<<

Personality and Social Review Copyright © 2006 by 2006, Vol. 10, No. 2, 111-132 Lawrence Eribaum Associates, Inc.

Interpersonal Rejection as a Determinant of and Mark R. Leary Department of Psychology Wake Forest University Jean M. Twenge Department of Psychology San Diego University Erin Quinlivan Department of Psychology The Pennsylvania State University

This article reviews the literature on the relationship between interpersonal rejection and aggression. Four bodies of research are summarized: laboratory experiments that manipulate rejection, rejection among adults in everyday life, rejection in child- hood, and individual differences that may moderate the relationship. The theoretical mechanisms behind the effect are then explored. Possible explanations for why rejec- tion leads to anger and aggression include: rejection as a source of , rejection as a source offrustration, rejection as a threat to self-esteem, mood improvement follow- ing aggression, aggression as , aggression as a means of reestablish- ing control, retribution, disinhibition, and loss of self-control.

As a broad category of , aggression is influ- ship, poverty, or drug use. Furthermore, as we will see, enced by an array of biological, psychological, inter- rejection has been implicated in an array of other ag- personal, and cultural factors. Behavioral researchers gressive in everyday life, including domestic have examined the effects of variables as and school shootings. Thus, it seems impor- diverse as hormones, brain abnormalities, , tant to explore whether rejection does, in fact, lead to attributions, ego-threats, observation of aggressive aggression and, if so, why. models, , and cultural norms (for re- The purpose of this article is to review the research views, see Baron & Richardson, 1994; Berkowitz, literature that deals with the relationship between inter- 1993; Geen, 1990). personal rejection and the propensity to aggress. Our re- However, one set of common influences on aggres- view focuses on two primary questions: (a) Does the re- sion have, until recently, received relatively little atten- search literature support the hypothesis that rejection tion—those associated with being rejected by other peo- reliably increases the propensity to aggress, and, if so, ple. Common observation suggests that people often (b) why does the relationship between rejection and ag- become angry, if not aggressive, when they feel that oth- gression exist? On the surface, it seems paradoxical that ers have rejected them. Jilted lovers, children ostracized a person who desired to be accepted would, upon per- by their peers, assistant professors denied tenure, and ceiving rejection, respond in angry, aggressive ways that contestants who are voted off reality game shows such further decrease his or her prospects for . as Survivor are among those in whom one may see evi- dence of aggressive impulses following events in which they feel devalued, unaccepted, or outright rejected. Conceptual Issues In fact, rejection may be one of the most common precursors to aggression. The Surgeon General's re- To begin, we must make it clear precisely what we port on youth violence (Office of the Surgeon General, mean by rejection and the propensity to aggress, and 2001) found that (conceptualized as offer a disclaimer regarding prosocial reactions to "weak social ties") was the most significant risk factor rejection. for adolescent violence, stronger than gang member-

Rejection Correspondence should be sent to , Department of Psychology, Wake Forest University, Winston-Salem, NC 27109. Rejection has been a difficult construct to define for E-mail: [email protected] two reasons. First, juxtaposing rejection against accep-

111 LEARY, TWENGE, QUINLIVAN tance leads one to treat these states as if they were di- other living being. People can be hurt in numerous chotomous when, in fact, shades of acceptance and re- ways, so that deliberately inflicting physical, psycho- jection clearly exist. Second, people often feel (and logical, social, or financial harm all qualify as acts of act) rejected even though they recognize that the other aggression. person accepts them. For example, a man who knows Of course, people often have an urge to aggress that that his wife him dearly may nonetheless feel re- they consciously control. Indeed, people probably sup- jected, hurt, and angry when she ignores him on a par- press their urges to aggress more frequently than they ticular occasion. Similarly, a child who is not selected act on them. For our purposes, it is important to con- first for a team may feel upset even though she is, in sider the effects of rejection not only on overt aggres- fact, chosen for the team, albeit later in the draft. sion but also on aggressive urges. Whether a rejected To provide a more precise way to conceptualize re- individual will actually aggress is presumably a func- jection that avoids dichotomizing experiences into ac- tion not only of his or her psychological state but also ceptance versus rejection, Leary (2001, 2005) sug- environmental factors and internal constraints. gested that acceptance and rejection may be viewed as In many ways, anger can be viewed as the emotional points along a continuum of "relational evaluation." concomitant of the propensity to aggress. According to People value their relationships with other individuals theorists, anger is associated with an action to varying degrees. Acceptance involves a state of rela- tendency toward agonistic behavior aimed at removing tively high relational evaluation in which a person re- an obstacle and asserting control (Frijda, 1986). Of gards his or her relationship with another individual to course, people do not always aggress when angry, and be very valuable or important, whereas rejection is a they may aggress even in a state of calm (as in the case state of low relational evaluation in which a person of a murder for hire). Yet, anger is a signal that bodily does not regard his or her relationship with another in- systems are prepared to take agonistic action. Thus, we dividual as particularly valuable or important. will consider literature on the link between rejection People's emotional and behavioral responses to ac- and anger in addition to that between rejection and ceptance and rejection seem to depend on their percep- overt aggression. tion of how much another person views the relation- ship as valuable or important. People feel accepted Prosocial Reactions to Rejection when perceived relational evaluation exceeds some level but rejected when perceived relational evaluation Although our review focuses on antisocial reactions falls below that criterion, regardless of how much oth- to rejection, we acknowledge at the outset that rejec- ers accept or reject them in an objective sense. Thus, tion may also lead to efforts to increase one's accept- people may feel rejected when perceived relational ability to others by behaving in socially desirable evaluation is not as high as they even though ways. People are strongly motivated to be accepted and they recognize that they are liked, valued, and accepted avoid rejection, and signs of low or declining relational (i.e., others' relational evaluation of them falls on the value often lead people to take steps to strengthen their positive side of neutrality). social bonds (Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Williams, Sometimes, people perceive not only that another 2001). In fact, the most common initial response to individual does not value their relationship as much as perceived rejection may be to behave in ways that pro- they desire but also that their relational value in that in- mote acceptance, for example by doing favors for oth- dividual's eyes has declined relative to some earlier ers, ingratiating, conforming, fostering socially desir- time. Declining relational evaluation, relational deval- able impressions, and otherwise trying to show that uation, is particularly distressing and seems to lead to one has high relational value. the strongest subjective sense of rejection (Buckley, Indeed, a handful of studies have shown that people Winkel, & Leary, 2004; Leary, 2001). To use this ter- who are rejected may make efforts to enhance their ac- minology, our review will focus on the relationship be- ceptance. For example, Williams, Cheung, and Choi tween perceived low relational evaluation, and particu- (2000) showed that ostracized participants conformed larly relational devaluation, and aggression. In some of to others' incorrect judgments more than those who the studies to be reviewed, participants were explicitly were not ostracized, presumably because rejected, excluded, or ostracized, but in others they re- promotes acceptance. Similarly, Williams and Sommer ceived indications that they had low relational value (1997) found that female (but not male) participants even though they were not behaviorally excluded. who were ostracized by other group members subse- quently contributed more solutions to a group task, al- though this effect occurred only if their individual con- Aggression and the Propensity tributions could not be identified. Likewise, people to Aggress who feel that their romantic partner's is wan- Aggression is any physical or verbal action that is ing often take positive steps to increase the strength of performed with the deliberate intention of hurting an- the bond (Gonzaga, Keltner, Londahl, & Smith, 2001).

112 REJECTION AND AGGRESSION

Evidence also suggests that rejection leads people to be urges to aggress, or acts of overt aggression (or, con- more attuned to social information about the rejector or versely, lowered ). rejectors (Gardner, Pickett, & Brewer, 2000), possibly for the purpose of fostering acceptance (although other Anger research shows that recalling past rejections lowers people's empathic accuracy; Pickett, Gardner, & As noted, anger can be viewed as the emotional Knowles, 2004). In any case, people regularly respond concomitant of the propensity to aggress. Thus, we be- to real, imagined, and potential rejection by trying to gin by examining the effects of interpersonal rejection increase their relational value. on anger. Casual observation suggests that people of- Even so, we have chosen to focus our review on an- ten become angry when they feel devalued, ger and aggression because antisocial reactions to re- unincluded, or outright rejected, but experimental doc- jection are, on the surface, quite paradoxical and con- umentation of this effect is speirse. trary to the pervasive desire for social acceptance. The To examine the relationship between rejection, an- fact that people often react prosocially when they do ger, and aggression, Buckley et al. (2004, Experiment not feel valued or accepted needs little explanation. 1) had pairs of participants exchange personal informa- The fact that they sometimes respond in an angry and tion about themselves with one another, then rate the aggressive manner that leads others to dislike, avoid, degree to which they wanted to work with the other and ostracize them begs for greater scrutiny. person on an upcoming task. The participants then re- ceived one of five levels of bogus feedback indicating the degree to which the other person wanted to work Overview with them. Results showed that participants who re- ceived extremely rejecting feedback (indicating that The literature relevant to our review is quite varied the other person definitely did not want to work with and scattered, so we have organized it into four distinct them) reported significantly more angry than but overlapping categories. We deal first with experi- those who received neutral or accepting feedback. mental studies that have examined participants' reac- In a second experiment, Buckley and colleagues tions to a rejecting experience, then focus on the role of (2004) provided ongoing bogus feedback from another rejection in instances of violence among adults in ev- person at 1-min intervals as participants talked about eryday life, such as domestic and homicide. The themselves over an intercom. Whereas some partici- relationship between peer and parental rejection and pants received consistently rejecting or accepting feed- aggressive behavior among children is then explored, back during their 5-min monologue, some participants followed by a review of research on whether individual initially received rejecting feedback that subsequently differences in anger, aggression, and are re- became more accepting over time, and other partici- lated to a sensitivity to feeling rejected. After review- pants initially received accepting feedback that then ing these four areas of empirical research, we turn our turned rejecting. Participants in the increasing rejec- to a theoretical examination of mechanisms tion condition displayed the greatest anger of all by which rejection may instigate aggression. groups, being significantly angrier than participants in the constant acceptance and increasing acceptance conditions. Importantly, participants in the increasing Experimental Studies rejection condition also felt least valued and accepted. Williams and his colleagues have studied people's A number of experiments have examined the emo- reactions to being ostracized while participating in a tional and behavioral effects of being rejected by other ball-toss game on a computer. In this "CyberBall" people. These studies have employed a variety of tech- game (e.g., Williams, Cheung, & Choi, 2000), com- niques for making people believe that others do not ad- puter icons representing other people initially tossed equately value them as social interactants, such as an on-screen ball to the participant's icon. Eventually, leading the participant to believe that other participants however, participants stopped receiving the ball from have voted against having him or her as a member of a the other players. Even when participants believed they laboratory group, making the participant feel left out of were playing against the computer, ostracized partici- a conversation or an ad hoc ball-tossing game, inform- pants reported greater of anger during the ing participants that they are likely to end up alone later game (Zadro, Williams, & Richardson, 2004). In fact, in life, choosing participants last for a laboratory team, this experiment found that led to greater an- or providing direct feedback that other individuals do ger when playing against the computer than when not wish to get to know the participant. We include playing against a virtual opponent controlled by an- studies in this review if they experimentally manipu- other person. lated acceptance or rejection and included measures of In a similar experiment (Williams, Cheung, et al., anger, negative evaluations of the rejecting individuals. 2000), several ostracized participants mentioned feel-

