Abstracts Booklet

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Abstracts Booklet The Twentieth Manchester Phonology Meeting ! 20 H¥PbNF<K Twentieth anniversary meeting ABSTRACTS BOOKLET Thursday 24th - Saturday 26th May 2012 Held at Hulme Hall, Manchester Organised by a collaboration of phonologists at the University of Edinburgh, the University of Manchester, and elsewhere. "#$%!&''()*+!,'-+.$-%!+#*!.&%+/.,+%!0'/!.))!+#*!1.1*/%!1/*%*-+*2!.+! +#*!twentieth Manchester Phonology Meeting3!#*)2!.+!45)6*!4.))3! 7.-,#*%+*/3!$-!7.8!9:;9<! ! ! ! "#*!.&%+/.,+%!./*!.//.-=*2!$-!.)1#.&*+$,.)!'/2*/!&8!+#*!%5/-.6*!'0! +#*!>0$/%+!-.6*2?!1/*%*-+*/<! ! ! ! "#*! .&%+/.,+%! 0'/! +#*! oral paper sessions ./*! 1/*%*-+*2! 0$/%+3! 0'))'@*2!&8!+#*!.&%+/.,+%!0'/!+#*!poster paper sessions3!.-2!+#*! &''()*+!,'-,)52*%!@$+#!.&%+/.,+%!0'/!+#*!special session<!! ! ! ! ! A))! %*%%$'-%! 0'/! 1.1*/%! )$%+*2! $-! +#$%! &''()*+! @$))! +.(*! 1).,*! $-! *$+#*/!+#*!Old Dining Hall, +#*!JCR '/ +#*!dining area!$-!45)6*!4.))<! "#*!'1*-$-=!.-2!,)'%$-=!.22/*%%*%!.-2!+#*!%1*,$.)!%*%%$'-!@$))!&*! #*)2! $-! +#*! B)2! C$-$-=! 4.))<! "#*! 1./.))*)! %*%%$'-%! 0'/! +#*! '/.)! 1.1*/%!@$))!&*!#*)2!$-!+#*!B)2!C$-$-=!4.))!.-2!+#*!DEF3!.-2!+#*! 1'%+*/!%*%%$'-%!@$))!&*!#*)2!$-!+#*!2$-$-=!./*.<! ! ! ! "#*!B)2!C$-$-=!4.))!$%!$-!+#*!6.$-!45)6*!4.))!&5$)2$-=3!51%+.$/%3! .-2! G5%+! +#/'5=#! +#*! &./! ./*.! .-2! +#*! 2$-$-=! ./*.<! "#*! DEF! $%! 2'@-%+.$/%!$-!+#*!%.6*!&5$)2$-=<!H+!+.(*%!.&'5+!.!6$-5+*!+'!@.)(! 0/'6! '-*! +'! +#*! '+#*/<! "#*! final programme3! $-,)52*2! $-! 8'5/! /*=$%+/.+$'-!1.,(3!.-2!.I.$).&)*!'-!+#*!9:606!@*&%$+*3!=$I*%!+#*! 2*+.$)%!'0!@#$,#!1.1*/%!./*!$-!@#$,#!/''63!.-2!.+!@#$,#!+$6*%<! Oral papers 6KDUHG QHXWUDOL]DWLRQV ZLWKRXW VKDUHG UHSUHVHQWDWLRQV $GDP $OEULJKW 0,7 DOEULJKW#PLWHGX $ ORQJVWDQGLQJ TXHVWLRQ LV ZKHWKHU OHDUQHUV VHHN XQLILHG IHDWXUDO UHSUHVHQWDWLRQV WR FDSWXUH GLV WULEXWLRQDO SDUDOOHOV DFURVV GLIIHUHQW PDQQHUV )RU H[DPSOH /DNKRWD 6LRXDQ KDV WKUHHZD\ ODU\QJHDO FRQWUDVWV IRU ERWK VWRSV DQG IULFDWLYHV EXW WKH SKRQHWLF QDWXUH RI WKH FRQWUDVWV GLIIHU XQDVSLUDWHG >ND@ DVSLUDWHG >NހD@ HMHFWLYH >N¶D@ VWRSV YV YRLFHOHVV >[D@ YRLFHG >ܵD@ JORWWDOL]HG >[¶D@ IULFDWLYHV∼ $VSLUDWHG∼ VWRSV YRLFHG IULFDWLYHV DQG HMHFWLYHV DUH∼ EDQQHG LQ PDQ\∼ RI WKH VDPH FRQWH[WV VXFK DV ZLWKLQ FOXVWHUV >NSD@ >[SD@ YV >NހSD@ >ܵSD@ >N¶SD@ >[¶SD@ ,W LV WHPSWLQJ WR SRVLW WKDW WKHVH VHJPHQWV FRQWDLQ D FRPPRQ UHSUHVHQWDWLRQDO HOHPHQW WKDW LV EDQQHG LQ FHUWDLQ FRQWH[WV VXFK DV D VSHFLILHGPDUNHG ODU\QJHDO QRGH RU D FRPSOH[ VHJPHQW &DUWHU , VKRZ WKDW VXFK DQ DQDO\VLV LV XQZRUNDEOH IRU /DNKRWD GXH WR GLIIHUHQFHV LQ ZKLFK YDOXH DFWV DV µXQPDUNHG¶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±F +RZHYHU WKHUH LV D ODUJH FODVV RI GLV\OODELF URRWV ZLWK ³GHIHFWLYH´ ILQDO V\OODEOHV WKH YRZHO DH UHVLVWV VWUHVV WKH RQVHW PXVW EH VLPSOH DQG WKH FRQVRQDQW PXVW EH DQ XQDVSLUDWHG VWRS RU D YRLFHG IULFDWLYH G &RQWH[WXDO QHXWUDOL]DWLRQV WR RQH ODU\QJHDO YDOXH &RQWH[W 6WRSV )ULFDWLYHV D &OXVWHUV & XQDVS &N9 &Nހ9 &N¶9 YFOV &[9 &ܵ9 &N¶9 E & > VRQ@ XQDVS N79 Nހ79 N¶79 YFOV [79 ܵ79 [¶79 − & >+VRQ@ YRL J59 Nހ59 W¶59 YFOV [59 ܵ59 [¶59 F &RGDV SDVVLYH YRL &9Nࡩ &9Nހ &9N¶ YFOV &9[ &Yܵ &Y[¶ G ޖı 9 XQDVS ޖ&9ND ޖ&9NހD ޖ&9N¶D YRL ޖ&9]D ޖ&9VD ޖ&9V¶D :K\ GRHV QHXWUDOL]DWLRQ VRPHWLPHV IDYRU YRLFHOHVV IULFDWLYHV D±F DQG VRPHWLPHV YRLFHG G DQG ZK\ LV WKH GLVWULEXWLRQ RI YRLFLQJ LQ VWRSV GLIIHUHQW" 6WDWHG LQIRUPDOO\ LQ FRQ WH[WV D±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±PV YV ±PV DQG PD\ EH VHOHFWHG WR DFKLHYH GXUDWLRQDO UHGXFWLRQ 7KH VDPH SUHVVXUH VKRXOG DOVR IDYRU QHXWUDOL]DWLRQ WR YRLFHG VWRSV KRZHYHU YRLFHG VWRSV DUH VR VKRUW WKDW WKH\ RIWHQ VXUIDFH DV IULFDWLYHV RU JOLGHV 9RLFHOHVV XQDVSLUDWHG VWRSV DUH URXJKO\ FRPSDUDEOH LQ GXUDWLRQ ±PV WR YRLFHG IULFDWLYHV VR WKH QHXWUDOL]DWLRQV LQ G DFKLHYH GXUDWLRQDO UHGXFWLRQ ZKLOH FRQVLVWHQWO\ PDLQWDLQLQJ PDQQHU FRQWUDVWV 7KHVH FRQIOLFWLQJ SUHV VXUHV DUH IRUPDOL]HG LQ D JUDPPDU RI GXUDWLRQDO WDUJHWV DQG FRQWUDVW FRQVWUDLQWV .DW] 7KH FRQIOLFWLQJ SDWWHUQV LQ D±F YV G FDQQRW EH GHULYHG E\ GHFODULQJ RQH PHPEHU RI HDFK VHULHV WR EH µXQPDUNHG¶ EXW IROORZ QHDWO\ IURP FRQVWUDLQWV RQ FRQWUDVW DQG GXUDWLRQ 7R WKH H[WHQW WKDW WKH VHULHV GR SDWWHUQ WRJHWKHU LW LV EHFDXVH WKH\ DUH VXEMHFW WR VLPLODU SKRQHWLF SUHV VXUHV :H FRQFOXGH IROORZLQJ %HFNPDQ HW DO WKDW QRWKLQJ LV JDLQHG KHUH RU SHUKDSV PRUH JHQHUDOO\ E\ DVVLJQLQJ XQLILHG UHSUHVHQWDWLRQV WR VHJPHQWV ZLWK VLPLODU GLVWULEXWLRQV The Stress-Epenthesis Opacity in Palestinian Arabic Sam Alxatib, MIT, [email protected] I present a monostratal account of the stress-epenthesis interaction in Palestinian Arabic (PA). PA stress is shown in (1): it is final if the final syllable is superheavy, penultimate if the penult is heavy, and antepenultimate otherwise. FINAL STRESS PENULTIMATE STRESS ANTEPENULTIMATE STRESS ba.ka.rá:t “spools” da.ra.bát.kom “she hit you.PL” bá.la.dak “your country” ba.ra:.mí:l “barrels” mak.táb.kom “your.PL desk” lá:.ma.to “she blamed him” Epenthetic vowels (italicized), which repair certain CC combinations in codas, are invisible to stress assignment. For example, in [ní.mir.kom] “your tiger”, where the penult is heavy, penultimate stress is expected, but antepenultimate is attested; in [ka.tá.bit] “I wrote”, where antepenultimate stress is expected, stress falls on the penult. In both cases, stress is assigned as if the epenthetic vowel were invisible. In multistratal OT approaches, this invisibility is captured by assigning stress at an earlier stage than epenthesis, e.g. (in Kiparsky 2000, 2003) lexically, and post-lexically. The alternative I propose derives the invisibility from two constraints: DEP-µ and *emptyNucleus. (3) DEP-µ: every mora in the output must have a carrier in the input. (4) *emptyNucleus: syllable heads are moraic. Combining (3) and (4) eliminates the one-to-one correspondence between vowels and syllables, for it makes it impossible for epenthetic vowels to head syllables on their own; a syllable headed by an epenthetic vowel will contain either a weightless nucleus (a violation of *emptyNucleus) or a moraic epenthetic segment (a violation of DEP-µ). An output candidate that contains an illegal coda, e.g. *[nimr.kom], will therefore lose to a candidate in which epenthesis repairs the cluster, but where epenthesis fails to introduce a new syllable, producing [nimir.kom]. Stress is then correctly assigned according to the general rule described above. The ban against moraic epenthesis finds support in Gouskova and Hall’s (2007) study of epenthesis in Lebanese Arabic, a closely related dialect. Their results indicate that epenthetic vowels are shorter and more central than their non-epenthetic counterparts, suggesting that epenthetic vowels are less likely to contribute to metrical structure. I also situate the prosodic weakness of epenthetic vowels in a more general condition of PA. PA syncope targets light high vowels more frequently than low vowels. I interpret this as setting a threshold on the sonority of open syllable nuclei: since high vowels are less sonorous than low vowels (Selkirk 1984, Parker 2002), they are kept from heading light syllables. The greater weakness of epenthetic vowels restricts them, correctly, to syllables that already contain a moraic element, i.e. closed syllables. Finally, I argue that a monostratal account is necessary, based on data that are problematic for Kiparsky’s LPM-OT. The data is centered on an interaction between epenthesis and prothesis (Abu-Salim 1980). Prothesis repairs illegal phrase-medial complex onsets, e.g. /ba:b#kbi:r/ ! [ba:bikbi:r] “a large door”. In cases where the prothesis site follows an illegal complex coda, e.g. /nimr#kbi:r/ ! [nímrikbi:r], the prothetic vowel bleeds word-internal epenthesis. But PA also allows further epenthesis, as is indicated by the grammaticality of [nímirikbi:r]. If prothesis is taken to apply post-lexically (as it should, given its sensitivity to phrases), then there is no reason for further epenthesis to apply at that stage, because it would incur further DEP violations without repairing any obvious sources of markedness. I argue that a monostratal approach can be used to account for the data, by invoking constraints on identity to isolation forms, e.g. [nimir]~[nimirikbi:r] (see Kenstowicz 1996, Benua 1998). In LPM-OT, this move would amount to post-lexical constraints that nevertheless make reference to words, a logical possibility that still seems undesirable given the natural morphological divisions underlying the framework. Vowel harmony in Even: insights from a perception study
Recommended publications
  • Casting a Wider Net Over Nǁng: the Older Archival Resources Tom Güldemann1 Humboldt University Berlin and Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology Leipzig
    1 Casting a wider net over Nǁng: the older archival resources Tom Güldemann1 Humboldt University Berlin and Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology Leipzig 1 What we know and don’t know In the wake of the end of Apartheid and the emergence of a democratic South Africa the late 1990s witnessed an important (re)discovery for linguists dealing with languages in South Africa and in particular of the ‘Khoisan’2 domain. The efforts of the South African San Institute (SASI) lead to the identification of descendants of the former southern Kalahari San population (cf. Crawhall 2004, 2005), including more than 20 speakers and rememberers of their heritage language. While listed by