Cultural Evolution of Music

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Cultural Evolution of Music 1 Cultural Evolution of Music 2 Patrick E. Savage 3 Keio University, Shonan Fujisawa Campua 4 Abstract. The concept of cultural evolution was fundamental to the foundation of 5 academic musicology and the subfield of comparative musicology, but largely 6 disappeared from discussion after World War II, despite a recent resurgence of 7 interest in cultural evolution in other fields. In this article, I draw on recent advances 8 in the scientific understanding of cultural evolution to clarify persistent 9 misconceptions about the roles of genes and progress in musical evolution and 10 review literature relevant to musical evolution ranging from macroevolution of 11 global song-style to microevolution of tune families. I also address criticisms 12 regarding issues of musical agency, meaning, and reductionism, and highlight 13 potential applications including music copyright, education, and sustainability. 14 While cultural evolution will never explain all aspects of music in culture, it offers 15 a useful theoretical framework for understanding diversity and change in the 16 world’s music. 17 ----------- 18 The concept of evolution played a central role during the formation of academic 19 musicology in the late nineteenth century (Adler 1885/1981; Rehding 2000). 20 During the twentieth century, theoretical and political implications of evolution 21 were heavily debated, leading evolution to go out of favor in musicology and 22 cultural anthropology (Carneiro 2003). In the twenty first century, refined concepts 23 of biological evolution were reintroduced to musicology through the work of 24 psychologists of music to the extent that the biological evolution of the capacity to 25 make and experience music ("evolution of musicality") has returned as an important 26 topic of contemporary musicological research (Wallin, Merker, and Brown 2000; 27 Huron 2006; Patel 2008; Lawson 2012; Tomlinson 2013; Honing 2018). Yet the 28 concept of cultural evolution of music itself ("musical evolution") remains largely 29 undeveloped by musicologists, despite an explosion of recent research on cultural 30 evolution in related fields such as linguistics. This absence has been especially 31 prominent in ethnomusicology, but is also observable in historical musicology and 32 other subfields of musicology1. 33 One major exception was the two-volume special edition of The World of 34 Music devoted to critical analysis of Victor Grauer's (2006) essay entitled "Echoes 35 of Our Forgotten Ancestors" (later expanded into book form in Grauer 2011). 36 Grauer proposed that the evolution and global dispersal of human song-style 37 parallels the evolution and dispersal of anatomically modern humans out of Africa, 38 and that certain groups of contemporary African hunter-gatherers retain the 39 ancestral singing style shared by all humans tens of thousands of years ago. The 40 two evolutionary biologists contributing to this publication found the concept of 41 musical evolution self-evident enough that they simply opened their contribution 42 by stating: "Songs, like genes and languages, evolve" (Leroi and Swire 2006:43). 43 However, the musicologists displayed much concern and some confusion over the 44 concept of cultural evolution. 45 My goal in this article is to clarify some of these issues in terms of the 46 definitions, assumptions, and implications involved in studying the cultural 47 evolution of music to show how cultural evolutionary theory can benefit 48 musicology in a variety of ways. I will begin with a brief overview of cultural 49 evolution in general, move to cultural evolution of music in particular, and then end 50 by addressing some potential applications and criticisms. Because this article is 51 aimed both at musicologists with limited knowledge of cultural evolution and at 52 cultural evolutionists with limited knowledge of music, I have included some 53 discussion that may seem obvious to some readers but not others. 54 What is “evolution”? 55 Although the term “evolution” is often assumed to refer to directional 56 progress and/or to require a genetic basis, neither genes nor progress are included 57 in contemporary definitions of evolution. Furthermore, while it is true that the 58 discovery of genes and the precise molecular mechanisms by which they change 59 revolutionized evolutionary biology, Darwin formulated his theory of evolution 60 without the concept of genes. 