Factors Determining the Development of Discourse Competence in Advanced Learners of English Author

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Factors Determining the Development of Discourse Competence in Advanced Learners of English Author View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE Title: Factors determining the development of discourse competence in advanced learners of English Author: Marcin Jaroszek Citation style: Jaroszek Marcin. (2008). Factors determining the development of discourse competence in advanced learners of English. Praca doktorska. Katowice : Uniwersytet Śląski MARCIN JAROSZEK FACTORS DETERMINING THE DEVELOPMENT OF DISCOURSE COMPETENCE IN ADVANCED LEARNERS OF ENGLISH A LONGITUDINAL STUDY Ph.D. Dissertation submitted in part fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy at the English Language Institute of the University of Silesia. Written under the supervision of prof. dr hab. Maria Wysocka UNIWERSYTET ŚLĄSKI 2008 MARCIN JAROSZEK CZYNNIKI OKREŚLAJĄCE ROZWÓJ KOMPETENCJI DYSKURSU U ZAAWANSOWANYCH UCZNIÓW JĘZYKA ANGIELSKIEGO BADANIE PODŁUŻNE Rozprawa doktorska napisana w Instytucie Języka Angielskiego Uniwersytetu Śląskiego pod kierunkiem prof. dr hab. Marii Wysockiej UNIWERSYTET ŚLĄSKI 2008 Table of contents INTRODUCTION 5 PART ONE: THEORETICAL ASPECTS OF DISCOURSE COMPETENCE 7 DEVELOPMENT 1. Discourse and Discourse Competence 7 1.1 Defining discourse 8 1.2 Discourse Competence and Communicative Competence 10 1.3 Discourse Devices 14 1.3.1 Interaction vs. intra-action in discourse construction 15 1.3.2 Turn-taking 17 1.3.3 Discourse markers 19 1.3.4 Back-channel responses 20 1.3.5 Grammatical cohesion and textuality 20 1.3.6 Lexis and discourse 24 1.3.7 Conjunction 27 1.3.8 Modality 28 1.4 Competence vs. Performance 32 2. Educational factors determining discourse competence development 35 2.1 The role of input in discourse competence development 35 2.2 Classroom discourse vs. natural discourse 39 2.2.1 Teacher talk 40 2.2.2 Classroom interaction 45 3. The role of transfer in discourse construction 55 3.1 Negative transfer vs. positive transfer 57 3.2 Discourse transfer 58 3.3 Language transfer requisites 63 4 Discourse and an advanced foreign language learner 65 4.1 The specificity of an advanced foreign language learner 65 4.2 Psycholinguistic factors and discourse competence 69 4.2.1 Individual differences in discourse construction 69 4.2.2 Personality 73 4.3 Sociolinguistic factors and discourse competence 76 4.3.1 Gender and discourse 76 4.3.2 Social class and discourse 80 4.3.3 Discourse and nation 84 5 Recapitulation 87 PART TWO: EMPIRICAL STUDIES 91 6. The concept of the longitudinal study 91 6.1 Objective 92 6.2 Research questions 92 6.3 Scheme of the study 94 3 6.4 Survey study 96 6.4.1 Method 96 6.4.2 Results 96 6.5 Pilot study 103 6.5.1 Method 103 6.5.2 Results 104 6.5.3 The effect of the survey study and the pilot study on the main study 107 6.6 The main study 109 6.6.1 Procedures 110 6.6.2 The course of the study 112 7. Study results: Factors determining the development of discourse competence 118 7.1 Introduction 118 7.2 Modality 120 7.2.1 Overall modality 120 7.2.2 Specific modality 122 7.2.3 Epistemic and deontic modality 126 7.2.4 Modality diversity 136 7.3 Conjunctions 139 7.3.1 Conjunctions: overall results 140 7.3.2 Specific conjunctions 142 7.3.3 Formal conjunctions 144 7.3.4 Conjunctions diversity 148 7.4 Relexicalization 151 7.5 Reiteration 155 7.6 Ellipsis and substitution 157 7.7 Classroom procedures 159 7.8 Recapitulation 161 FINAL CONCLUSIONS 164 APPENDIX 172 BIBLIOGRAPHY 216 SUMMARY 231 STRESZCZENIE 233 4 INTRODUCTION Nearly two decades from Poland’s opening to the innovative English as a Foreign Language (EFL) methodologies, the English language has become widely popularized in the country and the number of those speaking it has remarkably grown. Some have learned to pidginize English only, some use it accurately and fluently for professional purposes. It could seem then that these advanced users of English should demonstrate high levels of communicative competence, the development of which is the main objective of most teaching methods widely applied in EFL classrooms. However, day-to-day observation often verifies this claim. Many advanced EFL learners’ L2 (second language) production is rife with awkward utterances, unnatural wording or artificial responses in one-on-one communicative encounters. The reasons might be aplenty. One may be EFL teachers’ possible perception of communicative competence as comprising grammar competence and sociolinguistic competence