Lewis, Simons and Fennig (2014) under Nǀu [ngh], this is called here Nǁng (see §2.4 for a detailed discussion of the variable terminology). Project Researcher(s) Main focus 1 NSF project J. Brugman, C. Collins, A. Miller, L. selected linguistic Namaseb, B. Sands topics 2 Ph.D. project at the M. Exter phonetics-phonology University of Cologne 3 ELDP project at the T. Güldemann, M. (Shaw)-Ernszt, S. natural discourse data MPI-EVA Leipzig Siegmund, A. Witzlack-Makarevich and text annotation Table 1: Recent linguistic research projects on Nǁng Since then the linguistic remnants of Nǁng have been documented according to current standards of the discipline, especially in the framework of three dedicated projects listed in Table 1. Hence, a modern description of Nǁng is taking shape, which entails revised and extended analyses compared to the limited documentation achieved by earlier studies. Some notable results are grammar sketches (Güldemann 2003, Collins and Namaseb 2011), specific morpho-syntactic studies (Collins 2004; Ernszt, Güldemann and Witzlack- 1 I am very grateful to the organizers for inviting me to “The Nǁng (Nǀuu) Conference” held on 17/3/2014 at the University of Cape Town.
    [Show full text]
  • 14 Barnard & Boden Conclusions Final1
    Edinburgh Research Explorer Conjectural histories – Pros and Cons Citation for published version: Barnard, A & Boden, G 2014, Conjectural histories – Pros and Cons. in A Barnard & G Boden (eds), Southern African Khoisan Kinship Systems. Research in Khoisan Studies, vol. 30, Rüdiger Köppe Verlag, Cologne, pp. 263-280. Link: Link to publication record in Edinburgh Research Explorer Document Version: Peer reviewed version Published In: Southern African Khoisan Kinship Systems Publisher Rights Statement: © Barnard, A., & Boden, G. (2014). Conjectural histories – Pros and Cons. In A. Barnard, & G. Boden (Eds.), Southern African Khoisan Kinship Systems. (pp. 263-280). (Research in Khoisan Studies; Vol. 30). Cologne: Rüdiger Köppe Verlag. To be cited as: Alan Barnard / Gertrud Boden (eds.): Southern African Khoisan Kinship Systems (Research in Khoisan Studies , 2014, VI, 301 pp., ill. ISBN 978-3-89645-874-2 General rights Copyright for the publications made accessible via the Edinburgh Research Explorer is retained by the author(s) and / or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing these publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. Take down policy The University of Edinburgh has made every reasonable effort to ensure that Edinburgh Research Explorer content complies with UK legislation. If you believe that the public display of this file breaches copyright please contact [email protected] providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Download date: 02. Oct. 2021 Conclusions 263 Conjectural histories: pros and cons Alan Barnard & Gertrud Boden Overview The aim of this book was to contribute to untangling the historical relations between the indigenous peoples of the Kalahari Basin Area, often subsumed under the label “Khoisan”, yet increasingly thought of as making up a Sprachbund composed of three individual language families, viz.