61 Instead of genes, Darwin's theory of evolution by natural selection 62 contained three key requirements: 1) there must be variation among individuals; 2) 63 variation must be inherited via intergenerational transmission; 3) certain variants 64 must be more likely to be inherited than others due to competitive selection (Darwin 65 1859). These principles apply equally to biological and cultural evolution (Mesoudi 66 2011). 67 Evolution did often come to be defined in purely genetic terms during the 68 twentieth century, but recent advances in our understanding of cultural evolution, 69 epigenetics, and ecology have led to a new inclusive definition of evolution as: 70 'the process by which the frequencies of variants in a population change 71 over time', where the word ‘variants’ replaces the word ‘genes’ in order to 72 include any inherited information….In particular, this…should include 73 cultural inheritance. (Danchin et al. 2011:483-484) 2 74 Note that there is nothing about progress or direction contained in Danchin et al.'s 75 scholarly definition quoted above: evolution simply refers to changes in the 76 frequencies of heritable variants. These changes can be in the direction of simple 77 to complex - and it is possible that there may be a general trend towards complexity 78 (McShea and Brandon 2010; Currie and Mace 2011) - but the reverse is also 79 possible, as are non-directional changes with little or no functional consequences 80 (Nei, Suzuki, and Nozawa 2010). 81 Does culture “evolve”? 82 From the time Darwin (1859) first proposed that his theory of evolution 83 explained “The Origin of Species”, scholars immediately tried to apply it to explain 84 the origin of culture. Indeed, Darwin himself explicitly argued that language and 85 species evolution were "curiously parallel…the survival or preservation of certain 86 favoured words in the struggle for existence is natural selection" (Darwin 1871:89- 87 90). Scholars of cultural evolution have tabulated a number of such curious 88 parallels, to which I have added musical examples (Table 1). 89 3 90 Table 1. A simplified comparison of biological, linguistic, and musical 91 evolution. Biological evolution and linguistic evolution columns are adapted 92 from Atkinson and Gray (2005:514) and Pagel (2016:2). Biological evolution Linguistic Musical evolution evolution Discrete heritable units (e.g., Discrete heritable Discrete heritable amino acids, genes) units (e.g., units (e.g., notes, phonemes, words) phrases) DNA copying Teaching, learning Teaching, learning and imitation and imitation Mutation Mistakes, sound Mistakes, changes embellishment, composition Homology Lexical cognates, Melodic cognates, language families tune families Natural selection Social selection Selection by and trends audience, performers, judges, consumers, etc. Hybridization (e.g., horse Creoles (e.g., Syncretic music (e.g., with zebra) Surinamese) Métis fiddle music) Fossils Ancient texts Ancient notation, audio/video recordings Extinction Language death Repertoire loss 93 94 Theories about cultural evolution quickly adopted assumptions about 95 progress (e.g., Spencer 1875) linked with attempts to legitimize ideologies of 96 Western superiority and justify the oppression of the weak by the powerful as 97 survival of the fittest (Hofstadter 1955; Laland and Brown 2011; Stocking 1982)2. 98 It is no accident that Zallinger's famous March of Progress drawing showed a 99 gradual lightening of the skin from dark-skinned, ape-like ancestors to light- 100 skinned humans: evolution was used to justify scientific racism by eugenicists 4 101 (Gould 1989). Although both the lightening of skin and the linear progression from 102 ape to man are inaccurate (Gould 1989), this image unfortunately remains 103 extremely enduring and is commonly adapted to represent all kinds of evolution, 104 including musical evolution (see Figure 1). 