113 LEARY, TWENGE, QUINLIVAN ing angry when asked to report their thoughts (in this ogation itself sometimes an aggressive act intended to case, participants thought they were playing with other damage another person (by hurting their feelings, un- people). One stated, "I am starting to feel left out.. .get- dermining their , or tainting their image in ting a little angry that I'm not being included," and an- others' eyes), but derogating those who reject us might other ostracized participant said, "Why are they only increase the likelihood of aggressing against them. throwing to each other?...Okay, this is getting irritat- Several studies have shown that participants criti- ing" (p. 758). In another set of experiments, partici- cize, devalue, and derogate those who have rejected pants were left out of computer chat room discussions them. In perhaps the earliest demonstration of this ef- (Williams et al., 2002). Although anger was not mea- fect, Pepitone and Wilpizeski (1960) found that indi- sured directly in these experiments, some ostracized viduals who had been rejected subsequently rated their participants displayed what the authors called virtual rejectors as less likeable and as having less valid opin- bravado, responding angrily and sarcastically when ions than individuals who had not been rejected. Simi- they were excluded from the on-line conversation. larly, Geller, Goodstein, Silver, and Sternberg (1974) Although are typically regarded as attacks found that participants who were ignored by confeder- on a person's desired public identity (Felson, 1978; ates subsequently rated the confederates less favorably Tedeschi & Bond, 2001), they may also be viewed as than did participants who had not been ignored. In ad- an expression of low relational evaluation. When one dition, ignored participants did not work as hard to earn person insults another, whether the concerns the a reward for the confederates as did included target's competence, appearance, ethnic group, heri- participants. tage, or something else, the target is likely to conclude After letting participants get to know one another, that the perpetrator does not value him or her as a rela- Leary, Tambor, Terdal, and Downs (1995) led partici- tional partner as much as he or she might desire. Not pants to believe that other group members either did or surprisingly, insults evoke anger and, often, overt did not want them as members of a laboratory group. aggression. In fact, insults so reliably make people Participants who had been excluded by the other group angry that aggression researchers sometimes insult members subsequently rated them less positively than participants to elicit anger and aggression (e.g., those who had been included. This effect was not ob- Bushman, Baumeister, & Phillips, 2001; taine'd when participants were excluded from the group Harmon-Jones & Sigelman, 2001). on the basis of a random procedure. Buckley and col- Although most experiments have found a link be- leagues (2004) replicated this finding using a paradigm tween social rejection and anger, some have not. involving rejection by a lone individual rather than a Twenge, Catanese, and Baumeister (2003) had partici- group (Experiment 1) and also found that, compared to pants meet a group of peers and then learn that either participants who received constant or increasing ac- no one or everyone chose them for further interaction. ceptance as they talked about themselves for 5 min, Participants then rated themselves on a set of emotion those who received constant or increasing rejection words, including 8 positive and 33 negative rated the other person less positively, indicated that emotions (including 7 words about angry feelings). they liked him or her less, and said they desired to get Surprisingly, no significant differences were obtained to know him or her less (Experiment 2). in self-reports of angry feelings between the accepted Twenge, Baumeister, Tice, and Stucke (2001) also and rejected participants. The authors concluded that found that rejected individuals derogated other people rejected individuals may not always report feeling an- (Experiments 1,2, and 3). Based on their responses to a gry because they sometimes enter a state of emotional bogus inventory, some participants were withdrawal and numbness (see also Williams & Zadro, told that they would probably end up alone later in life. 2001). Although we do not know why rejection some- Following this feedback, all participants were led to times causes people to become very emotional, believe that the other participant in their session had in- whereas at other times they become affectively numb, sulted them (by criticizing an essay they had written). presumably the likelihood of aggression is low when Participants were then told that their evaluator was ap- rejected individuals experience numbness and psycho- plying to be a research assistant in the department and logical withdrawal. were asked to evaluate him or her for the job. Partici- pants who heard they were going to be alone later in life gave much more negative evaluations to the indi- Negative Evaluations vidual who had insulted them than did participants in People rarely aggress against those whom they like the control groups. or evaluate positively at a given time. As a result, some Williams and his colleagues have also found this ef- degree of derogation of the other person may be a pre- fect after experiences of exclusion in an online chat requisite for aggression. In light of this, we were inter- room (Williams et al., 2002). Participants who were ex- ested in the effects of rejection on negative evaluations cluded during a chat room conversation subsequently of the rejecting individual. Presumably, not only is der- rated the other chat room individuals as less friendly.

114 REJECTION AND AGGRESSION

helpful, caring, creative, and sincere, and as more bor- cial rejection and aggressive behavior. Following a ing, dishonest, selfish, and insensitive (Experiment 2). 15-min conversation among a group of participants, In another experiment (Experiment 3), excluded partici- each participant voted for the two people with whom pants also reported liking the other participants less. In he or she preferred to work. Some participants were Williams and Sommer (1997), some participants also then told that no one had chosen to work with them (re- made derogatory attributions about confederates who jection), whereas others heard that everyone wanted to excluded them during an in-person ball-tossing game. work with them (acceptance). They were then told they When asked why the confederates stopped throwing the would a reaction time game with a new person ball to them, some ostracized participants derogated the (who was not one of the rejecting group members). others' character (e.g., "they are stuck-up," "they are im- When the opponent lost, the participant was allowed to mature") or described them as dissimilar to themselves determine the length and intensity of a blast of white (e.g., "they are more aggressive than I am," "they are noise (a common measure of aggression; e.g., friends [with each other]," p. 701). Bushman & Baumeister, 1998). Compared to accepted Bourgeois and Leary (2001) led participants to be- participants, rejected participants were significantly lieve that another participant (who was actually a con- more aggressive toward the opponent. Furthermore, federate) had selected them either first or last for a this effect occurred whether the new person had in- five-person team. Participants who thought that they sulted them (Experiment 4) or was a completely inno- were chosen last subsequently rated the confederates cent target with no previous interaction involved (Ex- more negatively than those who were selected first. periment 5). Using a somewhat different manipulation of rejec- Warburton, Williams, and Cairns (2003) likewise tion, Snapp and Leary (2001) failed to find this deroga- showed a link between ostracism and aggression. Par- tion-of-the-rejector effect. In their study, two partici- ticipants experienced ostracism or inclusion in a virtual pants located in different cubicles talked about ball-tossing game and also experienced control (or no themselves simultaneously over an intercom system, control) over unpleasant noise. Participants then had believing that a third participant (actually a confeder- the opportunity to aggress against an innocent target; ate) was listening to them from another room. The con- they were told that the target person did not like spicy federate could ostensibly listen to only one participant foods and that they could assign him or her to eat hot at a time and, thus, had to switch back and forth be- sauce. Results showed that ostracized participants who tween them. Each participant could tell when the con- had no control over the noise were the most aggressive, federate was listening to him or her (rather than to the assigning the target person to eat four times as much other participant) by a light that illuminated in his or hot sauce as participants in the other conditions. Thus, her cubicle. Over the course of talking about them- when ostracized participants could not control an selves for 5 min, participants' lights in the rejection aversive situation, they were more aggressive toward condition were illuminated only one third as much as an innocent target. participants' lights in the acceptance condition. Thus, Harmon-Jones and Sigelman (2001) examined the some participants believed that the confederate clearly effects of a devaluing insult on aggression. After writ- preferred listening to the other person rather than them. ing an essay, some participants received a relatively Although participants felt significantly more rejected negative evaluation from another participant, punctu- when they thought the confederate had listened to them ated by the statement "I can't believe an educated per- only one third of the time, neither evaluations of the son would think like this," whereas other participants confederate nor self-reported anger were affected by received a relatively neutral evaluation. Compared to the acceptance-rejection manipulation. Interestingly, participants who received a neutral evaluation, partici- however, participants in the rejection condition liked pants who were insulted were more likely to select a the other participant less than those in the acceptance decidedly unpleasant-tasting beverage for the other condition. In everyday life, people harbor ill-will to- person to drink. Likewise, Bushman and colleagues ward rivals who they believe undermine their accep- (2001) found that a similar insult increased aggression tance by others. in the noise-blasting game. The results of a quasi-experimental study also Aggression, Aggressive Urges, showed a link between social rejection and aggression and Decreased Prosocial Behavior (Kirkpatrick, Waugh, Valencia, & Webster, 2002). Par- ticipants first completed several measures, including Beyond the feelings of anger and negative evalua- measures of perceived social inclusion. They were then tions of a rejector, other studies have examined the ef- given the opportunity to assign hot sauce to a peer who fects of rejection on the degree to which people engage did not care for spicy foods (as in Warburton et al., in, or desire to engage in, acts of aggression. 2003). When entered into a regression equation with In a series of experiments, Twenge and colleagues global self-esteem, superiority, and , per- (2001) demonstrated a causal connection between so- ceived predicted a greater allocation

115 LEARY, TWENGE, QUINLIVAN of hot sauce. Thus, participants who reported feeling jected them, feel a stronger urge to aggress, or are more generally less socially included were more aggressive likely to engage in actions that inflict distress or with- in the laboratory. hold positive outcomes than people who are socially Rejection also appears to acts of prosocial be- valued. These effects were obtained using a variety of havior. In a series of experiments, Twenge, Ciarocco, manipulations of relational evaluation (e.g., being Cuervo, and Baumeister (2003) manipulated social ex- voted out of a group, being ignored, receiving explicit clusion and gave participants the opportunity to act feedback that others are disinterested, being chosen prosocially. Excluded participants donated less money last for a team, being insulted), as well as different out- to a student fund, were less likely to volunteer for addi- come measures. Thus, rejected people are more likely tional experiments to help graduate students, and were to feel angry, act aggressively toward other people, and less helpful when the experimenter dropped pencils on refuse to help others. Although a few studies failed to the floor. In addition, excluded participants were less reveal effects of rejection on aggression, most sup- cooperative (and more competitive) in a game of Pris- ported the hypothesis that rejection can lead to the psy- oner's Dilemma. Excluded participants were consis- chological states that underlie aggression as well as to tently less prosocial, even when prosocial behavior aggression itself. would have benefited themselves (as in the Prisoner's Dilemma game). In Experiment 1 of Buckley and colleagues (2004), Anger and Aggression participants selected which of seven audio tapes, rang- in Everyday Life ing in pleasantness from "extremely pleasant" to "ex- tremely aversive," another person would listen to while (1890) was among the first to sug- working on a task. Results showed that rejected partici- gest that rejection in the course of everyday life may pants chose less pleasant tapes than participants who precipitate : received acceptance or neutral feedback. However, the average tape selected by participants in the extremely If no one turned round when we entered, answered rejected condition was "neutral" rather than aversive. when we spoke, or minded what we did, but if every Thus, rejected participants did not aggress by assign- person we met 'cut us dead,' and acted as if we were ing aversive tapes; instead, they failed to behave non-existing things, a kind of rage and impotent de- prosocially by assigning pleasant ones (as accepted spair would ere long well up in us, from which the cru- participants did). elest bodily torture would be a relief, (p. 281) Buckley and colleagues (2004; Experiment 2) also measured urges toward aggression and prosocial behav- The studies reviewed in this section dealt with links be- ior. Participants rated how much they felt like reacting in tween perceived rejection and aggression outside the eight prosocial ways (e.g., smiling at the other person) laboratory, including , violent , and eight antisocial or aggressive ways (e.g., humiliat- , and school shootings. We focus here on ing the other person). Compared to accepted partici- studies that used adolescent and adult samples, turning pants, rejected participants reported that they felt less to research on children in the subsequent section. like performing the prosocial actions but more hke per- forming the antisocial reactions. This study is important Self-Reported Reactions to Actual in documenting the presence of aggressive urges follow- Rejecting Events ing rejection even when people do not act on them. Importantly, the decrease in prosocial behavior In an investigation of the events that provoke anger, among rejected individuals may occur primarily with Mabel (1994) derived 10 factors from 360 situations regard to those who rejected them (however, see that participants reported made them angry. Four of the Twenge, Ciarocco, et al., 2003). In fact, evidence sug- 10 factors involved feeling rejected: being ignored or gests that rejection by one group may increase the de- treated badly by a significant other, people demonstrat- gree to which people behave prosocially toward mem- ing that they don't care about the person, being de- bers of another group (Williams, Cheung, et al., 2000). graded or treated unfairly and feeling powerless to do anything about it, and having one's authority, , or feelings being disregarded by others. These findings Summary of the Experimental suggest that a sizable portion of anger-producing situa- Research tions involve feeling rejected. With the exception of Snapp and Leary (2001) and Similarly, when people recount real-life instances of Twenge, Catanese, and Baumeister (2003), all of the rejection, they often report feeling angry and aggres- experimental studies demonstrated that people who sive. A study of incidents in which people's feelings think that other people do not value them feel more an- were hurt showed that people who experience hurt feel- gry, hold more negative opinions of the person who re- ings (a direct consequence of perceiving that another in-

