The Acoustics of Nasals and Laterals

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Acoustics of Nasals and Laterals 24.963 Linguistic Phonetics The acoustics of nasals and laterals 5 4 3 2 FREQ (kHz) 1 0 0 100 200 300 400 500 600 700 800 900 Image by MIT OCW. Adapted from Stevens, K. N. Acoustic Phonetics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1999, chapter 6. 1 Assignments • Talk to me about a paper topic, this week or next. 2 Damping and bandwidth • When an input of energy sets a body vibrating, the resulting vibrations die out as energy is dissipated through friction, transmission of energy to surrounding air, etc. • A wave of this type is described as damped. 0 .001 .002 .003 .004.005 .006 .007 .008 .009 .01 Time in seconds Image by MIT OCW. Adapted from Ladefoged, Peter. Elements of Acoustic Phonetics. Chicago, IL: Univeristy of Chicago Press, 1962. 3 Damping and bandwidth • The more heavily damped a sinusoid is, the wider the bandwidth of its spectrum. • A wider bandwidth implies a lower spectral peak, other things being equal, since the energy in the wave is distributed over a wider range of frequencies. 900 Hz Air pressure Amplitude 1,000 Hz 1,100 Hz Air pressure Amplitude Complex wave Air pressure Amplitude 0 .01 0 500 1000 Time in seconds Frequency in Hz a b c d e Various waves and their spectra 0 .001 .002 .003 .004 .005 Image by MIT OCW. Time in seconds Adapted from Ladefoged, Peter. Elements of Acoustic Image by MIT OCW. Phonetics. Chicago, IL: Univeristy of Chicago Press, 1962. Adapted from Ladefoged, Peter. Elements of Acoustic 4 Phonetics. Chicago, IL: Univeristy of Chicago Press, 1962. Damping and bandwidth • Standing waves in the vocal tract are damped due to friction, radiation losses to the outside air, absorption of energy by the elastic vocal tract walls etc. • Greater surface area results in more damping (and broader formant bandwidths). • Coupling the nasal cavity to the vocal tract in in nasal sounds increases the surface area of the vocal tract. 5 Nasal stops • A uvular nasal can be modeled in terms of a tube which is closed at the glottis and open at the nostrils. • It is not a uniform tube. • The effective length of the tube is greater than the length from glottis to lips – 21.5 cm (Johnson) or 20 cm (Stevens) vs. 16 cm. • So the resonances of the tube are at lower frequencies (and closer together) than in a vowel. 6 Nasal stops • In nasal consonants formed with an oral constriction further forward than the uvula, the oral cavity forms a side branch on the pharyngeal-nasal tube. • This side branch is longest in labials, shortest in velars. (A) (B) Nose Mouth (m) Pharynx (A) An X-ray tracing of the positions of the articulators during (m). (B) A tube model of this vocal tract configuration. Image by MIT OCW. Adapted from Johnson, Keith. Acoustic and Auditory Phonetics. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers, 1997. 7 Nasal stops • The side branch is a tube open at one end and resonates accordingly. • The side branch is not coupled to the outside air. Its resonances are zeros in the signal radiated from the nose - i.e. those frequencies are attenuated. • Zeroes also reduce the amplitude of higher frequencies, so nasals are characterized by less high frequency energy than vowels. • Frequencies of the zeroes depend on the size of the side branch, e.g. 8 cm for [m], 5.5 cm for [n]. Nose Mouth Pharynx A tube model of this vocal tract configuration. Image by MIT OCW. Adapted from Johnson, Keith. Acoustic and Auditory Phonetics. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers, 1997. 