only, often overshadowing the speaker’s capability of constructing textually coherent and cohesive stretches of speech. This negligent approach to discourse competence might account for why many advance learners’ L2 production is stigmatized with grammatically appropriate, yet somewhat unnatural collocations or sentence wording. L2 discourse is not merely a term restricted to any interactional act. In fact, its meaning and structure often exceed the interactional frame of communication. Whether discourse is clear, coherent and, above all, rich in natural discourse mechanisms often underlies the learner’s success or failure in L2 communication, unless the aim is to merely pidginize the language. Therefore, it seems relevant to investigate the place of discourse competence development in English Language Teaching (ELT). What poses a question is whether EFL teachers realize the significance of discourse competence, as well as whether they actually develop it in their classrooms. How advanced students’ discourse develops in the long term can also be an interesting investigation. So can a study on factors which might stimulate or 5 impede the process. The factors could include teacher talk, students’ personality and their discourse competence in L1. Part One of the research will present the theory of discourse competence and will review relevant literature which discusses its development. The first chapter of the study constitutes an initial introduction to discourse analysis in the context of foreign language teaching. This introductory chapter will accordingly sketch the theoretical position of Applied Linguistics and Sociolinguistics on discourse construction and discourse competence development. The subsequent chapters will overview the available literature regarding conceptions of competence, its distinction from performance, as well as the roles of language transfer and classroom communication in L2 discourse construction. The final section of Part One groups the chapters that deal with sociolinguistic and psycholinguistic influences in the construction of discourse and the development of discourse competence. Part Two, the empirical portion of the study, will present the results of a longitudinal study of thirteen advanced students of English developing their discourse competence throughout their three- year English as a Foreign Language tertiary education. With the methodology of the study stipulated, the section will then interpret the course of individual subjects’ discourse competence development, as well as will seek to identify those factors that might have determined possible alterations in the subjects’ discourse competence. This will be conducted correlatively as well as individually for each of the thirteen subjects. The appendix includes the overall numerical representations of the results. Student communication as well as teacher talk tapescripts are not included in the appendix but are available upon request 6 PART ONE: THEORETICAL ASPECTS OF DISCOURSE COMPETENCE DEVELOPMENT 1. Discourse and Discourse Competence Communication is not just a mechanical, raw transfer of information from the speakers to their recipients. Nor is it a disorderly exchange of turns or a meaningless, indefinite interactive tug of war. Communication, realized through discourse construction, is a spontaneous allocation of power and an unpredictable, yet logical flow of ideas. It is, or rather should be, structured poetry, with its stanzas placed by the speaker in a specific order, verses interacting with one another, and meaning inferable from the very specific context of this social act. To master this competence is quite an undertaking for a second language (L2) learner. Although successfully utilized in their first language, L2 discourse construction requires the learner to demonstrate specific knowledge of linguistic instruments, understanding of L2 cultural codes and the skills to combine these elements into an individual utterance, unique for the discourse maker, yet still not exceeding the bounds of the social communicative rigor. There have been numerous studies on discourse construction in the last few decades. The research has developed from linguistic analysis of discourse structures through studies on discourse construction by separate speech communities to the analysis of discourse in its semiotic dimension. This chapter will provide a theoretical background for the discussion of discourse competence development in the context of L2 learning by critically reviewing the relevant literature. The chapter will begin with providing the definition of discourse. Then discourse competence will be discussed with reference to other components of communicative competence. Following the discussion of discourse devices, the competence- performance distinction will be presented. The later portion of the literature review will analyze possible factors determining the development and construction of discourse, including language transfer, as well as psycholinguistic and sociolinguistic influences. 7 1.1. Defining discourse Discourse competence is the primary subject of this research. Its discussion then
