalmology. In Veterinary Ophthalmology (3rd ed) Gelatt KN ed. Lippincott, Williams and Wilkins, Philadelphia, 1307-1400, 1999. Neuroophthalmology I. The Pupil II. Visual Pathways III. Ocular Movement IV. Eyelid and Nictitans Movement V. Lacrimation Anatomic Pathways: Afferent Arm of PLR Retina Retina Optic Optic nerve Nerve Optic chiasm Optic tract Optic Chiasm Optic Tract Pretectal nuclei CNIII parasympathetic LGB PTN nuclei CNIII parasympathetic nuclei AFFERENT ARM OF PLR: RETINA: rod/cone photoreceptors retinal bipolar cells retinal ganglion cells and axons intrinsically photosensitive RGCs* Afferent Arm of PLR: Optic Chiasm optic nerve fibers converge at chiasm species dependent
    [Show full text]