    [Show full text]
  • Voicing on the Fringe: Towards an Analysis of ‘Quirkyʼ Phonology in Ju and Beyond
    Voicing on the fringe: towards an analysis of ‘quirkyʼ phonology in Ju and beyond Lee J. Pratchett Abstract The binary voice contrast is a productive feature of the sound systems of Khoisan languages but is especially pervasive in Ju (Kx’a) and Taa (Tuu) in which it yields phonologically contrastive segments with phonetically complex gestures like click clusters. This paper investigates further the stability of these ‘quirky’ segments in the Ju language complex in light of new data from under-documented varieties spoken in Botswana that demonstrate an almost systematic devoicing of such segments, pointing to a sound change in progress in varieties that one might least expect. After outlining a multi-causal explanation of this phenomenon, the investigation shifts to a diachronic enquiry. In the spirit of Anthony Traill (2001), using the most recent knowledge on Khoisan languages, this paper seeks to unveil more on language history in the Kalahari Basin Area from these typologically and areally unique sounds. Keywords: Khoisan, historical linguistics, phonology, Ju, typology (AFRICaNa LINGUISTICa 24 (2018 100 Introduction A phonological voice distinction is common to more than two thirds of the world’s languages: whilst largely ubiquitous in African languages, a voice contrast is almost completely absent in the languages of Australia (Maddison 2013). The particularly pervasive voice dimension in Khoisan1 languages is especially interesting for two reasons. Firstly, the feature is productive even with articulatory complex combinations of clicks and other ejective consonants, gestures that, from a typological perspective, are incompatible with the realisation of voicing. Secondly, these phonological contrasts are robustly found in only two unrelated languages, Taa (Tuu) and Ju (Kx’a) (for a classification see Güldemann 2014).
    [Show full text]
  • Trade Adjustment Assistance (TAA) and Its Role in U.S
    Trade Adjustment Assistance (TAA) and Its Role in U.S. Trade Policy J. F. Hornbeck Specialist in International Trade and Finance August 5, 2013 Congressional Research Service 7-5700 www.crs.gov R41922 CRS Report for Congress Prepared for Members and Committees of Congress Trade Adjustment Assistance (TAA) and Its Role in U.S. Trade Policy Summary Congress created Trade Adjustment Assistance (TAA) in the Trade Expansion Act of 1962 to help workers and firms adjust to dislocation that may be caused by increased trade liberalization. It is justified now, as it was then, on grounds that the government has an obligation to help the “losers” of policy-driven trade opening. TAA is also presented as an alternative to policies that would restrict imports, and so provides assistance while bolstering freer trade and diminishing prospects for potentially costly tension (retaliation) among trade partners. As in the past, critics strongly debate the merits of TAA on equity, efficiency, and budgetary grounds. Nonetheless, finding agreement on TAA remains important for forging a compromise on national trade policy. TAA program authorizations are scheduled to expire on December 31, 2013. The Trade Adjustment Assistance Extension Act of 2013 (S. 1357) was introduced in the 113th Congress. It would extend TAA programs through 2020. President Obama also supports TAA reauthorization, linking it to renewal of Trade Promotion Authority (TPA), which Congress may also take up this year. This report discusses the role of TAA in U.S. trade policy from its inception as a legislative option in the early 1950s to its core role as a cornerstone of modern trade policy that many argue has served to promote the long-term U.S.
    [Show full text]
  • Southern Africa As a Phonological Area
    Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology/Linguistics "Speaking (of) Khoisan" A symposium reviewing African prehistory 16/05/2015 Southern Africa as a phonological area Christfried Naumann & Hans-Jörg Bibiko [email protected] Quelle: Clements & Rialland ( 2008 : 37 ) Contents 1. Introduction 3-15 2. Procedure 16-19 3. Results: Kalahari Basin 20-28 4. Results: Southeastern Bantu 29-42 5. Results: Southern Africa 43-54 (6. Local and dependent features - excluded) 55-61 7. MDS and k-means 62-68 8. Summary 69 (9. Contact scenarios) 70-74 Acknowledgements 75 References 76-77 2 "Speaking (of) Khoisan", 16/05/2015 Southern Africa as a phonological area 1. Introduction Phonological similarities • large consonantal