Recommended publications
  • An Ultra-Condensed Introduction to Discourse Analysis: Two-Hour Tutorial at NICTA (National Information and Communication Technologies Australia)
    An ultra-condensed introduction to Discourse Analysis: two-hour tutorial at NICTA (National Information and Communication Technologies Australia) Rogelio Nazar Pontificia Universidad Católica de Valparaíso [email protected] 23 October 2015 Abstract: Linguistics has historically been devoted to the study of language as a system rather than its manifestation in particular texts. In spite of this, Discourse Analysis (DA) has been established for more than 60 years as a place of convergence of many disciplines interested in discourse. This tutorial is aimed at people with little or no knowledge of linguistic terminology and its purpose is to offer a shallow introduction to DA. This tutorial will not specifically address computational procedures for text analysis. On the contrary, the analytical methods explained here are to be conducted manually, but with systematic and almost mechanical methods. This is mainly a practical tutorial and the focus will be on analysing a real text in English. The first part presents the theoretical principles and analytical tools, while the rest is devoted to practical exercises implementing the proposed method. Given the limitations of time, this tutorial only comments upon a short number concepts. 1. Introduction It is a real pleasure for me to be here today in this not very sunny day in Canberra and I am delighted to have all of you here. As you probably know, I have been entrusted with the impossible task of summarizing 60 years of research in discourse analysis (DA) in a couple of hours. Facing such challenge, what I am going to do is not really to offer a real introduction, because we would need a semester course just to cover the essential readings.
    [Show full text]
  • Modal Sense Classification at Large / 3 Most Prominently, Epistemic (3.A), Deontic/Bouletic (3.B) and Circum- Stantial/Dynamic (3.C) Modality
    LiLT volume 14, issue 3 August 2016 Modal Sense Classification At Large Paraphrase-Driven Sense Projection, Semantically Enriched Classification Models and Cross-Genre Evaluations Ana Marasovićz?, Mengfei Zhouy?, Alexis Palmer?y, Anette Frankz y ?, zResearch Training Group AIPHES yLeibniz ScienceCampus “Empirical Linguistics and Computational Language Modeling” ?Department of Computational Linguistics Heidelberg University Abstract∗ Modal verbs have different interpretations depending on their context. Their sense categories – epistemic, deontic and dynamic – provide im- portant dimensions of meaning for the interpretation of discourse. Previous work on modal sense classification achieved relatively high performance using shallow lexical and syntactic features drawn from small-size annotated corpora. Due to the restricted empirical basis, it is difficult to assess the particular difficulties of modal sense classification and the generalization capacity of the proposed models. In this work we create large-scale, high-quality annotated corpora for modal sense classification using an automatic paraphrase-driven projec- tion approach. Using the acquired corpora, we investigate the modal sense classification task from different perspectives. We uncover the difficulty of specific sense distinctions by investigat- ∗This article represents an extension of prior work reported in Zhou (2015) and Zhou et al. (2015). It includes material of the published version in Zhou et al. (2015), with minor modifications. 1 2 / LiLT volume 14, issue 3 August 2016 ing distributional bias and reducing the sparsity of existing small-scale corpora used in prior work. We build a semantically enriched model for modal sense classification by designing novel features related to lexical, proposition-level and discourse-level semantic factors. Besides improved classification performance, closer examination of interpretable feature sets unveils relevant semantic and contextual factors in modal sense classification.