116 REJECTION AND AGGRESSION

dividual does not sufficiently value one's relationship) These figures may underestimate the role of rejection often reported becoming angry and lashing out at those in dating violence, however, because ac- who hurt them (Leary & Springer, 2000; Leary, counted for more than an additional 20% of the epi- Springer, Negel, Ansell, & Evans, 1998). In fact, 80% of sodes and sex accounted for another 18-38% (depend- participants reported that they had expressed anger, and ing on the nature of the dating relationship). Clearly, 62% indicated that they had said something "critical or jealousy entails feelings of rejection (Leary, Koch, & nasty" to the person who had hurt their feelings (Leary et Hechenbieikner, 2001), and many conflicts about sex al., 1998). In the same vein, analysis of 737 episodes of involve the connotations of sex (or the lack of it) for the real ostracism culled from participants' daily reports degree to which one is accepted and loved. found that 43% of the ostracizing experiences were as- Feeling rejected is among the most common sociated with feelings of anger (whereas only 7% were precipitants in cases of husbands killing their wives associated with lowered anger; Williams, 2001). Fur- (Barnard, Vera, Vera, & Newman, 1982; Crawford & thermore, interviews with people who have experienced Gartner, 1992). After examining 551 cases in which the "," often over a prolonged period of men had killed their wives, Crawford and Gartner con- time, revealed that anger is a prominent emotion in their cluded that 45% of the murders occurred in response to accounts. Of course, most of the time, rejection-elicited a real or imminent separation. In an additional 15% of anger does not lead to overt aggression, but the aggres- the cases, the husbands suspected that their wives were sive urges are often present even if not expressed (Wil- engaged in extramarital affairs, which obviously con- liams, 2001). veys relational devaluation. Along the same lines, Barnard and colleagues (1982) found that men who murdered their wives were more likely to perceive their General Studies of Real-World wives as unfaithful than men who had not murdered Crime and Violence their wives. For men, the event that precipitated the ac- In a report published in 2001, the Office of the Sur- tual murder was most often one in which they felt re- geon General reviewed research on the causes of youth jected. Not only was the couple typically separated at violence. The report found that social was the the time of the murder, but the men expUcitly reported most significant risk factor for adolescent violence. being unable to deal with the rejection or their lack of Somewhat surprisingly, an adolescent's social isola- control over their wives. In contrast, Barnard and col- tion was a stronger predictor of violence than gang leagues found that events that led women to murder membership, poverty, or drug use. Thus, young people their husbands most often involved some form of phys- who experience relational devaluation are more likely ical or by the husband. to be aggressive and violent. However, rejection in close relationships does ap- The relationship between rejection and aggression pear to increase other varieties of violence among appears among samples of adults as well. Single men, women. Downey, Freitas, Michaelis, and Khouri who are more socially isolated than married men, are (1998) showed that rejected women are most likely to significantly more likely to commit (Sampson behave aggressively in situations that activate concerns & Laub, 1993). This statistic remains similar even about the loss of a close, . When when age is controlled. The link even occurs in the so- women perceive that an important relationship is ciety as a whole: Lester (1994) performed a time-series threatened (i.e., feel relationally devalued relative to a analysis and found that statistics measuring social inte- previous time), they may respond aggressively. gration (divorce, marriage, and rates) showed a A great deal of anger and aggression in close rela- nearly perfect correlation with homicide rates when tionships arises from jealousy. Jealousy occurs when matched by year. people believe that another person does not sufficiently value their relationship because of the presence or in- trusion of a third party. One study found that men who Aggression in Close Relationships were abusive scored higher in interpersonal jealousy Sadly, people often treat those with whom they have than those who were not abusive (Dutton, van Ginkel, close relationships far worse than they treat strangers & Landolt, 1996). In addition, women who are abused and acquaintances (Miller, 1997). Many of these in- and the men who abuse them report jealousy as the stances of verbal and appear to stem most common precursor to violence (Daly & Wilson, from events that convey that the individual is not suffi- 1988). Among both men and women, intimate vio- ciently loved and valued in the context of a close lence is often provoked by real or imagined infidelity relationship. (Barnard et al., 1982; de Weerth & Kalma, 1993) In a study of dating couples. Makepeace (1989) which is perhaps the penultimate cause of feelings of found that rejection accounted for 15% of the violent relational devaluation. episodes for those who were dating steadily and 11% Violent husbands are also more likely than nonvio- of the violence for those who were living together. lent husbands to report about being abandoned by

117 LEARY, TWENGE, QUINLIVAN their partner (Dutton, van Ginkel, et al., 1996; Defending one's "honor." Cultural norms may Holtzworth-Munroe, Stuart, & Hutchinson, 1997). Al- dictate aggressive behavior in response to real or im- though abusive men will, at times, outwardly condemn plied rejection. In a "culture of honor," for example, an their partner and express feelings of callousness, they individual who insults someone and refuses to apolo- typically find the possibility of being abandoned dis- gize or retract the insult must be punished (Nisbett, tressful (a phenomenon called "masked dependency;" 1993; Nisbett & Cohen, 1996), Although this retalia- Dutton, 1988). Men who abandonment may be tory is typically viewed as necessary to de- overly sensitive to signs of rejection, perceiving even fend one's honor, these behaviors may also be concep- innocuous situations as threats to the relationship. tualized as a reaction to perceived low relational Dutton (1988) suggested that, in abusive relationships, evaluation. An affront to one's honor typically indi- "the most dangerous times for a woman are when she cates that the disrespectful person does not adequately separates, when she seeks shelter, and when she be- value the target as a social participant, relational part- comes pregnant" (p, 46), all of which raise the specter ner, or (perhaps) being. Thus, in a culture of of devaluation (even the expectant father may honor, violence is deemed appropriate and justified that the new baby will replace him in his wife's affec- when an individual feels that he or she has been so- tion). Thus, it appears that fear of separation, rejection, cially devalued. or abandonment may be an important factor in some Among the subcultures that ascribe to a culture of cases of domestic abuse. honor is that of the White male southerner (Nisbett & Attributions for rejection may be important media- Cohen, 1996), Statistics indicate that, although men tors of domestic violence. Partners who them- from the do not approve of vio- selves for the rejection may be more likely to experi- lence in general more than northerners, they more ence and withdrawal, but those who blame strongly endorse violence in response to being in- others (either the partner or third parties) may be more sulted. Not surprisingly, southern men also have a likely to respond aggressively (Levy, Ayduk, & higher rate of homicides that are provoked by argu- Downey, 2001). For example, compared to nonviolent ments, conflicts, and insults (Cohen, Nisbett, Bowdle, husbands, violent husbands were more likely to believe & Schwarz, 1996), that their wives had intentionally tried to hurt them In an experiment comparing northern and southern (Holtzworth-Munroe & Hutchinson, 1993). Addi- men, Cohen and colleagues (1996) found that men tionally, violent husbands were more likely to make at- from the South were more likely to respond to insults tributions of negative intent in particular situations, with anger and aggressiveness than men from the such as those involving perceived rejection. In other North, In addition, when participants had been insulted situations, such as the wife making a request from the in front of another individual, men from the South (vs, husband, there were no differences in the attributions the North) were much more likely to report that their of violent and nonviolent husbands. masculinity in other people's eyes would be affected. Research shows that real or perceived rejection Southern men also increased their dominance behavior plays an important role in violence between romantic and aggressiveness when interacting with the observer, partners. Of course, not all individuals who feel re- even though these behaviors were measured prior to jected by their partner respond aggressively. The likeli- the masculinity rating (Cohen et al., 1996). Thus, when hood of violence appears to depend on several factors, men who subscribe to a culture of honor feel that their including abandonment , attribution of negative public image of masculinity has been undermined by a intent to one's partner, and an actual or impending sep- devaluing insult or taunt, they may try to restore their aration. Furthermore, we suspect that rejections by in- image of masculinity by responding aggressively. timates evoke stronger emotional reactions of all kinds—including , anxious desperation, and Sexual assault. Some sexual offenses may also anger—than similar rejections by acquaintances and be seen as aggression in response to feelings of rejec- strangers. No research has directly examined differ- tion. For example, researchers have proposed that defi- ences in how people react to rejections as a function of cits in intimacy may be more common among sexual whether the individual is in a preexisting relationship offenders than among nonsexual offenders (Garlick, with the rejector, and work on this topic is needed. Marshall, & Thornton, 1996; Marshall & Hambley, 1996; Seidman, Marshall, Hudson, & Robertson, 1994), In fact, male sexual offenders have lower inti- macy scores than nonoffenders (Garlick et al., 1996; Nonintimate Aggression Marshall & Hambley, 1996; Seidman et al., 1994), and Rejection may also contribute to aggression outside lower intimacy scores correlate with higher levels of of close relationships. People may react aggressively to hostility toward women (Marshall & Hambley, 1996), real and imagined rejection by friends, acquaintances, Thus, male offenders may feel rejected due to a lack of and even strangers. intimate relationships and subsequently aggress

118 REJECTION AND AGGRESSION against those who they feel are responsible for these larly lonely and isolated, and that their actions may feelings. Accordingly, researchers have shown that have been precipitated by rejection by schoolmates or rapists often attribute their lack of intimacy to women others (e,g,, Perlstein, 1999; Peterson, 1999), To deter- (Garlick et al., 1996). Rapists commonly report having mine whether school shootings were, in fact, linked to conflicts with women arising from perceived or actual rejection, Leary, Kowalski, Smith, and Phillips (2003) rejection that led them to be lonely (McKibben, analyzed all well-documented cases of Proulx, & Lusignan, 1994). in the United States from January, 1995 (the year in Baumeister, Catanese, and Wallace (2002) pro- which school shootings began to receive national at- posed that rape is often caused by a combination of tention) to March, 2001, Of the 15 incidents they ex- and a sexual refusal (a clear relational de- amined, interpersonal rejection was clearly indicated valuation or rejection). This hypothesis was supported in 13 of the shootings. In many of these instances, the by a series of experiments (Bushman, Bonacci, van perpetrator(s) had experienced a pattern of malicious Dijk, & Baumeister, 2003), In one experiment, a fe- or , or ongoing ostracism that left the male actor refused to read a sexually arousing passage perpetrator on the periphery of the school's social life. to a male participant. Narcissistic men who experi- Importantly, in many of the incidents, the victims in- enced this refusal were subsequently more punitive to- cluded individuals who had teased, bullied, or rejected ward the woman, paying her less and recommending the shooter. In only two cases was there no evidence that she not be rehired. At least for narcissists, a sexual whatsoever that the perpetrator had been rejected or refusal can lead to punitive behavior. mistreated by other people. Intimacy deficits, feelings of , and sexual As Leary and colleagues (2003) noted, few indi- refusals may elicit of low relational evalu- viduals make it through without experi- ation. Therefore, it is possible that the aggression of in- encing a certain amount of rejection, yet most do not dividuals who commit sexual offenses is sometimes murder their classmates. Thus, peer rejection alone fueled by feelings of rejection. Moreover, the finding may not lead to school violence unless the rejected that men who have sexually assaulted women blame individual possesses other risk factors. In their analy- women for their perceived rejection suggests that these sis of the school shootings, Leary and colleagues men are aggressing against women due to their feel- found that most of the shooters also displayed psy- ings of rejection. chological problems, an abnormal in guns or explosives, or a fascination with death that may put Homicide. Many murders of strangers and ac- an individual at higher risk to perpetrate aggression quaintances are acts of vengeance (Palermo, 1997), when rejected. Examining the context of deadly vengeful acts sug- Narcissism may also play a role. In videotapes gests that perceived rejection is often involved. For ex- made before the incident, the two Columbine shooters ample, former employees have returned to their work- made several statements that were remarkably similar place to kill those who fired them. In addition, there to items on the Narcissistic Personality Inventory have been cases of homicide by individuals who felt (NPI; Twenge & Campbell, 2003), For example, Eric that they were rejected because of their race (Palermo, Harris said, "Isn't it fun to get the respect that we're go- 1997), Levin and Fox (1991) asserted that perceived ing to deserve?", which is very similar to NPI Item 14, rejection by members may lead to murder in ex- "I insist upon getting the respect that is due me," Harris treme cases. For example, a husband who feels intense also said, "I could convince them that I'm going to anger and loneliness due to rejection by his wife may climb Mount Everest, or I have a twin brother growing kill "'her' children in retaliation as if he were attacking out of my back, I can make you believe anything," his offensive partner" (p. 90), which is strikingly similar to NPI Item 35, "I can make In a study conducted by the Federal Bureau of In- anyone believe anything I want them to." Twenge and vestigation, the personal histories of 36 sexual killers Campbell tested this observation in a series of studies were examined. One conclusion of this study was that and found that narcissists who were rejected reacted these men did not form adequate bonds of attachment with greater anger and aggression than participants with their primary caregivers and that feelings of social who scored low in narcissism. isolation were a factor in their violence (Ressler, Bur- Although only suggestive, these findings indicate gess, & Douglas, 1988), Malmquist (1996) proposed that researchers should begin to search for variables that the isolation felt by these men led to extreme anger that moderate the occurrence of aggression in the face as they perceived themselves as being rejected by of rejection. What kinds of individuals do and do not . aggress when rejected? We return to this question in the following sections. School shootings. Following the rash of school shootings during the late 1990s, many commentators Gang violence. Membership in a gang may be observed that the perpetrators seemed to be particu- prompted by feelings of rejection, and in turn, lead to