8 Nasal stops 5000 5000 Frequency (Hz) (Hz) Frequency (Hz) Frequency 0 0 0.03722 0.2246 9.767 9.988 Time (s) Time (s) [n] nigh [m] my 9 Cues to place in nasal stops • Formant transitions – as in oral stops • Nasal murmur differs according to place of articulation, but in itself is a weak cue – murmur is relatively quiet. • But the interval around the release of the nasal may provide significant place cues: – Nasal place is identified more accurately from 6 periods centered around release compared to 6 periods of the murmur or formant transitions (Kurowski & Blumstein 1984) – But the differences are small: 12% vs. 13% vs. 20% • The interval around the onset of nasal closure does not provide comparable cues to place (Repp & Svastikula 1988) 10 Laterals • Laterals also have a side branch - the pocket behind the tongue tip is a side branch to the main tube(s) passing around the side(s) of the tongue. (a) 13.5 cm [l] 2.5 cm 40 cm3 2.5 cm A = 0.17 cm2 l = 1.0 cm Image by MIT OCW. Image by MIT OCW. Adapted from Johnson, Keith. Acoustic and Auditory Phonetics Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers, 1997. 11 Laterals • Laterals also have a side branch - the pocket behind the tongue tip is a side branch to the main tube(s) passing around the side(s) of the tongue. • Laterals are thus also characterized by zeroes - the lowest appears between F2 and F3, often significantly reducing the amplitude of F2. • The presence of zeroes and the coronal constriction reduce the intensity of laterals compared to most vowels. • On spectrograms, laterals look similar to nasals, but differ in the location of formants and zeros, and in their effects on neighboring vowels. 12 Laterals 5000 5000 Frequency (Hz) (Hz) Frequency Frequency (Hz) (Hz) Frequency 0 0 7.507 7.726 0.03722 0.2246 Time (s) Time (s) [n] nigh [l] lie 13 Laterals 5000 Frequency (Hz) 0 30.39 30.67 Time (s) [dli(ma)] (Hebrew) 14 Nasalized vowels • Nasalized vowels involve a complex vocal tract configuration. Nose Mouth Pharynx Image by MIT OCW. Adapted from Johnson, Keith. Acoustic and Auditory Phonetics. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers, 1997. 15 Nasalized vowels • The coupled nasal cavity contributes both formants and anti-formants (poles and zeroes) which combine with the formants of the oral tract. • The net acoustic effect of velum lowering depends on the locations of the formants in the corresponding oral vowel (Maeda 1993) 16 Nasalized vowels 40 30 A B NF1 ' F2 ' F1 F2 20 F1 F3 10 F1' Z1 i 0 NF1 0 NF2 F3' u -10 NF2 Magnitude (dB) -20 Z2 Oral Oral -30 Z1 Nasal (NC = 0.4) Z2 Nasal (NC = 0.4) 0 1 2 3 40 51 2 3 4 5 Frequency (kHz) Frequency (kHz) 40 C D 30 F1 F1 20 F2' F2 NF1 NF1 F11 F2' F3 10 F1' F3' ε 0 Z1 0 Z1 -10 NF2 Magnitude (dB) -20 Oral MF1 Z2 Oral -30 Nasal (NC = 0.2) Nasal (NC = 0.4) 0 1 2 3 40 51 2 3 4 5 Frequency (kHz) Frequency (kHz) 30 E 20 F2 F2' F2 F1 10 F2' F3 0 F3' -10 ο Z1 NF2 -20 Magnitude (dB) -30 Oral Nasal (NC = 1.6) -40 Z2 0 1 2 3 4 5 Frequency (kHz) Image by MIT OCW. Adapted from Maeda, Shinji. "Acoustics of vowel nasalization and articulatory shifts in French nasal vowels."In Nasals, Nasalization and the Velum. Edited by M. Huffman and R. Krakow. New York, NY: Academic Press, 2003, pp. 147-170. 17 Nasalized vowels • As a result it is difficult to give a general acoustic characterization of nasalization on vowels. • Maeda: nasalized vowels generally have a flat, low intensity spectrum in the low frequency region. – Addition of the lowest nasal formant in combination with broad bandwidth F1 can create a broad, low frequency prominence. 