inventory (45 c.) • clicks • aspirated and ejective stops • dorsal affricate 3 "Speaking (of) Khoisan", 16/05/2015 Southern Africa as a phonological area 1. Introduction Phonological similarities • large consonantal inventory (50 c.) • clicks • aspirated, slack voiced, ejective and imploisve stops •(dorsal affricate) lateral obstruents • 4 "Speaking (of) Khoisan", 16/05/2015 Southern Africa as a phonological area 1. Introduction Phonological similarities • large consonantal inventory (68 c.) • (clicks) • aspirated, breathy and implosive stops • lateral obstruents 5 "Speaking (of) Khoisan", 16/05/2015 Southern Africa as a phonological area 1. Introduction Example: Distribution of ejectives/glottalized consonants Clements & Rialland (2008: 62) Maddieson (2013) 6 "Speaking (of) Khoisan", 16/05/2015 Southern Africa
    [Show full text]
  • The Kx'a Family
    Journal of Asian and African Studies, No., Article The Kx’a Family A New Khoisan Genealogy1) Heine, Bernd (Institut für Afrikanistik Universität zu Köln) Honken, Henry (Independent Scholar) e question of whether there is a genetic unit called “Khoisan”, as proposed by Greenberg (1963), or whether there are a number of independent genetic stocks of languages within the “Khoisan” area has been discussed controver- sially in the history of Khoisan linguistics, with the second position now being prevalent. In the present study it is argued that there is a genetic unit that includes languages that are traditionally associated with both the Northern and the Southern Khoisan groupings, the languages included being !Xun (or “Ju” or “Ju|hoan”) and Hoan. Building on the work of Honken (2004), the comparative method will be employed to reconstruct some phonological fea- tures of the common ancestor of this language family that we propose to call the “Kx’a family”. 1 Introduction 1.1 e Kx’a languages 1.2 Earlier work 1.3 e present study 2 Phonological reconstruction 2.1 Introduction 2.2 Vowels 2.2.1 Oral vowels 2.2.2 Vowel combinations 2.2.3 Vowels separated by a consonant Keywords: Click Type, Comparative Method, Genetic Relationship, Khoisan, !Xun 1) e present was written while the fi rst-named author spent a year at the Research Institute for Languages and Cultures of Asia and Africa of the Tokyo University of Foreign Studies. is author wishes to thank the Institute for its support, but most of all to Professor Osamu Hieda, who assisted us in multiple ways in carrying out this work.
    [Show full text]
  • 20' Kx'a, Tuu, and Khoe Foragers in the Kalahari Basin Core
    1 2 20’ Kx’a, Tuu, and Khoe foragers in the Kalahari Basin core Lineages and Languages (L) or language complexes (LC) and (Sub)branches Selected dialects and dialect groups 1 General overview (1) KHOE-KWADI + modern linguistic picture changed dramatically in the last few centuries A Kwadi single L† > focus here on the foraging populations and their languages B Khoe Kalahari Khoe > not talk about later colonizing languages like: East Shua: Cara, Deti, ǀXaise, Danisi, etc. a) Afrikaans and English connected to late European colonization Tshwa: Kua, Cua, Tsua, etc. b) African languages pushed into the area during colonial conflicts, notably Khoekhoe and West Ts’ixa Herero from Namibia (former German Southwest Africa) in the west Kxoe: Khwe, ǁAni, etc. Gǁana: Gǁana, Gǀui, etc. > events responsible for major ethno-linguistic disruption in the western sphere of the area Naro: Naro, Ts’ao, etc. c) Bantu languages entering the area in precolonial periods, notably Kgalagadi and Tswana Khoekhoe (Cape K.)† LC from the southeast (ǃOra-Xiri) LC (Eini)† LC Nama-Damara LC Haiǁom ǂAakhoe (2) KX’A A Ju single LC: North: Angolan ǃXuun varieties North-central: Ekoka ǃXuun, Okongo ǃXuun, etc. Central: Grootfontein ǃXuun, etc. Southeast: various Juǀ’hoan varieties B ǂ’Amkoe single LC: ǂHoan, Nǃaqriaxe, Sasi (3) TUU A Taa-Lower Nossob Taa single LC: West: West ǃXoon, (Nǀuǁ’en) East: East ǃXoon, ’Nǀoha, (Nǀamani), (Kakia), etc. Lower Nossob (ǀ’Auni)† (ǀHaasi)† B ǃUi Nǁng: Langeberg, Nǀuu (= ǂKhomani or Nǀhuki), etc. (Danster)† (Vaal-Orange)†* (ǁXegwi)† ??? (ǃGãǃne)† (ǀXam)†: Strandberg, Katkop, Achterveld, etc. † = extinct, (...) = older data sources, * unanalyzed geographical cluster, Bold Relevant languages Figure 1: The three linguistic lineages traditionally subsumed under “Southern African Khoisan” and their preliminary internal composition + highly diverse: languages of all three families, all major branches of two families Map 1: Linguistic lineages of forager groups in the KB core (by C.