119 LEARY, TWENGE, QUINLIVAN

violent actions. Social rejection has been identified as a are occasionally rejected by their peers, studies show factor in the decision to join a gang (Caims, that some children are chronically rejected. In fact, Cadwallader, Estell, & Neckerman, 1997). In their 30-45% of children who can be classified as rejected at 16-year longitudinal study of gangs, Caims and Caims one point in time wili remain in the rejected category (1994) indicated that gang members often felt that they over the following 4 years (Coie & Dodge, 1983). were not accepted elsewhere in society, and Branch The designation of children as rejected has been (1999) proposed that an adolescent's need to belong made in many ways, through sociometric ratings, peer may be satisfied through affiliation with a gang. Run- nominations, self-reports, and teacher ratings, and the aways and homeless adolescents, for instance, are measures of aggressiveness have been equally diverse. likely candidates for gang membership (Caims & Regardless of the methods and measures used, dozens Caims, 1994). Garbarino (1999) found that many per- of studies have shown that children who are rejected by petrators of violence are young men who feel rejected their peer groups are more aggressive on average than by family members, peers, and society in generai. As children who are accepted. Several excellent reviews adolescents individuate from their and seek of the relationship between peer rejection and child- new places to feel accepted, a gang may appear to be a hood problem behaviors, including aggression, have viable option for some. Branch (1999) acknowledged been published (Cicchetti & Bukowski, 1995; many adolescents report that they joined a gang to Kupersmidt, Coie, & Dodge, 1990; McDougall, "gain respect" but argued that underlying this desire Hymel, Vaillancourt, & Mercer, 2001; Newcomb, for respect is the need for acceptance and approval. Bukowski, & Pattee, 1993; Parker & Asher, 1987; Ru- Furthermore, to achieve and maintain acceptance bin, Bukowski, & Parker, 1998), so we will touch on within a gang, members must often engage in violent be- only a few pertinent issues here. havior. Some of this behavior is assigned by leaders of The data consistently show that rejected children the gang. If a gang member is not given assignments, he tend to be more aggressive than children of average or may subsequently engage in violent behavior on his or popular status. For example. Dodge (1983) studied the her own, with the intention of gaining the acceptance behavior of 48 unacquainted second grade boys before and respect of other members (Branch, 1999). Conse- and after a peer social status had been formed from eight quently, perceived rejection by members of one's gang different i-hour sessions of playtime. In the beginning, can result in a recurring cycle of aggressive acts. the rejected boys approached peers more than the other boys, but they were brushed off quickly at high rates. Near the end of the sessions, they stopped attempting to Summary of the Evidence Regarding find a playmate because they had such a high failure rate. Everyday Aggression These rejected boys were more physically and verbally The evidence reviewed in this section goes beyond aggressive, and they engaged in more inappropriate be- laboratory experiments to provide support for a rela- havior, such as standing on tables and intermpting other tionship between rejection and aggression in everyday boys' activities. At the end of the study, the rejected boys life. Perceived rejection has been linked to a number of were viewed by their peers as highly aggressive, poor aggressive acts in daily life, including domestic vio- leaders, and selfish. In contrast, the popular boys were lence, homicides, school shootings, and gang violence. more physically attractive, less aggressive, and received Not only do individuals who commit aggressive acts more positive from their peers when initi- report greater feelings of rejection than nonaggressive ating a social interaction. individuals, but their self-reported motives for aggress- Although average and rejected boys become ing often contain elements of perceived rejection. Most equally angered and aggressive when provoked, re- of the studies reviewed in this section were descriptive jected boys behave more aggressively without justifi- or correlational in nature and, thus, firm inferences cation (Coie, Dodge, & Kupersmidt, 1990). Further- about cannot be drawn. Even so, when com- more, once an aggressive exchange has begun, rejected bined with the results from experimental research, children are more likely to intensify their aggression these studies clearly support a relationship between re- and less likely to submit than nonrejected children jection and aggression. (Coie, Dodge, Terry, & Wright, 1991). Furthermore, both chronic and recent peer rejection predicts extemalizing problems, such as aggression, even after Rejection in Childhood controlling for initial levels of psychological adjust- ment (DeRosier, Kupersmidt, & Patterson, 1994). Peer Rejection Although the link between rejection and aggression By far the greatest amount of research on the link be- in childhood is as incontrovertible as any in psychology, tween rejection and aggression has been conducted on researchers disagree regarding the primary direction of children, most of it involving rejection and aggression in influence. Are aggressive children simply more likely to the context of peer relationships. Although aii children be rejected by their peers, does peer rejection heighten 120 REJECTION AND AGGRESSION aggression, or does some other variable (e.g., poor so- children, including excessive aggression. In particu- cial skills) underlie both? The evidence supports a vari- lar, rejection by parents is associated with higher ety of causal pathways. First, considerable research sup- hostility and aggression in childhood (Edens, 1999; ports the idea that aggressive children tend to be more Ojha & Pramanick, 1995; Patterson, Reid, & Dishion, disliked and rejected than nonaggressive children (e.g., 1992; Pemberton & Benady, 1973; Symonds, 1939), Bukowski & Newcomb, 1984; Little & Garber, 1995). and perceptions of low parental support predict child- However, this direction of influence is irrelevant to our hood aggression, particularly for boys (Garbarino, review and will not be discussed further. 1999; Patterson, Kupersmidt, & Griesler, 1990). In More importantly for our purposes, studies that fact, one study concluded that parental rejection "was have attempted to determine whether rejection is an an- the most prominent predictor of synchronous aggres- tecedent of aggression have obtained positive findings. sion" (Lefkowitz, Huesmann, Walder, & Eron, 1973, For example, a longitudinal study of 880 elementary p. 39). and middle-school students showed that peer rejection In perhaps the most extensive study of the relation- was a consistently powerful predictor of future aggres- ship between parental rejection and children's behav- sion and other extemalizing problems. As rejection in- ior and , Rohner (1975) examined the cor- creased over time, so did the risk of aggressive behav- relates of parental acceptance and rejection in 60 ior (Kupersmidt, Burchinal, & Patterson, 1995; see worldwide. He concluded that "compared alsoMerten, 1994). with accepted children, rejected children throughout To make matters worse, children who be- the world are significantly more hostile, aggressive, or havioral problems, including aggression, are particu- passive aggressive," and estimated the correlation be- larly likely to be affected by peer rejection (for a re- tween parental acceptance and childhood hostility to view, see McDougall et al., 2001). One study showed be-.49 (pp. 100-101). that students who were both rejected and aggressive in Several studies have also shown that hostility and grade 5 had nearly a 93% chance of having later anger in adulthood correlate with perceptions that extemalizing difficulties such as truancy, police con- one's parents were less warm and more rejecting, and tact, or aggressive behavior (Kupersmidt & Coie, that one's family environment was less cohesive 1990). Importantly, these pattems tend to be stronger (Meesters & Muris, 1996; Woodall & Mathews, 1989). for boys than for girls, although this difference may Of course, such findings are only suggestive because of partly reflect an inadequate understanding of how ag- the risks of retrospective autobiographical reports. It is gression manifests in girls (McDougall et al., 2001). just as likely that hostile and angry people mis- Thus, the data suggest that peer rejection may both remember their parents as unsupportive as it is that precede and result from a chronic pattem of aggres- unsupportive parents produce hostile adults. sion. However, some researchers suggest that, even To address the issue of directionality, Mathews, when peer rejection precedes aggression, rejection per Woodall, Kenyon, and Jacob (1996) assessed the qual- se is not the cause. Rather, rejection may be an inciden- ity of parent-child interactions and hostility at two tal marker of some other problematic condition that points in time. Pubescent boys and their parents were predisposes children to be aggressive, such as family interviewed and observed while trying to resolve dis- problems, lack of , genetic predispositions, agreements. Three years later, measures of hostility or psychological difficulties (Burks, Dodge, & Price, and anger were administered, A high number of nega- 1995). Peer rejection and aggression may both stem tive parental behaviors and a low number of positive from some common underlying factor. parental behaviors during the parent-son discussions Virtually all of the research on childhood peer rejec- predicted sons' hostility and anger three years later, tion and aggression has been conducted at a even after adjusting for their initial level of hostility. As dispositional level of analysis, comparing rejected and the authors noted, these data "demonstrate prospec- nonrejected children. Thus, we know that children who tively that observed nonsupportive, affectively nega- are frequently rejected also tend to be aggressive, but tive family interactions lead to relatively high levels of we do not have much data bearing on the question of hostility in boys" (p. 35). whether specific incidents of rejection increase chil- In studying abusive personality characteristics dren's propensity to aggress, although circumstantial among men, Dutton, Starzomski, and Ryan (1996) evidence suggests that this is the case. found that patemai rejection was the strongest predic- tor of an abusive personality. Similarly, Bamow, Schuckit, Lucht, John, and Freyberger, (2002) identi- Parental Rejection fied parental rejection as a common factor in the devel- Children are rejected not only by their peers but opment of antisocial , which is also sometimes by parents and other caretakers. Pa- characterized by callousness and aggressiveness. rental indifference, neglect, rejection, and abuse are Using a large birth cohort of over 4,000 males in among the best predictors of behavioral problems in Denmark, Raine, Brennan, and Mednick (1994) found

121 LEARY, TWENGE, QUINLIVAN

that maternal rejection in infancy had a particularly ficient number of studies have examined their relation- pernicious long-term effect on boys who had experi- ship to aggression. enced complications during birth (such as breech de- livery, the need to use forceps, or long birth duration). Of the boys who had experienced both maternal rejec- Attachment Style tion at age 1 and birth complications, nearly 9% had Observations of young children revealed aggressive been involved in by age 18, compared to behavior stemming from maternal separation (Bowlby, less than 4% for those who had experienced either re- 1973). For example, children who were separated from jection, birth complications, or neither. Maternal rejec- their mothers tended to play in more aggressive ways tion did not predict nonviolent crime, and the effect than children not separated from their mothers. was not moderated by other stressors such as socioeco- Bowlby (1969) described this reaction as "an anger nomic status. It is unclear how these findings should be bom of fear." This fear is displayed through aggressive interpreted, but they support the notion that rejection acts, as the baby reacts in the only ways that he or she can moderate the relationship between early stressors can. The baby may "cry loudly, shake his cot, throw and other harmful conditions and vulnerability to anti- himself about, and look eagerly towards any sight or social behaviors (Bagwell, Newcomb, & Bukowski, sound which might prove to be his missing mother" 1998). Clearly, research is needed to study how rejec- (Bowlby, 1969, p. 38). Attachment theory proposes tion may interact with other developmental influences. that attachment systems serve to gain access to the at- tachment figure. For the baby, the purpose of aggres- sive actions is coercive. or throwing a temper Summary of the Developmental will often lead to the parent's return, overcome Evidence obstacles in the path of a reunion, and discourage fu- ture separation (Dutton, 1998). Virtually all studies have demonstrated a robust re- lationship between rejection and aggression in chil- These initial infant responses potentially transfer dren. Although most of this research has examined into adult responses, with separation from a loved one peer rejection, the available studies of parental rejec- leading to shouting, crying, and throwing or smashing tion demonstrate the same effect. The prevailing ques- objects (Dutton, 1998). In an intimate adult relation- tion has been whether rejection increases the tendency ship, anger and jealousy in response to a rejection may for children to aggress, or whether aggressive children serve to show the partner how much one cares or deter are more likely to receive rejecting feedback from their the partner from leaving. Although these actions ap- parents and peers. To the degree that these effects can pear to be maladaptive in adulthood, their origin in in- be disentangled, they both seem to occur. Although fancy suggests that they may initially serve an impor- some developmental psychologists have resisted the tant function. Bowlby (1984) contended that the idea that rejection per se can lead to aggression (e.g., purpose of angry behavior is to protect the relationship, Burks et al., 1995), when considered in light of the asserting that "in the right place, at the right time, and adult literatures examined in this review, it would be in right degree, anger is not only appropriate but may surprising if children did not become aggressive when be indispensable" (p. 11). they felt rejected. Bartholomew (1990) identified four attachment pat- terns in adulthood: secure, fearful, preoccupied, and dismissive. Secure and dismissive attachment patterns are not strongly associated with violence (Dutton, Personality Differences Saunders, Starzomski, & Bartholomew, 1994). Se- in Anger-Proneness, Hostility, curely attached individuals do not fear intimacy that and Aggressiveness arises within close relationships; dismissively attached individuals tend to devalue the necessity of intimate at- The conclusion that interpersonal rejection pro- tachments and, thus, do not often experience relation- motes aggression leads to the hypothesis that individ- ship insecurity. In contrast, individuals whose attach- ual differences in anger, aggressiveness, and hostility ment style is fearful or preoccupied are more likely to should be related to people's general perceptions of be- become angry in intimate relationships, particularly ing accepted versus rejected by other people. As we when the relationship is threatened. have seen, this relationship seems to hold among chil- Men who exhibit a fearful or preoccupied attach- dren, as the most rejected children also tend to be the ment style are more likely to have a history of relation- most aggressive. Although many individual difference ship abuse. In addition, statements consistent with variables may moderate the link between rejection and fearful and preoccupied attachment styles had stronger aggression, two in particular—attachment style and re- positive associations with measures of anger and jeal- jection sensitivity—will be reviewed here, both be- ousy than did statements consistent with secure and cause the conceptual links are clear and because a suf- dismissive attachment styles (Dutton et al., 1994). 122 REJECTION AND AGGRESSION

Similarly, in a study measuring the behavior of both reacted with greater hostility than women low in rejec- husbands and wives, Marchand (2004) reported that in- tion sensitivity only when the situation involved dividuals who reported being anxious about rejection feeling rejected. Thus, expectations of rejection may or abandonment from their partners were more likely result in antisocial, if not aggressive, behavior that to say that they would attack their partner in response leads to rejection. Based on this evidence, it appears to a marital confiict than individuals who did not report that a destructive cycle of rejection may occur in the re- attachment anxiety. lationships of high rejection-sensitive women. People with a preoccupied or fearful attachment Although Downey and colleagues (1998) found that style are anxious about gaining approval from their men's expectations of rejection did not lead to behavior partner yet are fearful of rejection or abandonment. As that elicited rejection, men high in rejection sensitivity a result, they vacillate between wanting to be intimate may nonetheless engage in antisocial behaviors when and wanting to avoid intimacy (Dutton, 1998; Dutton they feel rejected. Downey, Feldman, and Ayduk et al., 1994). This struggle between craving and fearing (2000) revealed that men who were both high in rejec- intimacy may make these individuals most likely to ag- tion sensitivity and highly invested in their romantic re- gress within a close relationship (Dutton et al., 1994). lationship were much more likely to engage in actual Moreover, men with a fearful attachment style were violence against their dating partners than either men found to consistently attribute negative intent to their who were low in rejection sensitivity or men who were partner (Starzomski, 1993, as cited in Dutton, 1998). high in rejection sensitivity but less invested in their re- As discussed previously, individuals who believe that lationships. In contrast, men who sought to avoid inti- their partner has intentionally hurt them are more macy were more likely to reduce their involvement in likely to respond aggressively. Therefore, men with a the relationship in response to anxiety about rejection. fearful attachment style not only struggle between Thus, one variable that may moderate the rejection-ag- wanting and avoiding intimacy, but they also tend to gression pathway in the context of relationships is the perceive their partners in a negative light. This combi- amount of commitment and investment in the relation- nation of an internal confiict and external blame may ship (Downey et al., 2000; Levy et al., 2001). The para- suggest that individuals who are fearfully attached will dox is that people who are worried about rejection and be most aggressive in response to rejection. invested in close relationships behave in ways that damage those relationships when they feel rejected.