    [Show full text]
  • Grammar and Levels of Addressivity Exploring Ungarinyin Engagement Received May 4, 2018; Accepted Jan 21, 2020
    Open Linguistics 2020; 6:1–18 Research Article Stef Spronck* Grammar and levels of addressivity Exploring Ungarinyin engagement https://doi.org/10.1515/opli-2020-0001 Received May 4, 2018; accepted Jan 21, 2020 Abstract: Evans et al. (2018a,b) introduce the notion of ‘engagement’ as a new grammatical domain related to intersubjective coordination of knowledge. The present paper applies this notion to data from the Aus- tralian Aboriginal language Ungarinyin. It identifies three markers/construction types in the language as ex- pressions of engagement and develops a descriptive framework rooted in Bakhtinian Dialogism in order to demonstrate why these expressions represent the category. It is argued that the main problems that arise in the analysis of engagement are very similar to those that have been encountered in the description of (other) TAME-categories as well, and that these may be overcome by applying Mikhail Bakhtin’s idea of ‘addressivity’. It concludes that a better understanding of the category of engagement that explores its relation to addressiv- ity may contribute to the development of an approach to grammar in which sociality takes priority, a Dialogic linguistics. Keywords: Dialogic linguistics; epistemicity; intersubjectivity; Ungarinyin 1 Introduction: observing and interpreting engagement ‘To say that the human being behaves individually at one moment and socially at another is as absurd as to declare that matter follows the laws of chemistry at a certain time and succumbs to the supposedly different laws of atomic physics at another’ (Sapir, 1949, 545) In an agenda-setting discussion of epistemicity in grammar, Evans et al. (2018a,b) argue for the adoption of a new grammatical category, that of engagement.
    [Show full text]
  • A Linguistically-Motivated Annotation Model of Modality in English and Spanish: Insights from MULTINOT
    LiLT volume 14, issue (4) August 2016 A linguistically-motivated annotation model of modality in English and Spanish: Insights from MULTINOT Julia LAVID, Marta CARRETERO and Juan Rafael ZAMORANO-MANSILLA, Universidad Complutense de Madrid Abstract In this paper we present current work on the design and validation of a linguistically-motivated annotation model of modality in English and Spanish in the context of the MULTINOT project.1 Our annotation model captures four basic modal meanings and their subtypes, on the one hand, and provides a fine-grained characterisation of the syntactic realisations of those meanings in English and Spanish, on the other. We validate the modal tagset proposed through an agreement study per- formed on a bilingual sample of four hundred sentences extracted from original texts of the MULTINOT corpus, and discuss the difficult cases encountered in the annotation experiment. We also describe current steps in the implementation of the proposed scheme for the large-scale annotation of the bilingual corpus using both automatic and manual procedures. 1The MULTINOT project is financed by the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness, under grant number FF2012-32201. We gratefully acknowledge the support provided by the Spanish authorities. We also thank the comments and suggestions provided by the anonymous reviewers which have helped to improve the current manuscript. 1 2 / LiLT volume 14, issue (4) August 2016 1 Introduction In this paper we describe the construction and empirical validation of a modality annotation scheme for English and Spanish in the context of the MULTINOT project, whose main aim is the development of a parallel English-Spanish corpus which is balanced – in terms of register diversity and translation directions – and whose design and enrichment with multiple layers of linguistic annotations focuses on quality rather than on quantity (see Lavid et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Affirmative and Negated Modality*
    Quaderns de Filologia. Estudis lingüístics. Vol. XIV (2009) 169-192 AFFIRMATIVE AND NEGATED MODALITY* Günter Radden Hamburg 0. INTR O DUCTI O N 0.1. Stating the problem Descriptions of modality in English often give the impression that the behaviour of modal verbs is erratic when they occur with negation. A particularly intriguing case is the behaviour of the modal verb must under negation. In its deontic sense, must is used both in affirmative and negated sentences, expressing obligation (1a) and prohibition (1b), respectively. In its epistemic sense, however, must is only used in affirmative sentences expressing necessity (2a). The modal verb that expresses the corresponding negated epistemic modality, i.e. impossibility, is can’t (2b). (1) a. You must switch off your mobile phone. [obligation] b. You mustn’t switch off your mobile phone. [prohibition] (2) a. Your mobile phone must be switched off. [necessity] b. Your mobile phone can’t be switched off. [impossibility] Explanations that have been offered for the use of epistemic can’t are not very helpful. Palmer (1990: 61) argues that mustn’t is not used for the negation of epistemic necessity because can’t is supplied, and Coates (1983: 20) suggests that can’t is used because mustn’t is unavailable. Both “explanations” beg the question: why should can’t be used to denote negated necessity and why should mustn’t be unavailable? Such questions have apparently not been asked for two interrelated reasons: first, the study of modality and negation has been dominated by logic and, secondly, since the modal verbs apparently do * An earlier version of this paper was published in Władysław Chłopicki, Andrzej Pawelec, and Agnieszka Pokojska (eds.), 2007, Cognition in Language: Volume in Honour of Professor Elżbieta Tabakowska, 224-254.