    [Show full text]
  • L3: Organization of Speech Sounds
    L3: Organization of speech sounds • Phonemes, phones, and allophones • Taxonomies of phoneme classes • Articulatory phonetics • Acoustic phonetics • Speech perception • Prosody Introduction to Speech Processing | Ricardo Gutierrez-Osuna | CSE@TAMU 1 Phonemes and phones • Phoneme – The smallest meaningful contrastive unit in the phonology of a language – Each language uses small set of phonemes, much smaller than the number of sounds than can be produced by a human – The number of phonemes varies per language, with most languages having 20-40 phonemes • General American has ~40 phonemes (24 consonants, 16 vowels) • The Rotokas language (Paupa New Guinea) has ~11 phonemes • The Taa language (Botswana) has ~112 phonemes • Phonetic notation – International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA): consists of about 75 consonants, 25 vowels, and 50 diacritics (to modify base phones) – TIMIT corpus: uses 61 phones, represented as ASCII characters for machine readability • TIMIT only covers English, whereas IPA covers most languages Introduction to Speech Processing | Ricardo Gutierrez-Osuna | CSE@TAMU 2 [Gold & Morgan, 2000] Introduction to Speech Processing | Ricardo Gutierrez-Osuna | CSE@TAMU 3 • Phone – The physical sound produced when a phoneme is articulated – Since the vocal tract can have an infinite number of configurations, there is an infinite number of phones that correspond to a phoneme • Allophone – A class of phones corresponding to a specific variant of a phoneme • Example: aspirated [ph] and unaspirated [p] in the words pit and spit • Example:
    [Show full text]
  • 189 Tom Güldemann and Anne-Maria Fehn
    book reviews 189 Tom Güldemann and Anne-Maria Fehn (eds.) 2014. Beyond ‘Khoisan’. Historical Relations in the Kalahari Basin. xii, 331 pages. Amsterdam / Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company, Current Issues in Linguistic Theory 330. The so-called Khoisan languages, which are famous for their phonemic clicks, are still spoken today in wide areas of Botswana and Namibia as well as sev- eral adjacent regions. The volume under discussion also shows this on a map (p. 12), which, however, deliberately omits the two Tanzanian representatives Hadza and Sandawe (see the figure on p. 27): They are too far away to be indi- cated on a map of this scale. More importantly, they fall outside the scope of this volume, which is restricted, according to its subtitle, to the Kalahari Basin. The main title Beyond ‘Khoisan’ still underlines the fact that the editors primar- ily pursue an areal linguistic approach and that their main aim is to challenge the idea of a homogeneous Khoisan language family. This becomes obvious in the back cover text, which criticises any attempt at establishing a Khoisan fam- ily as lacking the proper evidence and praises the volume for its interdisciplin- ary analyses of the complex situation. Ironically, the work coming under fire here is lacking in the list of references (p. 308), namely the article by Greenberg (1954). As a matter of fact, however, this early contribution excludes the two Tanzanian languages Hadza and Sandawe from the “Khoisan” concept, which unites the Northern, Central and Southern groups in southern Africa (Green- berg, 1954: map). The volume includes eleven contributions, divided into an introduction and four main parts, by thirteen different authors.
    [Show full text]
  • Endangered African Languages Featured in a Digital Collection: the Case of the ǂkhomani San | Hugh Brody Collection
    Proceedings of the First workshop on Resources for African Indigenous Languages (RAIL), pages 1–8 Language Resources and Evaluation Conference (LREC 2020), Marseille, 11–16 May 2020 c European Language Resources Association (ELRA), licensed under CC-BY-NC Endangered African Languages Featured in a Digital Collection: The Case of the ǂKhomani San | Hugh Brody Collection Kerry Jones, Sanjin Muftic Director, African Tongue, Linguistics Consultancy, Cape Town, South Africa Postdoctoral Research Fellow, English Language and Linguistics, Rhodes University, Makhanda, South Africa Research Associate, Department of General Linguistics, Stellenbosch University, Stellenbosch, South Africa; Digital Scholarship Specialist, Digital Library Services, University of Cape Town, South Africa [email protected], [email protected] Abstract The ǂKhomani San | Hugh Brody Collection features the voices and history of indigenous hunter gatherer descendants in three endangered languages namely, N|uu, Kora and Khoekhoe as well as a regional dialect of Afrikaans. A large component of this collection is audio-visual (legacy media) recordings of interviews conducted with members of the community by Hugh Brody and his colleagues between 1997 and 2012, referring as far back as the 1800s. The Digital Library Services team at the University of Cape Town aim to showcase the collection digitally on the UCT-wide Digital Collections platform, Ibali which runs on Omeka-S. In this paper we highlight the importance of such a collection in the context of South Africa, and the ethical steps that were taken to ensure the respect of the ǂKhomani San as their stories get uploaded onto a repository and become accessible to all. We will also feature some of the completed collection on Ibali and guide the reader through the organisation of the collection on the Omeka-S backend.