Rejection Sensitivity Summary of the Personality Research People who are rejection sensitive tend to expect, perceive, and overreact to rejection (Downey & Research on attachment style and rejection sensitiv- Feldman, 1996; Feldman & Downey, 1994). Accord- ity suggests that some individuals are more prone than ing to Feldman and Downey, parental rejection causes others to be aggressive in response to perceived rejec- children to become sensitive to signs of rejection. This tion. Individuals who have a secure attachment style or hypervigilance for rejection leads people who are high who are low in rejection sensitivity may be less likely in rejection sensitivity to perceive even ambiguous to respond aggressively to rejection. negative behavior as rejecting (Downey & Feldman, 1996). Not only do rejection-sensitive individuals perceive Psychological Mechanisms Underlying rejection in the absence of intentional rejection but, for Rejection-Elicited Aggression women, expectations of rejection lead to abusive and aggressive behaviors toward their partners. Downey Our survey of the literature supports the hypothesis and colleagues (1998) explored this self-fulfilling that interpersonal rejection plays a role in the instiga- prophecy in the context of confiicts occurring in dating tion of aggression. A relationship between rejection relationships. Whereas the behavior of rejec- and aggressive behavior has been found using a wide tion-sensitive men during a confiict did not lead their array of methods, including controlled experiments, partners to reject them, the behavior of rejec- correlational research, and longitudinal studies. Fur- tion-sensitive women during confiicts tended to elicit thermore, the findings not only link rejection to en- rejecting responses from their partners. Compared to acted aggressive behavior but also to potential media- low rejection-sensitive women, high rejec- tors of the relationship such as anger and interpersonal tion-sensitive women demonstrated more negative be- derogation. haviors during a confiict, including a hostile or nega- Unfortunately, few studies have made any effort to tive tone of voice, demeaning or mocking one's understand precisely why rejection sometimes leads to partner, and gestures of or disapproval anger and an impulse to aggress, nor when this effect (Downey et al., 1998). Ayduk, Downey, Testa, and Yen does and does not occur. In this section, we speculate (1999) found that women high in rejection sensitivity regarding nine possible explanations for the link be-

123 LEARY, TWENGE, QUINLIVAN tween rejection and aggression: rejection as a source of Given the robustness of the relationship between pain, rejection as a source of frustration, rejection as a pain and aggression and the extremely hurtful nature of threat to self-esteem, mood improvement following many rejections, we suspect that the sheer aversiveness aggression, aggression as social influence, aggression of the experience may facilitate aggressive behavior. as a means of reestablishing efficacy and control, retri- This hypothesis could be tested by administering bution, disinhibition, and loss of self-control. Most of agents that increase or decrease the pain threshold to these explanations have both conceptual and empirical determine whether the likelihood or strength of rejec- support, and the current literature is inadequate to tion-elicited aggression is affected. Although the pain- eliminate any of them. At present, it seems likely that fulness of some rejections may heighten the likelihood rejection may lead to aggression via a number of inde- of aggression, we that this is the only mechanism pendent routes, which may explain why the effect is so by which rejection infiuences aggression. robust.

Rejection as a Source of Frustration

Rejection as a Source of Pain A second possibility is that rejected people aggress because rejecting events frustrate their efforts to obtain The effect of aversive stimuli on aggressive attack is social acceptance or other desired outcomes that de- well-documented. In both human and nonhuman ani- pend on acceptance. This explanation fits the perspec- mals, physical pain can induce anger and aggression. tive of the classic frustration-aggression hypothesis who receive an electric shock spontaneously at- (Dollard, Doob, Miller, Mowrer, & Sears, 1939). The tack other rats who are nearby (Azrin, 1967), and phys- hypothesis that aggression occurs when people per- ical discomfort increases people's willingness to blast ceive an external obstacle to reaching a goal has been another person with aversive noise (Berkowitz, 1983). supported by numerous studies (Berkowitz, 1989). The target of the aggression need not have caused the As Williams (1997, 2001) noted, ostracism (and re- pain, much as a person who hits his thumb with a ham- jection more generally) thwarts a number of basic mer may fiy into a rage despite the fact that the injury needs, and the resulting frustration may lead to aggres- was self-inflicted. Findings such as these led sion. Frustration may emerge due to the rejecting be- Berkowitz (1989) to suggest that all aggression is a re- havior of a specific individual whose acceptance one sponse to aversive affect. He reviewed the many physi- (as when a person is "stood up" by his or her cally aversive experiences that have been linked to ag- date) or the rejecting behavior of anyone who thwarts a gressive behavior, including unpleasantly high person's general need for acceptance or temperatures, foul odors, receiving shocks, and im- (as when one is snubbed by a stranger or ostracized by mersing one's arm in ice water. Berkowitz argued that a group). these painful and unpleasant experiences cause aggres- Ojha and Pramanick (1995) favored a frustra- sion because they increase negative affect. Given that tion-aggression explanation of the link between paren- rejection can cause exceptionally unpleasant emotions tal behavior and children's hostility: "As a result of (such as hurt feelings, sadness, and jealousy; Leary et control and rejection by the parents, the basic psycho- al., 2001), it may automatically induce anger and ag- logical needs of the child, for example, security, emo- gression by the same route. Along these lines, Thomas tional contact and affection, are not met and he feels (1995) suggested that the painful feelings that often re- frustrated which ultimately results into aggressive ten- sult from rejection might provoke anger and dencies" (p. 34). aggression. These first two models, based on aversive stimuli A recent fMRI study provided support for this idea. and frustration respectively, regard rejection-elicited Eisenberger, Lieberman, and Williams (2003) had par- aggression as a specific case of more general psycho- ticipants play the ostracism game, CyberBall, while logical processes. However, other models posit a par- undergoing an fMRI brain scan. Ostracized partici- ticular, unique effect of interpersonal rejection beyond pants showed activation in two brain regions that are the fact that rejection is aversive and frustrating. also active when people experience physical pain. The authors concluded that "social pain is analogous in its neurocognitive function to physical pain, alerting us Rejection as a Threat to Self-Esteem when we have sustained injury to our social connec- Baumeister and his colleagues have suggested that tions" (p. 292). Thus, people who are socially rejected aggression is sometimes the result of events that react physiologically much like people who experience threaten a person's ego or lower his or her self-esteem physical pain. Given the clear connection between (Baumeister, Smart, & Boden, 1996). Their review of physical pain and aggression, rejection may lead to ag- the evidence suggested that losses of self-esteem are gression through this pathway (see also MacDonald & associated with an increased likelihood of aggression Leary, 2005). and violence. They argued that high self-esteem people

124 REJECTION AND AGGRESSION who experience a threat may lash out in violence be- after aggressing. Their results did suggest, however, cause their favorable view of themselves is not that aggression may stimulate positive affect under matched by external appraisals. some circumstances. Whether or not aggression actu- Williams' analysis of the four needs that are ally improves mood, people who think that it does may thwarted by ostracism reaches the same conclusion. As be motivated to aggress when rejected. In fact, some Williams (2001) noted, "Ostracism threatens targets' recent experiments demonstrate that rejected people self-esteem because it is associated with punishment; it actively manage their moods, and that quick mood carries with it the implicit accusation that the target has management after rejection is correlated with height- done something wrong" (p. 61). Thus, when people are ened aggression (Twenge, Cacho, & MacDonald, ostracized, they begin to what is wrong with 2004). them. This musing may lead to lowered self-esteem as ostracized people begin to think that other people do Aggression as Social Influence not find them worthy of association. Given that most ego-threats result from events that A fourth explanation suggests that people aggress connote real or potential relational devaluation (Leary when rejected as a social influence tactic—either to in- & Baumeister, 2000; Leary et al., 1995), we can just as timidate the individual into not abandoning them or to easily say that these effects are caused by perceived re- show more generally that they are not to be devalued or lational devaluation as by threats to the individual's dismissed. One way to induce other people to do as one ego or self-esteem. When people are criticized, ig- wishes is to threaten them with harm, but such threats nored, or rejected, they are likely to perceive that other lose their power unless the individual carries through people do not adequately value their relationships and on them at least occasionally. Viewed in this way, ag- experience lowered state self-esteem. Thus, viewed gression can be viewed as a social infiuence tactic from this perspective, rejection-based aggression (Felson, 1978; Tedeschi, Smith, & Brown, 1974). arises not from threats to one's ego or self-esteem per Post-rejection aggression can be viewed as an effort to se but rather from the that one's relational infiuence other people not to reject the individual. Peo- value is low. These two competing ways of conceptual- ple are often likely to aggress, for example, when they izing the mediating role of ego-threat on rejec- are publicly insulted, demeaned, or devalued (Bond & tion-elicited aggression could be contrasted by com- Venus, 1991; Felson, 1978, 1982). In such cases, the paring situations in which the reason for being rejected aggressive action may be performed as much for its im- does or does not threaten the individual's ego. pact on onlookers as on the rejector. By aggressing, the offended party can demonstrate, not only to the rejector but to others as well, that he or she is not to be Mood Improvement devalued, rejected, or treated with disrespect. This is People who experience aversive emotions under- presumably the process that operates in a culture of standably desire to be rid of them. Because unex- honor, discussed earlier, in which people feel com- pressed anger may feel unpleasant and aggression may pelled to retaliate against aspersions upon their image, release anger, people who are angry might conceivably reputation, or honor. feel better after aggressing. Thus, aggression may Pinker (1997) made a similar point about aggres- sometimes be cathartic, not in the sense of reducing the sion as a means of deterring one's partner from being likelihood of subsequent aggression (a notion that has unfaithful or leaving altogether. Pinker offered an evo- been soundly discredited; Geen & Quanty, 1977), but lutionary argument to explain why aggression in such in terms of making people feel better afterwards. In circumstances is more common among men than fact, studies have found that angry people sometimes women (i.e., the risk of an unfaithful partner to one's find aggressive actions pleasurable (Bushman, reproductive fitness is greater for men due to parental Baumeister, & Stack, 1999). uncertainty), but the use of aggression to induce com- Support for the notion that people may aggress to pliance by one's partner is not confined to men. Of alleviate aversive feelings of anger is provided by a se- course, people typically do not want to hurt their loved ries of studies by Bushman and colleagues (1999). ones, yet they do so because occasional aggression is These studies showed that people were more likely to needed to maintain the credibility of their threats. As aggress after being insulted if they believed that ag- Pinker observed, "For every killing of an estranged gressing would make them feel better, either because wife or girlfriend there must be thousands of threats they held existing beliefs to that effect or were pro- made credible by signs that the man is crazy enough to vided with information that aggressing was cathartic. carry them out regardless of the cost" (p. 489). Interestingly, Bushman and colleagues found little evi- In his classic study of violent men, Toch (1992) dis- dence that aggressing actually reduced angry partici- cussed two types of men who are frequently violent to pants' negative moods. In fact, participants who be- promote or defend their public images. In Toch's tax- lieved in the usefulness of venting actually felt worse onomy, a self-image promoter "works hard at manu-