105 106 Figure 1. An example of an inaccurate but widespread representation of 107 evolution as a linear march of progress. Source: 108 http://www.mandolincafe.com/archives/spoof.html (retrieved October 14, 109 2016) 110 Ideas of linear progress through a series of fixed stages continued to 111 dominate cultural evolution for over a century (see Carneiro 2003 for an in-depth 112 review). For example, Morgan (1877) proposed a universal set of stages of cultural 113 evolution, beginning with different gradations of “savagery” and “barbarism” until 114 societies reached “civilization”. Around the 1930s, scholars of biological evolution 115 such as Sewall Wright and Ronald Fisher began to develop more sophisticated 116 concepts based on careful modeling and measurement of changing gene frequencies, 117 which became known as the “Modern Synthesis”. However, cultural evolution 118 maintained an emphasis on theories of progress for many decades after the Modern 119 Synthesis (e.g., White 1959; Sahlins and Service 1960), even as contradictory 120 evidence was mounting from ethnographic fieldwork pioneered by Franz Boas 121 (1940) and his disciples. 5 122 It was not until late in the 20th century that several teams of scholars
Recommended publications
  • Linguistics for the Use of African History and the Comparative Study of Bantu Pottery Vocabulary
    LINGUISTICS FOR THE USE OF AFRICAN HISTORY AND THE COMPARATIVE STUDY OF BANTU POTTERY VOCABULARY Koen Bostoen Université Libre de Bruxelles1 Royal Museum for Central Africa Tervuren 1. Introduction Ever since African historical linguistics emerged in the 19th century, it has served a double purpose. It has not only been practiced with the aim of studying language evolution, its methods have also been put to use for the reconstruction of human history. The promotion of linguistics to one of the key disciplines of African historiography is an inevitable consequence of the lack of ancient written records in sub-Saharan Africa. Scholars of the African past generally fall back on two kinds of linguistic research: linguistic classifi- cation and linguistic reconstruction. The aim of this paper is to present a con- cise application of both disciplines to the field of Bantu linguistics and to offer two interesting comparative case studies in the field of Bantu pottery vocabulary. The diachronic analysis of this lexical domain constitutes a promising field for interdisciplinary historical research. At the same time, the examples presented here urge history scholars to be cautious in the applica- tion of words-and-things studies for the use of historical reconstruction. The neglect of diachronic semantic evolutions and the impact of ancient lexical copies may lead to oversimplified and hence false historical conclusions. 2. Bantu languages and the synchronic nature of historical linguistics Exact estimations being complicated by the lack of good descriptive ma- terial, the Bantu languages are believed to number at present between 400 and 600. They are spoken in almost half of all sub-Saharan countries: Camer- 1 My acknowledgement goes to Yvonne Bastin, Claire Grégoire, Jacqueline Renard, Ellen Vandendorpe and Annemie Van Geldre who assisted me in the preparation of this paper.
    [Show full text]
  • Full Programme
    Full Programme www.bristol.ac.uk/ehbea2014 1 Contents Welcome 3 Key Events 5 Schedule at a Glance 6 Plenary Abstracts 9 Talk Abstracts 15 Poster Abstracts 61 2 Welcome Welcome to the 9th Annual Conference of the European Human Behaviour and Evolution Association! We are delighted to welcome you to Bristol, a unique and bustling city in South West England. The conference venue At-Bristol is located in the central area, by the historic harbourside. Most of the attractions are within walking distance: Bristol Aquarium, Brunel’s ss Great Britain, M Shed (museum of Bristol’s history), Bristol Cathedral, Bristol Shopping Quarter, Bristol Museum and Art Gallery, and University of Bristol. The Bristol Zoo Gardens and the world- famous Clifton Suspension Bridge are a 40-minute walk (or a short bus ride) from the centre. There are also many green spaces nearby: College Green, Queen Square, Brandon Hill, and Castle Park. For the conference, we are delighted to welcome our plenary speakers Russell Gray, Martie Haselton, Daniel Hruschka, Annette Karmiloff-Smith, and Samir Okasha. We are also excited to have as our sixth plenary speaker, the 2014 New Investigator Award winner Willem Frankenhuis. In addition, we have 46 talks and 104 posters spanning a wide range of topics and approaches from researchers around the world. On Monday morning, Prof Nick Lieven (Pro Vice-Chancellor, University of Bristol) will open the conference, which will be followed by the first plenary. The Poster Session will take place on Monday evening, but we invite the presenters to have their posters displayed for the duration of the conference starting Sunday evening.