    [Show full text]
  • Prison and Garden
    PRISON AND GARDEN CAPE TOWN, NATURAL HISTORY AND THE LITERARY IMAGINATION HEDLEY TWIDLE PHD THE UNIVERSITY OF YORK DEPARTMENT OF ENGLISH AND RELATED LITERATURE JANUARY 2010 ii …their talk, their excessive talk about how they love South Africa has consistently been directed towards the land, that is, towards what is least likely to respond to love: mountains and deserts, birds and animals and flowers. J. M. Coetzee, Jerusalem Prize Acceptance Speech, (1987). iii iv v vi Contents Abstract ix Prologue xi Introduction 1 „This remarkable promontory…‟ Chapter 1 First Lives, First Words 21 Camões, Magical Realism and the Limits of Invention Chapter 2 Writing the Company 51 From Van Riebeeck‟s Daghregister to Sleigh‟s Eilande Chapter 3 Doubling the Cape 79 J. M. Coetzee and the Fictions of Place Chapter 4 „All like and yet unlike the old country’ 113 Kipling in Cape Town, 1891-1908 Chapter 5 Pine Dark Mountain Star 137 Natural Histories and the Loneliness of the Landscape Poet Chapter 6 „The Bushmen’s Letters’ 163 The Afterlives of the Bleek and Lloyd Collection Coda 195 Not yet, not there… Images 207 Acknowledgements 239 Bibliography 241 vii viii Abstract This work considers literary treatments of the colonial encounter at the Cape of Good Hope, adopting a local focus on the Peninsula itself to explore the relationship between specific archives – the records of the Dutch East India Company, travel and natural history writing, the Bleek and Lloyd Collection – and the contemporary fictions and poetries of writers like André Brink, Breyten Breytenbach, Jeremy Cronin, Antjie Krog, Dan Sleigh, Stephen Watson, Zoë Wicomb and, in particular, J.
    [Show full text]
  • From Modern Khoisan Languages to Proto-Khoisan: the Value of Intermediate Reconstructions
    From Modern Khoisan Languages to Proto-Khoisan: The Value of Intermediate Reconstructions George pí~êçëíáå Moscow, Russian State University for the Humanities 0.0. INTRODUCTION. 0.. In a previous article [STAROSTIN 2003] I have argued that a reasonable first step towards reconstructing Proto-Khoisan, or, in fact, towards ascer- taining whether Proto-Khoisan exists in the first place, would be to run the attested lexical evidence through a general lexicostatistical test, bound by certain maximally formalised restrictions. My idea was that not only would such a test be useful in confirming (or refuting) our current theor- ies of the genetic classification of Khoisan languages, but that it could also clarify our understanding of the nature of phonological correspondences between the various Khoisan subgroups, and thus provide us with a few practical clues on how to proceed with the actual reconstruction. Despite several obvious problems with applying glottochronology to Khoisan material (such as the extreme scarcity of data on rare and extinct languages, as well as the lack of a well-established system of phonetic cor- respondences that would allow us to adequately determine cognation), the procedure still managed to yield what I would consider as rather signific- ant results. In regard to the genealogical tree of Khoisan (see Fig. ), it was shown that the resulting classification closely follows some of the already existing conceptions, if not in terms of absolute dating of the subbranches then at least as to their relations to each other. Thus, glottochronology confirms the old subdivision of Khoisan into the North (Zhu), South (Taa-ǃWi), and Central (Khoe) families, as well as the more recent split of the latter two into, respectively, the Taa and ǃWi subgroups, and the Khoekhoe and Non-Khoekhoe subgroups.
    [Show full text]