    [Show full text]
  • Modality and Factivity: One Perspective on the Meaning of the English Modal Auxiliaries
    MODALITY AND FACTIVITY: ONE PERSPECTIVE ON THE MEANING OF THE ENGLISH MODAL AUXILIARIES by NICOLA M,BREWER Submitted in accordance with the requirements for the degree of PhD The University of Leeds Department of Linguistics and Phonetics December 1987 ii ABSTRACT This study concentrates on modality as expressed by the set of modal auxiliaries and seeks to establish that these verbs share semantic as well as syntactic properties by identifying a single core meaning which they share. The relationship between modality and factivity is examined with the aim of gaining an insight into the former, more complex concept. When viewed from this perspective, the defining characteristic of all the modal auxiliary verbs in almost all of their uses is found to be nonfactivity. The meanings expressed by this set of verbs are classified according to a framework derived from modal logic consisting of three basic types of modality each of which relates to a different set of laws or principles; the relative factivity associated with the modal auxiliaries is seen to vary with the nature of modality as defined and classified by this framework. Within each of the three types of modality, a semantic scale is identified and modality is described as a gradable concept for which scalar analysis is appropriate, both within and beyond these three scales. Relative factivity is also shown to vary according to the degree of modality expressed by each of the modal verbs. The nature and degree of modality expressed interact with features of the linguistic (and pragmatic) context to determine the particular factive or a contrafactive interpretation conveyed by a given modal auxiliary token.
    [Show full text]
  • MODULATING GRAMMAR THROUGH MODALITY: a DISCOURSE APPROACH* Francisco Gonzálvez García Universidad De Almería
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by idUS. Depósito de Investigación Universidad de Sevilla ELIA I, 2000 MODULATING GRAMMAR THROUGH MODALITY: A DISCOURSE APPROACH* Francisco Gonzálvez García Universidad de Almería This paper argues for a dynamic, discourse-based approach to modality which departs from the traditional, monolithic account of modality in a number of interesting respects: (i) modality is characterized as conveying the speaker’s involvement in the propositional content of a given utterance (either in the form of agency or subjectivity) (ii) modality can be said to ramify across the whole lexico-grammatical architecture of the language, (iii) many of the meanings usually ascribed to individual modal verbs are in fact derived either from the verb’s sentential environment or from some wider context of utterance, and (iv) modal meaning in discourse can be said to arise out of the interaction of two closely connected layers of meaning: one embracing the inherent linguistic meaning of the modal verb in conjunction with that of other neighbouring modal devices, and another concerned with principles connected with politeness and face-saving strategies. 1. Introduction Modality in English and other languages has been the focus of attention of scholars from distinct disciplines and approaches over the last thirty years. Within linguistics, the study of modality has witnessed a gradual shift from a monolithic, static conception to a more dynamic understanding of modality taking into account the relevance of linguistic and extralinguistic contextual factors in the production and interpretation of modal utterances in discourse (Bybee & Fleischman 1995) or the creation of modal textual * I would like to express my gratitude to Paul J.