    [Show full text]
  • Human Evolution Timeline 1
    Name: Human Origins Web Inquiry 1. Got to the website: http://humanorigins.si.edu/evidence 2. Read the paragraph under “Evidence of Evolution.” 3. In the maroon box on the left, click on “Timeline Interactive” 4. Start by clicking on the red bands at the bottom of the timeline, beginning with Homo sapiens and the rest of the Hominids. Read the information boxes that pop up when you click. 5. When you come across a species you find particularly interesting, record that information (including dates) in the box below. Record facts of at least 5 interesting hominid species, including Homo sapiens. 6. Then begin to explore the rest of the timeline. As you go, record at least 5 other interesting species. 7. Use the magnifier tool at the bottom of the timeline. Click on “color key” to identify the different color dots. Each dot represents a specific piece of evidence scientists have discovered in their study of evolution. 8. Click on various colored dots to learn about tools, events, geology, climate information, behavior, and skeletal adaptations discovered by scientists that add to the evidence for human evolution. Record 7 pieces of evidence you find interesting or particularly important in the box on the back. Make sure to put down different types of evidence (different colors). 9. Take 15-20 minutes to explore the whole timeline and record interesting facts. 10. Build your own timeline on the next page, filling in the interesting species and evidence you recorded in their proper place in the timeline. Hominids Species Dates Interesting fact(s) Homo sapiens Name: Evidence Type of Date Interesting fact(s) Evidence Name: Human Evolution Timeline 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Contesting Regimes of Variation: Critical Groundwork for Pedagogies of Mobile Experience and Restorative Justice
    Robert W. Train Sonoma State University, California CONTESTING REGIMES OF VARIATION: CRITICAL GROUNDWORK FOR PEDAGOGIES OF MOBILE EXPERIENCE AND RESTORATIVE JUSTICE Abstract: This paper examines from a critical transdisciplinary perspective the concept of variation and its fraught binary association with standard language as part of the conceptual toolbox and vocabulary for language educators and researchers. “Variation” is shown to be imbricated a historically-contingent metadiscursive regime in language study as scientific description and education supporting problematic speaker identities (e.g., “non/native”, “heritage”, “foreign”) around an ideology of reduction through which complex sociolinguistic and sociocultural spaces of diversity and variability have been reduced to the “problem” of governing people and spaces legitimated and embodied in idealized teachers and learners of languages invented as the “zero degree of observation” (Castro-Gómez 2005; Mignolo 2011) in ongoing contexts of Western modernity and coloniality. This paper explores how regimes of variation have been constructed in a “sociolinguistics of distribution” (Blommaert 2010) constituted around the delimitation of borders—linguistic, temporal, social and territorial—rather than a “sociolinguistics of mobility” focused on interrogating and problematizing the validity and relevance of those borders in a world characterized by diverse transcultural and translingual experiences of human flow and migration. This paper reframes “variation” as mobile modes-of-experiencing- the-world in order to expand the critical, historical, and ethical vocabularies and knowledge base of language educators and lay the groundwork for pedagogies of experience that impact human lives in the service of restorative social justice. Keywords: metadiscursive regimes w sociolinguistic variation w standard language w sociolinguistics of mobility w pedagogies of experience Train, Robert W.