125 LEARY, TWENGE, QUINLIVAN

facturing the impression that he is not to be trified Warburton and colleagues (2003) supported the with.... He goes out of his way to make sure that peo- idea that control might play a role in the link between ple understand how important he is and how important rejection and aggression. In this study, participants it is to him that he is important" (p. 135). Such men pre- could either stop and start blasts of unpleasant noise in emptively strike so as not to be seen as weak or insig- the laboratory or had no control over the aversive noise. nificant and thereby to maintain their infiuence over Compared to participants who were not ostracized or others. A self-image defender, in contrast, is excep- who were ostracized and had control, ostracized partic- tionally "sensitive to the implications of other people's ipants who had no control assigned a peer to eat four actions to his integrity, manliness, or worth" (p. 141). times as much hot sauce. Ostracized people who had In essence, both types seem to be trying to stave off at- no control over an unpleasant environment apparently tributions that may lead people to value them less as so- chose to exercise some control by aggressing against cial interactants and group members. another person. Although most of the school shootings seem to be in Presumably, rejected individuals use aggression as retribution (to be discussed later), at least some of the a means of control primarily when they believe that ag- perpetrators were seeking public respect as well. After gressive actions will lower the likelihood that others killing three and injuring five in West Paducah, Ken- will exclude, ostracize, or abandon them. The range of tucky, Michael Carneal was quoted as saying "People situations in which aggression deters rejection is prob- respect me now." Similarly, Klebold and Harris, the ably rather small, confined to cases where there is a Columbine killers, fantasized that they would be fa- large status or power differential between the individu- mous, garner respect, and that movie directors would als, the rejecting individual has few options for retalia- fight over making a movie of their story (Gibbs & tion, and the rejected individual is more concerned Roche, 1999). Thus, the search for respect may have with maintaining a nominal relationship with the been one of the behind Columbine and rejector as opposed to being valued, liked or loved as other school shootings (Twenge & Campbell, 2003). an individual. This explanation differs from those presented thus far in regarding aggression as an interpersonal tactic intended to infiuence others' perceptions or behavior rather than a relatively automatic reaction to Related to this explanation is a seventh one: that aversiveness, frustration, negative mood, or ego-threat. people aggress when rejected as revenge or retaliation We are not suggesting that people necessarily consider (Ayduk et al., 1999). The desire for revenge has been the tactical pros and cons of aggression but rather that cited as the basis of a wide array of antisocial behav- their goal is sometimes to infiuence other people's im- iors, including homicide, rape, and other acts of ag- pressions of and reactions to them. gression (Counts, 1987; Pfefferbaum & Wood, 1994; Scully & Marolla, 1985). Furthermore, a sense of in- justice—the basis of all revenge—is a primary cause of Reestablishing Efficacy and Control anger and aggression (Brown, 1986). Rejected participants might also seek to establish According to McCullogh, Bellah, Kilpatrick, and self-efficacy and control. In several experiments, Wil- Johnson (2001), vengeance may serve at least three liams and his colleagues have shown that ostracism functions. First, it balances the interpersonal scales, re- leads to decreased feelings of control (Williams et al., storing equity by repaying harm with harm. As we 2002; Zadro et al., 2004). Williams (2001; Williams & have seen, rejection is experienced as painful (and may Zadro, 2001) suggested that many of the behaviors of even utilize neural pathways involved in aspects of people who feel ostracized refiect efforts to reestablish physical pain; MacDonald & Leary, 2005), so that peo- control. Because being ignored, shunned, and disre- ple who are rejected may restore equity by distributing garded threatens a person's sense of efficacy, people the pain. Second, vengeance may serve as moral in- may be motivated to regain it. Thus, verbal and physi- struction intended to teach the offender "a lesson," typ- cal aggression may serve to elicit a response—any re- ically that his or her behavior is unacceptable and will sponse—from the otherwise unresponsive rejector. In- not be tolerated. In the case of rejection, the lesson terviews with victims of long-term ostracism showed sometimes involves the perpetrator's global dismissive that they would sometimes behave provocatively, by mistreatment of people and sometimes it involves the yelling, insulting, throwing things, or being physically perpetrator's treatment of the person exacting revenge. combative, to get a reaction. In such instances, it may In either case, the message is that the individual can not not matter that the reaction itself is an angry, unpleas- get away with treating others in this way. Third, ven- ant one; any reaction may be better than none. Success- geance may serve as a face-saving tactic, as in cultures fully eliciting even an angry response may be hailed as of honor discussed earlier. If the rejected individual a victory by the rejected individual (Williams, perceives that the rejector regards him or her negatively Bernieri, Faulkner, Grahe, & Gada-Jain, 2000). and as not worthy of better treatment, aggression may

126 REJECTION AND AGGRESSION send the message, both to the perpetrator and to on- pected to interact further with their game partners were lookers, that the individual is worthy of respect and not aggressive. In another experiment, rejected partici- should not be mistreated (Kim & Smith, 1993). pants were aggressive toward targets who had been ac- Evidence that rejection-based aggression is some- cepted by another group (and, thus, seemed dissimilar) times motivated by revenge is scarce, although several and against targets rejected by another group (e.g., writers have suggested that people may seek revenge low-status individuals). However, they were not ag- for being rejected. For example. Buss (1961) noted that gressive against neutral members of another group, revenge typically occurs after a person has had a possibly because these individuals were perceived as chance to "mull over rejections, attacks, and disap- possible sources of acceptance. In addition. Experi- pointment" (p. 15). In their review of recent school ment 3 of Twenge and colleagues (2001) showed that shootings, Leary and colleagues (2003) suggested that excluded participants were not aggressive toward the primary motive in most of the school shootings someone who had praised them, although they were seems to have been retribution, either for an ongoing highly aggressive toward someone who had insulted pattern of ostracism and teasing or for an acute rejec- them. If a participant was told that no one had chosen tion such as a romantic breakup. In fact, many of the to work with him or her but was subsequently treated cases were characterized by both an ongoing pattern of kindly by the likely target of aggression, then aggres- rejection and a specific rejection experience, suggest- sive behavior did not result. Taken together, these re- ing that the recent rejection may have been the straw sults suggest that rejected people act aggressively to- that broke the camel's back. ward people who are not potential relational partners In most cases of revenge, the retaliatory response is (e.g., those who are unavailable for future interaction, more intense and harmful than the original precipitat- dissimilar to them, low in status, or unfriendly). How- ing event (Axelrod, 1984), and this pattern is often seen ever, rejected people are less likely to behave aggres- when people aggress after being rejected. No matter sively toward people who could potentially provide how hurtful rejection might be, beating, maiming, or meaningful acceptance (e.g., those they will interact killing the rejector hardly seems commensurate with with in the future, are neutral representatives from an- being rejected. In light of this, people are likely to ex- other , or are friendly people). act revenge against those who reject them only when The second experiment of Williams, Cheung, and they have given up the goal of being accepted and are colleagues (2000) goes even further, showing in- motivated by pure retribution. creased efi'orts to be accepted when rejected individu- als interact with a new group. Participants who were ostracized or included in a computerized ball-tossing Loosening Social Inhibitions game later did a perception task with a new group of to Aggress participants. Participants who had earlier been ostra- Another explanation suggests that rejection does cized were more likely to conform with the incorrect not cause aggression but rather loosens the constraints judgments of the new group members. on aggression that exist between people who are on The loosening-of-restraints-against-aggression hy- good terms with one another. People who feel socially pothesis may help to explain why aggression occurs accepted have an incentive to act nonaggressively and only occasionally after rejection. Being rejected often prosocially because they do not wish to harm a valu- makes people indifferent, rather than hostile, to others' able relationship or jeopardize others' goodwill and welfare, so in many cases, they simply withdraw from support. However, once they feel rejected, the costs of contact with the rejector rather than aggress. However, behaving antisocially are lowered. As a result, inhibi- if conditions arise under which aggression may serve a tions against aggression are weakened should the urge function (in terms of social infiuence, establishing con- to aggress arise for other reasons. Thus, rejected peo- trol, improving mood, retribution, or whatever), the ple may feel that they have nothing to lose by being ag- fact that the dissolution of social bonds loosens re- gressive, especially if they do not believe the other per- straints against aggression increases the likelihood of son will ever accept them. aggressive behavior. Some recent experimental research supports this ex- planation, showing that rejection leads to aggression only when the target is not a potential source of belong- Lowered Self-Control ing (Twenge et al., 2004). In one experiment, partici- pants experienced rejection or acceptance, and heard Resisting one's aggressive impulses requires that they would either interact or not interact with their self-control. For example, relationship partners low in partner in a noise-blasting game. Replicating previous self-control are more likely to give in to impulses to re- research, rejected participants who did not expect fu- spond destructively to their partners by picking a fight, ture interaction were more aggressive than accepted sulking, or issuing an insult (Finkel & Campbell, participants. However, rejected participants who ex- 2001). After a provocation, yelling and hitting are of-

127 LEARY, TWENGE, QUINLIVAN ten natural and easy reactions, and people sometimes heighten the likelihood of aggression, the pervasive- find it difficult to control these aggressive urges. ness and robustness of the rejection-aggression effect Unfortunately, rejection itself may reduce is not surprising. We that future research will test self-control. A series of experiments showed that so- each of these explanations and identify the domain of cially excluded participants are more likely to fail at application for each. self-regulation (Baumeister, DeWall, Ciarocco, & Twenge, 2004). Rejected participants ate more cookies in the context of a taste test, were able to drink fewer Conclusions ounces of an unpleasant beverage despite a monetary incentive, did not persist as long in solving puzzles, Our review has demonstrated that interpersonal re- and had trouble attending to information in a dichotic jection affects not only aggressive behavior but also listening task. In another study, excluded participants mediators of aggression such as anger and the deroga- tended to make poor, self-defeating choices such as tion of other people. A relationship between rejection choosing a risky but less profitable lottery, eating un- and aggression has been demonstrated in numerous healthy food, and procrastinating rather than studying experimental, correlational, and longitudinal studies for a test (Twenge, Catanese, & Baumeister, 2002). that have been conducted on varied samples using Rejection may reduce self-control because people many different operationalizations of rejection and ag- do not process information as deeply or carefully after gression. Furthermore, we have identified nine possi- being rejected. A series of experiments showed that ble explanations of the effect, some of which have been people who were socially excluded experienced decre- directly supported by research and others for which the ments in cognitive performance (Baumeister, Twenge, evidence is only suggestive. & Nuss, 2002). Apparently, rejection undermines cog- Throughout our review, we were continually struck nitive processing so that rejected people do not analyze by the paradox of the rejection-aggression effect. situations as accurately, consider their options as care- Given that human beings are highly motivated to be ac- fully, or have the necessary cognitive resources to regu- cepted and to belong to social groups (Baumeister & late themselves effectively. As a result, they may go Leary, 1995), one might expect that people who fmd with their initial impulse to act in their short-term (but themselves socially rejected should quickly take steps not long-term) interest without engaging in cognitive to correct this situation, seeking ways to rejoin groups elaboration and self-regulation as they otherwise and re-establish relationships. In fact, this often hap- might. Given that aggression is typically an impulsive pens, as when people apologize for their misdeeds, behavior that may feel good in the short term but lead seek approval from those who have formed unflatter- to negative consequences in the long run, failing to ing impressions of them, do favors for those who are elaborate on long-term implications may impair the in- angry with them, send to estranged loved ones dividual's ability to regulate aggressive actions. Al- with whom they want to reconcile, and work hard to though it is not clear why rejection undermines convince others that they deserve to belong to a partic- thoughtful cognition, one possibility is that rejected in- ular group. Yet, in most of the research that we re- dividuals devote attention to regulating their emotional viewed, rejection was associated with higher aggres- reactions, leaving them with inadequate resources sion. Rather than placate those who rejected them and needed for self-control (Muraven, Tice, & Baumeister, seek to reestablish their social ties, people became an- 1998; Twenge et al., 2004). gry, punitive, and aggressive. In many ways, this be- This explanation is only partial, however, because it havior seems irrational and counter-productive be- does not account for why rejected individuals behave cause acting aggressively is more likely to drive people aggressively when they lose self-control as opposed to away than to bring them closer. As we have seen, there acting in some other uncontrolled fashion. Even so, it may be hidden benefits to rejection-induced aggres- suggests that people who feel aggressive after being re- sion (in terms of releasing frustration, infiuencing oth- jected may have more difficulty controlling their anti- ers, or establishing control, for example), yet this effect social impulses. may maintain a vicious circle of rejection and aggres- sion in which people who are rejected lash out with ag- gression that leads to further rejection. Summary Our review has focused on instances of interper- The literature provides little or no basis for choos- sonal rejection in which a person is rejected by one or ing among these nine hypotheses, and we suspect that more other individuals, presumably for personal rea- most, if not all of them may have some merit. Further- sons. Yet, other lines of research also show that people more, these nine processes appear to be relatively dis- respond aggressively when their group is devalued, tinct and are not easily subsumed within each other or avoided, or excluded by members of other groups within broader, more general processes. Given the (Miller & Major, 2000; Struch & Schwartz, 1989). number of potential routes by which rejection may However, what is unclear is whether people experience