'dot bend' 'Don's bed' 5 4 3 2 FREQ (kHz) 1 0 0 100 200 300 400 500 600 700 800 900 0 100 200 300 400 500 600 700 800 900 Image by MIT OCW. Adapted from Stevens, K. N. Acoustic Phonetics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1999. 18 Nasalized vowels • Chen (1997) proposes the measures A1-P0 and A1-P1 to quantify nasalization. – A1 is amplitude of F1 – P0 is amplitude of the low frequency nasal peak. – P1 is amplitude of a higher nasal peak, between F1 and F2. 19 Non-nasal Vowel A1 P0 • A1 amplitude of F1 60 • P0 amplitude of lower frequency 40 P1 nasal peak - intensity of harmonic MAG (dB) with greatest amplitude at low 20 frequency. 0 • P1 amplitude of nasal peak - 0 1 2 3 4 5 FREQ (kHz) measured as amplitude of highest harmonic near to 950 Hz. Nasal Vowel • These measures are sensitive to 60 P0 A1 P1 vowel quality - Chen proposes 40 corrections to adjust for this. MAG (dB) 20 0 0 1 2 3 4 5 FREQ (kHz) Image by MIT OCW. Adapted from Chen, Marilyn Y. "Acoustic correlates of English and French nasalized vowels." The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 102 (1997): 2360. 20 Nasalized vowels • Perceptually, nasal vowels are less distinct from each other than oral vowels, both in terms of confusability (Bond 1975) and similarity judgements (Wright 1986). u ~ ι ~ι -.4 u u u υ o 400 -.2 ι υ ~ι υ o~ o 600 e ~ ~ ~ ε o 0 a in mels e 1 ~ F ε Dimension I a a e ~e υ .2 ~ 800 ε ε ae a ae~ .4 ae ae~ -.6 -.4 -.2 0 .2 1600 1400 1200 1000 Dimension II F2 in mels The first two dimensions in the F1 versus F2 plot of the mel values of perceptual space as determined by the stimuli INDSCAL Image by MIT OCW. Adapted from.Wright, James T. "The behavior of nasalized vowels in the perceptual vowel space." In Experimental Phonology. Edited by J. J. Ohala and J. J. Jaeger. Orlando, FL: Academic Press, 1986, 45-67.
Recommended publications
  • On Phonetics and Phonology: a Broad-Termed
    On Phonetics and Phonology: A Broad-Termed Comparison and Contrast between Broad and Narrow Transcription Name: Pouria Ebrahimi E-mail: [email protected] Takestan Azad University (M.A.) Feb. 2010 Page | 1 Abstract As a subfield of linguistics, phonetics and phonology have as their main axis the concern of articulation of sounds; that is, how human beings produce speech. Although dated back over 2000 years ago, modern contributions of scientists and scholars regarding phonetics and phonology have involved various fields of science and schools of thought such as philosophy, physiology, psychology, and even American structuralism. So, in line with all this, this study starts with a general view toward phonetics and phonology holding the view of early contributors and the role of aforementioned sciences and schools of thought. Then, thru representing figures and tables, this study continues to consider two major aspects of the filed—namely broad and narrow transcription. A broad-termed comparison and contract between the two, this study aims to imply, indicates the former transcription harmful to the same extent, if not more one should like to emphasize, it could be of assistance. It is because it does not represent the exact subtleties of phonetics and, thus, prevent the person from achieving a native-like pronunciation. Key words: phonetics, phonology, broad transcription, narrow transcription Page | 2 Introduction Phonetics is the study, investigation, and description of sound system in a given language. Articulation of sounds is mostly concerned with the movement of speech organs including lips and tongue; but this is just the beginning. To investigate and describe sound systems, one needs to pierce deeper where other organs and factors are in play.