    [Show full text]
  • A Different Kind of Animal: How Culture
    © Copyright, Princeton University Press. No part of this book may be distributed, posted, or reproduced in any form by digital or mechanical means without prior written permission of the publisher. INTRODUCTION Stephen Macedo What makes humans special? Is it, as many have argued, our superior intelligence that sets us apart from other species? In the lectures and discussions that follow, Robert Boyd, a distinguished professor of human evolution and social change, refines the question and rejects the common answer. Putting aside the more familiar question of human unique- ness, Boyd asks why humans so exceed other species when it comes to broad indices of ecological success such as our ability to adapt to and thrive in such a wide variety of hab- itats across the globe. Ten thousand years ago, humans al- ready occupied the entire globe except Antarctica and a few remote islands. No other species comes close. What explains our outlier status if not our “big brains”? Humans adapt to a vast variety of changing environments not mainly by applying individual intelligence to solve prob- lems, but rather via “cumulative cultural adaptation” and, over the longer term, Darwinian selection among cultures with different social norms and moral values. Not only are humans part of the natural world, argues Boyd, but human culture is part of the natural world. Culture makes us “a different kind of animal,” and “culture is as much a part of human biology as our peculiar pelvis or the thick enamel that covers our molars.” With his many coauthors, especially Peter Richerson, Robert Boyd has for three decades pioneered an important approach to the study of human evolution that focuses on the population dynamics of culturally transmitted informa- tion.
    [Show full text]
  • The Adaptive Significance of Human Language
    University of Tennessee, Knoxville TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange Senior Thesis Projects, 1993-2002 College Scholars 2000 The Adaptive Significance of Human Language Nathan Oesch Follow this and additional works at: https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_interstp2 Recommended Citation Oesch, Nathan, "The Adaptive Significance of Human Language" (2000). Senior Thesis Projects, 1993-2002. https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_interstp2/52 This Project is brought to you for free and open access by the College Scholars at TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. It has been accepted for inclusion in Senior Thesis Projects, 1993-2002 by an authorized administrator of TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. For more information, please contact [email protected]. The Adaptive Significance of Human Language Nathan Oesch Department of Psychology University of Tennessee, Knoxville noesch @ utk. edu Abstract Many experts have argued that human language is fundamentally incompatible with the principles of traditional Darwinian evolutionary theory. According to conventional Darwinian explanations, specific traits evolved among species according to gradual and incremental genetic changes, each of which that were in some way so favorable to the survival and reproduction of ancestral generations that they were ultimately preserved within successive generations of those species. Human language, it has been said, is simply to complex to be explained as a result of Darwinian explanations, since each successive step in the evolution of language would confer no obvious survival benefits to its recipients. According to this idea, language is such an "all-or­ none system," that it could not possibly have existed in any immediately beneficial intermediate forms and thus could not have evolved according to conventional Darwinian modes of explanation.
    [Show full text]
  • Cultural Group Selection Plays an Essential Role in Explaining Human Cooperation: a Sketch of the Evidence
    BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2016), Page 1 of 68 doi:10.1017/S0140525X1400106X, e30 Cultural group selection plays an essential role in explaining human cooperation: A sketch of the evidence Peter Richerson Emily K. Newton Department of Environmental Science and Policy, University of California– Department of Psychology, Dominican University of California, San Rafael, CA Davis, Davis, CA 95616 94901 [email protected] [email protected] http://emilyknewton.weebly.com/ www.des.ucdavis.edu/faculty/richerson/richerson.htm Nicole Naar Ryan Baldini Department of Anthropology, University of California–Davis, Graduate Group in Ecology, University of California–Davis, Davis, CA 95616 Davis, CA 95616 [email protected] https://sites.google.com/site/ryanbaldini/ [email protected] Adrian V. Bell Lesley Newson Department of Anthropology, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT 84112 Department of Environmental Science and Policy, University of California– [email protected] http://adrianbell.wordpress.com/ Davis, Davis, CA 95616 [email protected] [email protected] Kathryn Demps https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Lesley_Newson/ Department of Anthropology, Boise State University, Boise, ID 83725 [email protected] Cody Ross http://sspa.boisestate.edu/anthropology/faculty-and-staff/kathryn- Santa Fe Institute, Santa Fe, NM 87501 demps/ [email protected] http://scholar.google.com/citations?user=xSugEskAAAAJ Karl Frost Graduate Group in Ecology, University of California–Davis, Davis, CA 95616 Paul E. Smaldino [email protected] https://sites.google.com/site/karljosephfrost/ Department of Anthropology, University of California–Davis, Davis, CA 95616 [email protected] http://www.smaldino.com/ Vicken Hillis Department of Environmental Science and Policy, University of California– Timothy M.