128 REJECTION AND AGGRESSION rejection based on group membership as merely a spe- Berkowitz, L. (1983). Aversively-stimulated aggression: Some par- cial case of interpersonal rejection or whether group allels and differences in research with animals and . American Psychologist, 38, 1135-1144. membership intensifies, attenuates, or otherwise Berkowitz, L. (1989). Frustration-aggression hypothesis: Examina- changes their reaction (see Crocker & Major, 1989; tion and reformulation. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 59-73. Miller & Major, 2000). Berkowitz, L. (1993). Aggression: Its causes, consequences, and We must reemphasize that aggression is by no control. New York: McGraw-Hill. means the only—or even the most common—response Bond, M. H., & Venus, C. K. (1991). Resistance to group or personal insults in an ingroup or outgroup context. International Journal to rejection. People who are rejected sometimes react of Psychology, 26, 83-94. in nonaggressive ways, either by merely withdrawing Bourgeois, K. S., & Leary, M. R. (2001). with rejection: Der- from interactions with those who have rejected them ogating those who choose us last. and Emotion. 25, (Leary et al., 1998; Williams, 2001) or by behaving in 101-111. ways that they believe will increase their relational Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss (Vol. 1: Attachment). Lon- value in others' eyes (e.g., Williams, Cheung, et al., don: Hogarth Press. Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss (Vol. 2: Separation). Lon- 2000; Williams & Sommer, 1997). Even so, our review don: Hogarth Press. suggests that perceived rejection (or, more precisely, Bowlby, J. (1984). Violence in the family as a disorder of the attach- perceived low relational evaluation) causes angry, ag- ment and caregiving systems. American Journal of Psycho- gressive reactions under some circumstances. Consid- analysis. 44, 9-27. erable research is needed to understand fully the condi- Branch, C. (Ed.). (1999). Adolescent gangs: Old issues, new ap- tions under which these reactions occur and the proaches. New York: Routledge. Brown, R. (1986). . New York: Rree Press. psychological processes that underlie them. Buckiey, K. E., Winkel, R. E., & Leary, M. R. (2004). Emotional and behavioral responses to interpersonal rejection: Anger, sadness, hurt, and aggression. Journal of Experimental Social Psychol- ogy, 40, 14-28. References Bukowski, W. M., & Newcomb, A. R (1984). Stability and determi- nants of sociometric status and choice: A longitudi- Axelrod, R. (1984). The of cooperation. New York: Basic nal perspective. , 20,941-952. Books. Burks, V, Dodge, K. A., & Price, J. M. (1995). Models of internaliz- Ayduk, O., Downey, G., Testa, A., & Yen, Y (1999). Does rejection ing outcomes of early rejection. Development and elicit hostility in rejection sensitive women? Social Cognition, Psychopathology, 7, 683-695. 17,245-211. Bushman, B. J., & Baumeister, R. R (1998). Threatened egotism, Azrin, N. H. (1967, May). Pain and aggression. Psychology Today, narcissism, self-esteem, and direct and displaced aggression: 27-33. Does self- or self-hate lead to violence? Journal of Person- Bagwell, C. L., Newcomb, A. R, & Bukowski, W. M. (1998). ality and Social Psychology, 75, 219-229. Preadoiescent friendship and peer rejection as predictors of Bushman, B. J., Baumeister, R. R, & Phillips, C. M. (2001). Do peo- adult adjustment. Child Development, 69, 140-153, ple aggress to improve their mood? Catharsis beliefs, affect reg- Barnard, G. W., Vera, H., Vera, M. I., & Newman, G. (1982). Till ulation opportunity, and aggressive responding. Journal of Per- death do us part; A study of spouse murder. Bulletin of the sonality and Social Psychology, 81. 17-32. American Academy of and the Law, 10, 271-280. Bushman, B. J., Baumeister, R. R, & Stack, A. D. (1999). Catharsis, Bamow, S., Schuckit, M. A., Lucht, M., John, U., & Freyberger, H. J. aggression, and persuasive influence: Self-fulfilling or (2002). The importance of a positive family history of alcohol- seif-defeating prophecies? Journal of Personality and Social ism, parental rejection and emotional warmth, behavioral prob- Psychology, 76, 367-376. lems and peer substance use for alcohol problems in teenagers: Bushman, B. J., Bonacci, A. M., van Dijk, M., & Baumeister, R. F. A path analysis. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 63, 305-315. (2003). Narcissism, sexual refusal, and aggression: Testing a Baron, R. A., & Richardson, D. R. (1994). Human aggression (2nd narcissistic reactance model of sexual . Journal of Per- ed.). New York: Plenum. sonality and Social Psychology, 84, 1087-1040. Bartholomew, K. (1990). Avoidance of intimacy: An attachment per- Buss, A. H. (1961). The psychology of aggression. New York: spective. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 7, Wiley. 147-178. Cairns, R. B., Cadwallader, T. W., Estell, D. B., & Neckerman, H. J. Baumeister, R. R, Catanese, K. R., & Wallace, H. M. (2002). Con- (1997). Groups to gangs: Developmental and criminoiogical quest by force: A narcissistic reactance theory of rape and sex- perspectives and relevance for prevention. In D. M. Stoff, J. D. ual coercion. Review of General Psychology, 6, 92-135. Maser, & J. Breiling (Eds.), Handbook of antisocial behavior Baumeister, R. R, DeWall, C. N. J., Ciarocco, N. J., & Twenge, J. M. (pp. 194-204). New York: Wiley. (2004). Social exclusion impairs self-regulation. Unpublished Cairns, R. B,, & Cairns, B. D. (1994). Lifelines and risks: Pathways manuscript, Florida State University, Tallahassee. of youth in our time. New York: Cambridge University Press. Baumeister, R. R, & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire Cicchetti, D., & Bukowski, W. M. (1995). Developmental processes for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motiva- in peer relationships and psychpathology. Development and tion. Psychological Bulletin, 117,497-529. Psychopathology, 7,587-589. Baumeister, R. R, Smart, L., & Boden, J. M. (1996). Relation of Cohen, D., Nisbett, R. E., Bowdle, B., & Schwarz, N. (1996). Insult, threatened egotism to violence and aggression: The dark side of aggression, and the Southern culture of honor: An "experimen- high self-esteem. Psychological Review, 103, 5-33. tal ethnography." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Baumeister, R. R, Twenge, J. M., & Nuss, C. K. (2002). Effects of 70, 945-960. social exclusion on cognitive processes: Anticipated aioneness Coie, J. D., & Dodge, K. A. (1983). Continuities and changes in chil- reduces intelligent thought. Journal of Personality and Social dren's social status: A five-year longitudinal study. Merrill Psychology, 83, 817-827. Palmer Quarterly, 29, 261-282.

129 LEARY, TWENGE, QUINLIVAN

Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., & Kupersmidt, J. B. (1990). be- Finkel, E. J., & Campbell, W. K. (2001). Self-control and accommo- havior and social status. In S. R. Asher & J, D. Coie (Eds.), Peer dation in close relationships: An interdependence analysis. rejection in childhood (pp. 17-59). New York: Oxford Univer- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 263-277. sity Press. Frijda, N. (1986). The emotions. New York: Cambridge University Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., Terry, R., Wright, V. (1991). The role of Press. aggression in peer relations: An analysis of aggression episodes Garbarino, J. (1999). Lost boys: Why our sons turn violent and how in boys' play groups. Child Development, 62, 812-826. we can save them. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Counts, D. A. (1987). Femaie and wife abuse: A Gardner, W. L., Pickett, C. L., & Brewer, M. B. (2000). Social exclu- cross-cultural perspective. Suicide and Life-Threatening Be- sion and selective memory: How the need to belong influences haviors, 17, 194-204. memory for social events. Personality and Social Psychology Crawford, M., & Gartner, R. (1992). Women killing: Intimate Bulletin, 26,486-496. femicide in Ontario, 1974-1990. Toronto: Women We Honour Garlick, Y, Marshall, W. L., & Thornton, D. (1996). Intimacy defi- Action Committee. cits and attribution of blame among sexual offenders. Legal and Crocker, J., & Major, B. (1989). and self-esteem: The Criminoiogical Psychology, 1, 251-258. self-protective of stigma. Psychological Review, 96, Geen, R. G. (1990). Human aggression. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/ 608-630. Cole. Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (i988). Homicide. New York: Aldine De Geen, R. G., & Quanty, M. B. (1977). The catharsis of aggression: Gruyter. An evaluation of a hypothesis. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances DeRosier, M., Kupersmidt, J., & Patterson, C. (1994). Children's ac- in experimental social psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 1-37). New ademic and behavioral adjustment as a function of the York: Academic. chronicity and proximity of peer rejection. Child Development, Geller, D. M., Goodstein, L., Silver, M., & Sternberg, W. C. (1974). 65, 1799-1813. On being ignored: The effects of the violation of implicit rules De Weerth, C, & Kalma, A. P (1993). Female aggression as a re- of social interaction. Sociometry, 37, 541-556. sponse to sexuai jealousy: A sex role reversal? Aggressive Be- Gibbs, N., & Roche, T (1999, Dec. 20). The Columbine tapes. havior, 19, 265-279. Newsweek, 154 (25), 40-51, Dodge, K. A. (1983). Behavioral antecedents of peer social status. Gonzaga, G, C, Keltner, D., Londahl, E. A., & Smith, M. D. (2001). Child Development, 54, 1386-1989. Love and the commitment problem in romantic relations and Dollard, J., Doob, L. W., Miller, N. E., Mowrer, O. H., & Sears, R. R. friendship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, (1939). Frustration and aggression. New Haven, CT: Yaie Uni- 247-262. versity Press. Harmon-Jones, E., & Sigelman, J. (2001). State anger and prefrontal Downey, G., & Feidman, S. I. (1996). Implications of rejection sen- brain activity: Evidence that insult-related relative sitivity for intimate relationships. Journal of Personality and left-prefrontal activation is associated with experienced anger Social Psychology, 70, 1327-1343. and aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Downey, G., Feldman, S., & Ayduk, O. (2000). Rejection sensitivity 80, 797-803. and male violence in romantic relationships. Personal Relation- Holtzworth-Munroe, A., & Hutchinson, G. (1993). Attributing nega- ships, 7,45-61. tive intent to wife behavior: The attributions of maritally violent Downey, G., Freitas, A. L., Michaelis, B., & Khouri, H. (1998). The versus nonviolent men. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102. self-fulfilling prophecy in close relationships: Rejection sensi- 206-211. tivity and rejection by romantic partners. Journal of Personality Holtzworth-Munroe, A., Stuart, G. L., & Hutchinson, G. (1997). Vi- and Social Psychology, 75, 545-560. olent versus nonviolent husbands: Differences in attachment Dutton, D. G. (1988). The domestic assault of women: Psychological patterns, dependency, and jealousy. Journal of Family Psychol- and criminal justice perspectives. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. ogy, 77,314-331. Dutton, D.G. (1998). The abusive personality. New York: Guilford. James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. Chicago: Encyclo- Dutton, D. G., Saunders, K., Starzomski, A., & Bartholomew, K. pedia Britannica. (1994). Intimacy-anger and insecure attachment as precursors Kim, S. H., & Smith, R. H. (1993, January). Revenge and conflict es- of abuse in intimate relationships. Journal of Applied Social calation. Negotiation Journal, pp. 37-43. Psychology, 24, 1367-1386. Kirkpatrick, L. A., Waugh, C. E., Valencia, A., & Webster, G. D. Dutton, D. G., Starzomski, A., & Ryan, L. (1996). Antecedents of (2002). The functional domain specificity of self-esteem and abusive personality and abusive behavior in wife assaulters. the differential prediction of aggression. Journal of Personality Journal of Family Violence, 11, 113-132. and Social Psychology, 82,15(>-1(>1. Dutton, D. G., van Ginkei, C, & Landolt, M. A. (1996). Jealousy, in- Kupersmidt, J. B., Burchinal, M., & Patterson, C. J. (1995). Develop- timate abusiveness, and intrusiveness. Journal of Family Vio- mental patterns of childhood peer relations as predictors of lence, 11, 411^23. externalizing behavior problems. Development and Edens, J. R (1999). Aggressive children's self-systems and the qual- Psychopathology. 7, 825-843. ity of their relationships with significant others. Aggression and Kupersmidt, J. B., & Coie, J. D. (1990). Preadolescent peer status, violent behavior, 4, 151-177. aggression and school adjustment as predictors of externalizing Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). problems in adolescence. Child Development. 61, 1350-1362. Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Sci- Kupersmidt, J. B., Coie, J. D., & Dodge, K. A. (1990). The role of ence, 302, 290-292. poor peer relationships in the development of disorder. In S. R. Feldman, S. I., & Downey, G. (1994). Rejection sensitivity as a me- Asher & J. D. Coie (Eds.), Peer rejection in childhood (pp. diator of the impact of childhood exposure to family violence 274-305). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. on adult attachment behavior. Development and Psycho- Leary, M. R. (2001). Toward a conceptualization of interpersonal re- pathology, 6,23\-241. jection. In M. R. Leary (Ed.), Interpersonal rejection (pp. Felson, R. B. (1978). Aggression as . Social 3-20). New York: Oxford University Press. Psychology Quarterly, 41, 205-213. Leary, M. R. (2005). Varieties of interpersonal rejection. In K. D. Felson, R. B. (1982). Impression management and the escalation of Williams, J. P. Forgas, & W. von Hippel (Eds.), The social out- aggression and violence. Social Psychology Quarterly, 45, cast: Ostracism, social exclusion, rejection, and bullying. New 245-254. York: Psychology Press. 130 REJECTION AND AGGRESSION