    [Show full text]
  • Covert Modality in Non-Finite Contexts
    University of Pennsylvania ScholarlyCommons IRCS Technical Reports Series Institute for Research in Cognitive Science January 2000 Covert Modality in Non-Finite Contexts Rajesh Bhatt University of Pennsylvania Follow this and additional works at: https://repository.upenn.edu/ircs_reports Bhatt, Rajesh, "Covert Modality in Non-Finite Contexts" (2000). IRCS Technical Reports Series. 32. https://repository.upenn.edu/ircs_reports/32 University of Pennsylvania Institute for Research in Cognitive Science Technical Report No. IRCS-00-01. This paper is posted at ScholarlyCommons. https://repository.upenn.edu/ircs_reports/32 For more information, please contact [email protected]. Covert Modality in Non-Finite Contexts Abstract This dissertation investigates the distribution and interpretation of covert modality. Three environments where covert modality appears are analyzed. These environments are infinitival relative clauses, infinitival questions, and ability modals. Infinitival relative clauses are shown to not form a unified class structurally. Subject infinitival relative clauses are assimilated to the class of reduced relative clauses. They lack a CP projection. Non-subject infinitival relative are assimilated to the class of full relative clauses. Like full relatives, they have a CP projection. The infinitival [+wh] Cº is argued to be the source of the modality in a non-subject infinitival relative clause. All nonsubject infinitival relative clauses and infinitival questions involve modality because of the obligatory presence of the infinitival [+wh] Cº. Since subject infinitival relative clauses do not involve the infinitival [+wh] Cº, they are not necessarily modal. If they are modal, the source of the modality lies within the infinitival clause. The conditions under which a subject infinitival relative can receive a non-modal interpretation are analyzed.
    [Show full text]
  • Epistemic Modality and Deontic Modality at the Crossroads
    Proceedings of The 3rd Annual International Conference Syiah Kuala University (AIC Unsyiah) 2013 In conjunction with The 2nd International Conference on Multidisciplinary Research (ICMR) 2013 October 2-4, 2013, Banda Aceh, Indonesia EPISTEMIC MODALITY AND DEONTIC MODALITY AT THE CROSSROADS JUMINO SUHADI Department of English Literature, Faculty of LiteratureIslamic University of North Sumatra, Medan, Indonesia. Email: [email protected] ABSTRACT This article is a descriptive survey on the similarities and differences between epistemic and deontic modalities as introduced by Halliday (1994: )and Downing and Locke (199: 283) . Epistemic modality refers to the use of modality which is based on the speaker's evaluation and judgment in relation to the degree of confidence of the knowledge on the proposition. It functions to comment on and evaluate an interpretation of reality in carrying out speech functions. Deontic modality indicates whether the proposition expressed by a command is obligatory, advisable or permissible according to some normative backgroundsuch as law, morality, convention, etc. In many instances, the meanings of some modals are relative and complements to each other. The evidence of some extrinsic modalities such as must, should, may, and can indicates that they may fall under different types; thus, they are at the crossroads; two types of modality which may use the same realization but they goes to different meanings. Keywords:Systemic Functional Linguistics, epistemic modality, deontic modality, Introduction One of the three metafunctions of language in Systemic Functional Grammar pioneered by Halliday (1994) is the interpersonal function, in which language is used to express a speaker’s attitude and judgment for communicating with others.