    [Show full text]
  • Bálint Veres Moholy-Nagy University of Art&Design [email protected]
    TACTILE TACTICS IN ϮϭST CENTURY CULTURAL DISPLAYS ϭ͘ ^ƚĂƌƚŝŶŐ ƉŽŝŶƚ͗ an example ĄůŝŶƚsĞƌĞƐ Moholy-Nagy University of Art&Design [email protected] In September 2013, during the Budapest Design Week, together with two of my colleagues, I had the privilege to encourage a team of art students to perform an exhi- ABSTRACT: There is a long tradition to see museums and similar cultural displays as sites of knowing and self- bition intervention, hosted by the Museum of Ethnogra- education; and also as tools of political and ideological phy, an initiative that is still rare in the Hungarian tuition, indoctrination indeed. The critical approach of 1 new museology in the late 20th century launched a museum practice. The museum invited us to be com- systematic revision of the social and epistemological role pletely free in our approach to the task, and at first we museums play in contemporary culture. New museology highly increased, or at least required, the self-reflexivity deemed it as a double task. On the one hand, there was of cultural displays, however, an increase in reflexivity a chronological exhibition of partly folk, partly artistic, does not involve, in a self-evident way, an increase of intensified experience, which became crucial to our professional and industrial exhibition materials, under contemporary life, especially in the perspective of the title of The living tradition of ryijy – Finnish rugs from somaesthetics. Thus, more recent museological and curatorial approaches, oriented according to the a private collection. On the other hand, the bustle of the corporeal turn in philosophy and social sciences, Design Week and the freshness of art students’ creativity emphasize the effects and consequences of the sensorial range in use within cultural displays.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolutionary Musicology
    I THE BEGINNING 1 An Introduction to Evolutionary Musicology Steven Brown, Björn Merker, and Nils L. Wallin Abstract In this introduction to the new field of evolutionary musicology, we see that the study of music origins provides a fresh and exciting approach to the under- standing of human evolution, a topic that so far has been dominated by a focus on language evolution. The language-centered view of humanity has to be expanded to include music, first, because the evolution of language is highly inter- twined with the evolution of music, and, second, because music provides a spe- cific and direct means of exploring the evolution of human social structure, group function, and cultural behavior. Music making is the quintessential human cul- tural activity, and music is an ubiquitous element in all cultures large and small. The study of music evolution promises to shed light on such important issues as evolution of the hominid vocal tract; the structure of acoustic-communication signals; human group structure; division of labor at the group level; the capacity for designing and using tools; symbolic gesturing; localization and lateralization of brain function; melody and rhythm in speech; the phrase-structure of lan- guage; parent-infant communication; emotional and behavioral manipulation through sound; interpersonal bonding and synchronization mechanisms; self- expression and catharsis; creativity and aesthetic expression; the human affinity for the spiritual and the mystical; and finally, of course, the universal human attachment to music
    [Show full text]
  • Intersubjectivity Evolved to Fit the Brain, but Grammar Co
    BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2008) 31, 489–558 Printed in the United States of America doi:10.1017/S0140525X08004998 Language as shaped by the brain Morten H. Christiansen Department of Psychology, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853, and Santa Fe Institute, Santa Fe, NM 87501 [email protected] http://www.psych.cornell.edu/people/Faculty/mhc27.html Nick Chater Division of Psychology and Language Sciences, University College London, London, WC1E 6BT, United Kingdom [email protected] http://www.psychol.ucl.ac.uk/people/profiles/chater_nick.htm Abstract: It is widely assumed that human learning and the structure of human languages are intimately related. This relationship is frequently suggested to derive from a language-specific biological endowment, which encodes universal, but communicatively arbitrary, principles of language structure (a Universal Grammar or UG). How might such a UG have evolved? We argue that UG could not have arisen either by biological adaptation or non-adaptationist genetic processes, resulting in a logical problem of language evolution. Specifically, as the processes of language change are much more rapid than processes of genetic change, language constitutes a “moving target” both over time and across different human populations, and, hence, cannot provide a stable environment to which language genes could have adapted. We conclude that a biologically determined UG is not evolutionarily viable. Instead, the original motivation for UG – the mesh between learners and languages – arises because language has been shaped to fit the human brain, rather than vice versa. Following Darwin, we view language itself as a complex and interdependent “organism,” which evolves under selectional pressures from human learning and processing mechanisms.