    [Show full text]
  • LINGUISTICS 221 LECTURE #3 the BASIC SOUNDS of ENGLISH 1. STOPS a Stop Consonant Is Produced with a Complete Closure of Airflow
    LINGUISTICS 221 LECTURE #3 Introduction to Phonetics and Phonology THE BASIC SOUNDS OF ENGLISH 1. STOPS A stop consonant is produced with a complete closure of airflow in the vocal tract; the air pressure has built up behind the closure; the air rushes out with an explosive sound when released. The term plosive is also used for oral stops. ORAL STOPS: e.g., [b] [t] (= plosives) NASAL STOPS: e.g., [m] [n] (= nasals) There are three phases of stop articulation: i. CLOSING PHASE (approach or shutting phase) The articulators are moving from an open state to a closed state; ii. CLOSURE PHASE (= occlusion) Blockage of the airflow in the oral tract; iii. RELEASE PHASE Sudden reopening; it may be accompanied by a burst of air. ORAL STOPS IN ENGLISH a. BILABIAL STOPS: The blockage is made with the two lips. spot [p] voiceless baby [b] voiced 1 b. ALVEOLAR STOPS: The blade (or the tip) of the tongue makes a closure with the alveolar ridge; the sides of the tongue are along the upper teeth. lamino-alveolar stops or Check your apico-alveolar stops pronunciation! stake [t] voiceless deep [d] voiced c. VELAR STOPS: The closure is between the back of the tongue (= dorsum) and the velum. dorso-velar stops scar [k] voiceless goose [g] voiced 2. NASALS (= nasal stops) The air is stopped in the oral tract, but the velum is lowered so that the airflow can go through the nasal tract. All nasals are voiced. NASALS IN ENGLISH a. BILABIAL NASAL: made [m] b. ALVEOLAR NASAL: need [n] c.
    [Show full text]
  • The Violability of Backness in Retroflex Consonants
    The violability of backness in retroflex consonants Paul Boersma University of Amsterdam Silke Hamann ZAS Berlin February 11, 2005 Abstract This paper addresses remarks made by Flemming (2003) to the effect that his analysis of the interaction between retroflexion and vowel backness is superior to that of Hamann (2003b). While Hamann maintained that retroflex articulations are always back, Flemming adduces phonological as well as phonetic evidence to prove that retroflex consonants can be non-back and even front (i.e. palatalised). The present paper, however, shows that the phonetic evidence fails under closer scrutiny. A closer consideration of the phonological evidence shows, by making a principled distinction between articulatory and perceptual drives, that a reanalysis of Flemming’s data in terms of unviolated retroflex backness is not only possible but also simpler with respect to the number of language-specific stipulations. 1 Introduction This paper is a reply to Flemming’s article “The relationship between coronal place and vowel backness” in Phonology 20.3 (2003). In a footnote (p. 342), Flemming states that “a key difference from the present proposal is that Hamann (2003b) employs inviolable articulatory constraints, whereas it is a central thesis of this paper that the constraints relating coronal place to tongue-body backness are violable”. The only such constraint that is violable for Flemming but inviolable for Hamann is the constraint that requires retroflex coronals to be articulated with a back tongue body. Flemming expresses this as the violable constraint RETRO!BACK, or RETRO!BACKCLO if it only requires that the closing phase of a retroflex consonant be articulated with a back tongue body.
    [Show full text]
  • Recognition of Nasalized and Non- Nasalized Vowels
    RECOGNITION OF NASALIZED AND NON- NASALIZED VOWELS Submitted By: BILAL A. RAJA Advised by: Dr. Carol Espy Wilson and Tarun Pruthi Speech Communication Lab, Dept of Electrical & Computer Engineering University of Maryland, College Park Maryland Engineering Research Internship Teams (MERIT) Program 2006 1 TABLE OF CONTENTS 1- Abstract 3 2- What Is Nasalization? 3 2.1 – Production of Nasal Sound 3 2.2 – Common Spectral Characteristics of Nasalization 4 3 – Expected Results 6 3.1 – Related Previous Researches 6 3.2 – Hypothesis 7 4- Method 7 4.1 – The Task 7 4.2 – The Technique Used 8 4.3 – HMM Tool Kit (HTK) 8 4.3.1 – Data Preparation 9 4.3.2 – Training 9 4.3.3 – Testing 9 4.3.4 – Analysis 9 5 – Results 10 5.1 – Experiment 1 11 5.2 – Experiment 2 11 6 – Conclusion 12 7- References 13 2 1 - ABSTRACT When vowels are adjacent to nasal consonants (/m,n,ng/), they often become nasalized for at least some part of their duration. This nasalization is known to lead to changes in perceived vowel quality. The goal of this project is to verify if it is beneficial to first recognize nasalization in vowels and treat the three groups of vowels (those occurring before nasal consonants, those occurring after nasal consonants, and the oral vowels which are vowels that are not adjacent to nasal consonants) separately rather than collectively for recognizing the vowel identities. The standard Mel-Frequency Cepstral Coefficients (MFCCs) and the Hidden Markov Model (HMM) paradigm have been used for this purpose. The results show that when the system is trained on vowels in general, the recognition of nasalized vowels is 17% below that of oral vowels.