Leary, M. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). The nature and function of McKibben, A., Proulx, J., & Lusignan, R. (1994). Relationships be- self-esteem: theory. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances tween conflict, affect and deviant sexual behaviors in rapists in experimental social psychology (Vol. 32, pp. 1-62). San and child molesters. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, Diego, CA: Academic. 571-575. Leary, M. R., Koch, E. J., & Hechenbieikner, N. R. (2001). Emo- Meesters, C, & Muris, P (1996). The relationship between hostility tional responses to interpersonal rejection. In M. R. Leary (Ed.), and perceived parental rearing behaviors: A study of male myo- Interpersonal rejection (pp. 145-188). New York: Oxford Uni- cardial infarction patients and healthy controls. Personality and versity Press. Individual Differences, 21, 271-281. Leary, M. R., Kowalski, R. M., Smith, L., & Phillips, S. (2003). Merten, D. E. (1994). The cultural context of aggression: The transi- Teasing, rejection, and violence: Case studies of the school tion to junior high school. Anthropology and Education Quar- shootings. Aggressive Behavior, 29, 202-214 terly 25, 29-43. Leary, M. R., & Springer, C. (2000). Hurt feelings: The neglected Miller, C. T, & Major, B. (2000). Coping with prejudice and stigma. emotion. In R. M. Kowalski (Ed.), Behaving badly: Aversive In T. Heatherton, R. Kleck, & J. G. Hull (Eds.), The social psy- behaviors in interpersonal relationships (pp. 151-175). Wash- chology of stigma (pp. 243-272). New York: Guilford. ington, DC: American Psychological Association. Miller, R. S. (1997). We always hurt the ones we love: Aversive inter- Leary, M. R., Springer, C, Negel, L., Ansell, E., & Evans, K. (1998). actions in close relationships. In R. M. Kowalski (Ed.), Aversive The causes, phenomenology, and consequences of hurt feel- interpersonal behaviors (pp. 11-29). New York: Plenum. ings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 74, Muraven, M., Tice, D.M., & Baumeister, R.E (1998). Self-control as 1225-1237. limited resource: Regulatory depletion patterns. Journal of Per- Leary, M. R., Tambor, E. S., Terdal, S. K. & Downs, D. L. (1995) sonality and Social Psychology, 74, 774-789. Self-esteem as an interpersonal monitor: The sociometer hy- Newcomb, A. F., Bukowski, W. M., & Pattee, L. (1993). Children's pothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, peer relations: A meta-analytic review of popular, rejected, ne- 518-530. glected, controversial, and average sociometric status. Psycho- Lefkowitz, M. M., Huesman, L. R., Walder, L. O., & Eron, L. D. logical Bulletin, 113, 99-128. (1973). Environmental variables as predictors of aggressive be- Nisbett, R. E. (1993). Violence and U.S. regional culture. American havior. International Journal of Group Tensions. 3, 30-47. Psychologist. 48. 441-449. Lester, D. (1994). Time-series analysis of the murder and homicide Nisbett, R. E., & Cohen, D. (1996). Culture of honor: The psychol- rates in the USA. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 79, 862. ogy of violence in the South. Denver, CO: Westview Press. Levin, J., & Fox, J. A. (1991). Mass murder: America's growing Office of the Surgeon General. (2001). Youth violence: A report of the menace. New York: Berkley Books. Surgeon General. U.S. Department of and Human Ser- Levy, S. R., & Ayduk, O., & Downey, G. (2001). The role of rejection vices. Retrieved August, 2003 from http:// sensitivity in people's relationships with significant others and www.mentalhealth.org/youthviolence/default.asp valued social groups. In M. R. Leary (Ed.), Interpersonal rejec- Ojha, H., & Pramanick, M. (1995). Parental behavior as related to tion (pp. 251-289). New York: Oxford University Press. some personality traits of adolescents. Psychologia, JS, 31-37. Little, S. A., & Garber, J. (1995). Aggression, depression, and - Palermo, G. B. (1997). The berserk syndrome: A review of mass ful life events predicting peer rejection in children. Develop- murder. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 2, 1-8. ment and Psychopathology, 7, 845-856. Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1987). Peer relations and later personal Mabel, S. (1994). Empirical determination of anger provoking char- adjustment: Are low-accepted children at risk? Psychological acteristics intrinsic to anger provoking circumstances. Journal Bulletin, 102, 357-389. of Social and Clinical Psychology, 13, 174-188. Patterson, C. J., Kupersmidt, J. B., & Griesler, P. C. (1990). MacDonald, G., & Leary, M. R. (2005). Why does social exclusion Children's perceptions of self and of relationships with others hurt? The relationship between social and physical pain. Psy- as a function of sociometric status. Child Development, 61, chological Bulletin. 131, 202-223. 1335-1349. Makepeace, J. (1989). Dating, living together, and vio- Patterson, G. R., Reid, J. R., & Dishion, T. J. (1992). Antisocial boys. lence. In M. A. Pirog-Good & J. Stets (Eds.), Violence in dating Eugene, OR: Castalia. relationships (pp. 94-107). New York: Praeger. Pemberton, D. A., & Benady, D. R. (1973). Consciously rejected Malmquist, C. (1996). Homicide: A psychiatric perspective. Wash- children. British Journal of Psychiatry. 123, 578-578. ington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Pepitone, A., & Wilpizeski, C. (1960). Some consequences of exper- Marchand, J. (2004). Husbands' and wives' marital quality: The role imental rejection. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, of adult attachment orientations, depressive symptoms, and 60, 359-364. behaviors. Attachment and Human Develop- Perlstein, L. (1999, November 2). In Maryland, exploring ways to ment, 6, 99-112. promote safety; Student perspectives on violence. The Wash- Marshall, W. L., & Hambley, L. S. (1996). Intimacy and loneliness, ington Post, p. B02. and their relationship to rape myth acceptance and hostility to- Peterson, K. S. (1999, May 6). Teens understand how taunts lead to ward women among rapists. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, crimes. The Des Moines Register, p. 3. 11, 586-592. Pfefferbaum, B., & Wood, P B. (1994). Self-report study of impul- Mathews, K. A., Woodall, K. L., Kenyon, K., & Jacob, T (1996). sive and delinquent behavior in college students. Journal of Ad- Negative family environment as a predictor of hostile attitudes, olescent Health, 15, 295-302. interview behavior, and anger expression. Health Psychology, Pickett, C. L., Gardner, W. L., & Knowles, M. (2004). Getting a cue: 15, 30-37. The need to belong and enhanced sensitivity to social cues. Per- McCullough, M. E., Bellah, C. G., Kilpatrick, S. D., & Johnson, J. L. sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1095-1107. (2001). Vengefulness: Relationships with , rumina- Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York: Norton. tion, well-being, and the big five. Personality and Social Psy- Raine, A., Brennan, P, & Mednick, S. A. (1994). Birth complica- chology Bulletin, 27, 601-610. tions combined with early maternal rejection at age 1 year pre- McDougall, P., Hymel, S., Vaillancourt, T, & Mercer, L. (2001). The dispose to violent crime at age 18 years. Archives of General consequences of childhood peer rejection. In M. R. Leary (Ed.), Psychiatry, 51, 984-988. Interpersonal rejection (pp. 213-247). New York: Oxford Uni- Ressler, R. K., Burgess, A. W., & Douglas, J. E. (1988). Sexual homi- versity Press. cide: Patterns and motives. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.

131 LEARY, TWENGE, QUINLIVAN

Rohner, R. P. (1975). They love me, they love me not: A worldwide Twenge, J. M., & Campbell, W. K. (2003). "Isn't it fun to get the re- study of the effects of parental acceptance and rejection. New spect that we're going to deserve?": Narcissism, social rejec- Haven, CT: Human Relations Area Files Press. tion, and aggression. Personality and Social Psychology Bulle- Rubin, K. H., Bukowski, W., & Parker, J. G. (1998). Peer interac- tin, 29. 261-272. tions, relationships, and groups. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg Twenge, J. M., Catanese, K. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2002). Social (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3, Social, emo- exclusion causes self-defeating behavior. Journal of Personal- tional, and personality development (5th ed., pp. 619-700). ity and Social Psychology, 83, 606-615. New York: Wiley. Twenge, J. M., Catanese, K. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2003). Social Sampson, R. J., & Laub, J. H. (1993). Crime in the making: Path- exclusion and the deconstructed state: Time perception, mean- ways and turning points through life. Cambridge, MA: Harvard inglessness, lethargy, lack of emotion, and self-awareness. University Press. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 409-423. Scully, D., & Marolla, J. (1985). "Riding the bull at Gilley's": Con- Twenge, J. M., Ciarocco, N. J., Cuervo, D., & Baumeister, R. F. victed rapists describe the rewards of rape. Social Problems, 32, (2003, October). Social exclusion reduces prosocial behavior. 251-262. Paper presented at the Preconference on the Self, annual meet- Seidman, B. T, Marshall, W. L., Hudson, S. M., & Robertson, P. ing of the Society for Experimental Social Psychology, Boston, (1994). An examination of intimacy and loneliness in sex of- MA. fenders. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. 9, 518—534. Warburton, W., Williams, K. D., & Cairns, D. (2003, April). Effects Snapp, C. M., & Leary, M. R. (2001). Hurt feelings among new ac- of ostracism and loss of control on aggression. Paper presented quaintances: Moderating effects of interpersonal familiarity. at the 32nd Annual Meeting of the Society of Australasian So- Journal of Personal and Social Relationships, 18, 315-326. cial Psychology, Sydney, Australia. Struch, N., & Schwartz, S. H. (1989). Intergroup aggression: Its pre- Williams, K. D. (1997). Social ostracism. In R. M. Kowalski (Ed.), dictors and distinctness from in-group bias. Journal of Person- Aversive interpersonal behaviors (pp. 133-170). New York: ality and Social Psychology, 56. 364-373. Plenum. Symonds, P. H. (1939). The psychology of parent-child relation- Williams, K. D. (2001). Ostracism: The power of silence. New York: ships. New York: Appleton Century. Guilford. Tedeschi, J. T., & Bond, M. H. (2001). Aversive behavior and aggres- Williams, K. D., Bernieri, E, Faulkner, S., Grahe, J., & Gada-Jain, N. sion in cultural perspective. In R. M. Kowalski (Ed.), Behaving (2000). The Scarlet Letter Study: Five days of social ostracism. badly: Aversive behaviors in interpersonal relationships (pp. Journal of Personal and Interpersonal Loss. 5, 19-63. 257-293). Washington, DC: American Psychological Williams, K. D., Cheung, C. K. T, & Choi, W. (2000). Association. CyberOstracism: Effects of being ignored over the Internet. Tedeschi, J. T, Smith, R. B., Ill, & Brown, R. C. (1974). A reinter- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 748-762. pretation of research on aggression. Psychological Bulletin, 81, Williams, K. D., Govan, C. L., Croker, V., T^nan, D., Cruickshank, 540-563. M., & Lam, A. (2002). Investigations into differences between Thomas, H. E. (1995). Experiencing as a precursor to vio- social- and Cyberostracism. Group Dynamics: Theory, Re- lence. Bulletin of the Academy of Psychiatry and Law, 23, search, and Practice, 6, f>5-ll. 587-593. Williams, K. D., & Sommer, K. L. (1997). Social ostracism by one's Toch, H. (1992). Violent men: An inquiry into the psychology of vi- coworkers: Does rejection lead to loafing or compensation? olence. Washington, DC: American Psychological Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 693-706. Association. Williams, K. D., & Zadro, L. (2001). Ostracism: On being ignored, Twenge, J. M., Baumeister, R. F., Tice, D. M., & Stucke, T. S. (2001). excluded, and rejected. In M. R. Leary (Ed.), Interpersonal re- If you can't join them, beat them: Effects of social exclusion on jection (pp. 21-53). New York: Oxford University Press. aggressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- Woodall, K. L., & Matthews, K. A. (1989). Familial environment as- ogy, 81, 1058-1069. sociated with Type A behaviors and psychophysiological re- Twenge, J. M., Cacho, J. C, & MacDonald, D. (2004, January). Ag- sponses to stress in children. Health Psychology. 8, 403-426. gression after social rejection: The effects of defensive of Zadro, L., Williams, K. D., & Richardson, R. (2004). How low can emotion and social situations. Paper presented at the Groups you go? Ostracism by a computer lowers mood, belonging, con- and Intergroup Processes Preconference, Society for Personal- trol, self-esteem, and meaningful existence. Journal of Experi- ity and Social Psychology, Austin, TX. mental Social Psychology, 40, 560-567.

132