    [Show full text]
  • A 130,000-Year-Old Archaeological Site in Southern California, USA Steven R
    LETTER doi:10.1038/nature22065 A 130,000-year-old archaeological site in southern California, USA Steven R. Holen1,2, Thomas A. Deméré2, Daniel C. Fisher3,4, Richard Fullagar5, James B. Paces6, George T. Jefferson7, Jared M. Beeton8, Richard A. Cerutti2, Adam N. Rountrey3, Lawrence Vescera7 & Kathleen A. Holen1,2 The earliest dispersal of humans into North America is a fragments (Extended Data Fig. 2 and Supplementary Table 5). One contentious subject, and proposed early sites are required to meet tusk was found lying horizontally, and the other was oriented vertically the following criteria for acceptance: (1) archaeological evidence with the distal portion penetrating the underlying strata. Femora were is found in a clearly defined and undisturbed geologic context; represented by detached femoral heads and spiral-fractured diaphyseal (2) age is determined by reliable radiometric dating; (3) multiple fragments that had been broken while fresh14 (Fig. 2 and Extended lines of evidence from interdisciplinary studies provide consistent Data Figs 3a, b, 4a–e), whereas several fragile ribs and vertebrae were results; and (4) unquestionable artefacts are found in primary unbroken. context1,2. Here we describe the Cerutti Mastodon (CM) site, an Two concentrations of spiral-fractured bone and broken molar frag- archaeological site from the early late Pleistocene epoch, where ments were delineated, each clustered around a separate andesite cobble in situ hammerstones and stone anvils occur in spatio-temporal (concentrations 1 and 2 (Fig. 1b, c)). Refitting bone fragments were association with fragmentary remains of a single mastodon found in concentration 1 (Fig. 1c), where both femoral heads lay adja- (Mammut americanum).
    [Show full text]
  • Biological Basics and the Economics of the Family Author(S): Donald Cox Reviewed Work(S): Source: the Journal of Economic Perspectives, Vol
    American Economic Association Biological Basics and the Economics of the Family Author(s): Donald Cox Reviewed work(s): Source: The Journal of Economic Perspectives, Vol. 21, No. 2 (Spring, 2007), pp. 91-108 Published by: American Economic Association Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/30033719 . Accessed: 31/08/2012 19:26 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp . JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. American Economic Association is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Journal of Economic Perspectives. http://www.jstor.org Journal of EconomicPerspectives-Volume 21, Number2-Spring 2007-Pages 91-108 Biological Basics and the Economics of the Family Donald Cox any economic models of the family are based on a generic "person one/person two" household or "parent-child" family, rather than their anatomicallyanatomically correct counterparts: sons and daughters, fathers and mothers, and grandfathers and grandmothers. These economic models can offer powerful insights into family behavior, but also can leave certain patterns unex- plained and neglect potentially important crosscurrents. Melding biological in- sights with family economics can cast new light on existing knowledge and open up novel paths for research. For example, study after study has found that putting family income in the hands of mothers, rather than fathers, tends to increase the consumption of children, as noted in this journal in Lundberg and Pollak (1996).
    [Show full text]