    [Show full text]
  • Deontic Modality, Generically
    Deontic Modality, Generically Federico L. G. Faroldi Abstract This position paper aims to explore some preliminary suggestions to develop a theory of deontic modalities under a generic understanding. I suggest, for in- stance, that a sentence such as ‘Everyone ought to pay taxes’ is true just in case the generic (deontically relevant) individual pays taxes. Different degrees of generic- ity are explored, without assuming too much about a specific theory of genericity. I argue that such an analysis captures our intuitions about exceptions and the gen- eral character of deontic claims better than classical approaches based on possible- world semantics and than defeasibility-based approaches, while remaining within a broadly deductive framework. [...] law can never issue an injunction binding on all which really embodies what is best for each: it cannot prescribe with perfect accuracy what is good and right for each member of the community at any one time. The differences of human personality, the variety of men’s activities and the inevitable unsettlement attending all human experience make it impossible for any art whatsoever to issue unqualified rules holding good on all questions at all times [...] [one] will lay down laws in general form [for the majority, roughly meeting the cases of individuals [...] under average circumstances. Plato Politicus, 294a–b. [...] all law is universal but about some things it is not possible to make a universal statement which shall be correct. In those cases, then, in which it is necessary to speak universally, but not possible to do so correctly, the law takes the usual case, though it is not ignorant of the possibility of error.
    [Show full text]
  • Epistemic and Deontic Modality: a Linguistic Indicator of Disciplinary Variation in Academic English
    Epistemic and Deontic Modality: A Linguistic Indicator of Disciplinary Variation in Academic English Jordi Piqué-Angordans (Universitat de València, Spain) Santiago Posteguillo (Universitat Jaume I de Castelló, Spain) J.-Vicent Andreu-Besó (Universitat de València, Spain) 1. Modalization and disciplinary variation in academic English According to pragmatic linguistic analyses, language has to be analyzed and interpreted in relation to the social context which generates and uses it. Language is not to be considered an isolated system of either symbols or mental rules, but a system used in a specific setting and for specific communicative purposes. It is within this pragmatic paradigm that English for Specific Purposes (ESP) develops in the 60s and continues to the present. In this context, RAs are among the most widely researched genres, but most of these studies have focused on their structural organization (Crookes, 1986; Swales, 1990; Nwogu, 1990, 1991, 1997; Bhatia, 1993; Berkenkotter & Huckin, 1995; Posteguillo, 1996a-b, 1999; Estévez & Piqué, 1997; Piqué & Andreu-Besó, 2000), or on the structural organizations of individual sections within the RA itself (Swales, 1981; Dudley-Evans & Henderson, 1990; Brett, 1994; Holmes, 1997; Piqué & Andreu-Besó, 1998; Bloor, 1999). It is true that these macrostructural descriptions represent a major feature of RAs as a genre, but there are other linguistic and communicative phenomena which differentiate the RA from other genres which have not been analyzed in such detail, especially having in mind disciplinary variation. For instance, some other authors have studied alternative discourse functions of RAs, such as the use of citations and 49 references (Jordan, 1990), or the use of argumentation (Hyland, 1990; Thompson, 1993).
    [Show full text]
  • Modal Concord
    Modal Concord Hedde Zeijlstra University of Amsterdam 1. Introduction Expressions consisting of multiple modal expressions normally yield a cumulative reading. The two modal sentences in (1) and (2) contain two modal elements each (maybe / has and should / allowed, respectively). Semantically, both (1) and (2) contain two modal operators. (1) Maybe Mary has to leave (2) John should be allowed to read this file However, if two modal elements are of the same modal type (epistemic/ deontic/…) and have similar quantificational force (universal/existential), the most salient reading is mostly not a cumulative one but a concord reading, where the semantics seems to contain only one modal operator. This phenomenon has first been observed by (Halliday 1970) and (Lyons 1977) and has been first analysed by (Geurts and Huitink 2006) who have dubbed it Modal Concord (MC). (3) You may possibly have read my little monograph upon the subject ‘The speaker thinks that it is possible that you read his little monograph’ ? ‘The speaker thinks that it is possible that it is possible that you read his little monograph’ (4) Power carts must mandatorily be used on cart paths where provided ‘It is obligatory that power cats are used on cart paths where provided’ ?‘It is obligatory that it is obligatory that power cats are used on cart paths where provided’ In this paper I attempt to analyse the concord interpretations in (3) and (4). First I argue that the concord readings cannot be the result of an entailment relation and that the phenomenon of Modal Concord is indeed a grammatical phenomenon.
    [Show full text]