    [Show full text]
  • Nasal Assimilation in Quranic Recitation Table of Contents
    EmanQuotah Linguistics Senior Paper Hadass Sheffer, Advisor Swarthmore College December 9, 1994 Nasal Assimilation in Quranic Recitation Table of Contents Introduction 1 TheQuran 3 Recitation and Tajwi:d 7 Nasal Assimilation in Quranic Recitation 10 Arabic geminates 12 Nasal assimilation rules 15 Blocking of assimilation by pauses 24 Conclusion 26 Bibliography Grateful acknowledgements to my father, my mother and my brothers, and to Hadass Sheffer and Donna Jo Napoli. Introduction This paper is concerned with the analysis of certain rules governing nasality and nasal assimilation during recitation of the holy Quran. I These rules are a subset of tajwi:d, a set of rules governing the correct prescribed recitation and pronunciation of the Islamic scriptures. The first part of the paper will describe the historical and cultural importance of the Quran and tajwi:d, with the proposition that a tension or conflict between the necessity for clarity and enunciation and the desire for beautification of the divine words of God is the driving force behind tajwi:d's importance. Though the rules are functional rather than "natural," these prescriptive rules can be integrated into a study lexical phonology and feature geometry, as discussed in the second section, since prescriptive rules must work within those rules set by the language's grammar. Muslims consider the Quran a divine and holy text, untampered with and unchangeable by humankind. Western scholars have attempted to identify it as the writings of the Prophet Muhammad, a humanly written text like any other. Viewing the holy Quran in this way ignores the religious, social and linguistic implications of its perceived unchangeability, and does disservice to the beliefs of many Muslims.
    [Show full text]
  • English Phonetic Vowel Shortening and Lengthening As Perceptually Active for the Poles
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE Title: English phonetic vowel shortening and lengthening as perceptually active for the Poles Author: Arkadiusz Rojczyk Citation style: Rojczyk Arkadiusz. (2007). English phonetic vowel shortening and lengthening as perceptually active for the Poles. W: J. Arabski (red.), "On foreign language acquisition and effective learning" (S. 237-249). Katowice : Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Śląskiego Phonological subsystem English phonetic vowel shortening and lengthening as perceptually active for the Poles Arkadiusz Rojczyk University of Silesia, Katowice 1. Introduction Auditory perception is one of the most thriving and active domains of psy­ cholinguistics. Recent years have witnessed manifold attempts to investigate and understand how humans perceive linguistic sounds and what principles govern the process. The major assumption underlying the studies on auditory perception is a simple fact that linguistic sounds are means of conveying meaning, encoded by a speaker, to a hearer. The speaker, via articulatory gestures, transmits a message encoded in acoustic waves. This is the hearer, on the other hand, who, using his perceptual apparatus, reads acoustic signals and decodes them into the meaning. Therefore, in phonetic terms, the whole communicative act can be divided into three stages. Articulatory - the speaker encodes the message by sound production. Acoustic - the message is transmitted to the hearer as acous­ tic waves. Auditory - the hearer perceives sounds and decodes them into the meaning. Each of the three stages is indispensable for effective communication. Of the three aforementioned stages of communication, the auditory percep­ tion is the least amenable to precise depiction and understanding.
    [Show full text]
  • NWAV 46 Booklet-Oct29
    1 PROGRAM BOOKLET October 29, 2017 CONTENTS • The venue and the town • The program • Welcome to NWAV 46 • The team and the reviewers • Sponsors and Book Exhibitors • Student Travel Awards https://english.wisc.edu/nwav46/ • Abstracts o Plenaries Workshops o nwav46 o Panels o Posters and oral presentations • Best student paper and poster @nwav46 • NWAV sexual harassment policy • Participant email addresses Look, folks, this is an electronic booklet. This Table of Contents gives you clues for what to search for and we trust that’s all you need. 2 We’ll have buttons with sets of pronouns … and some with a blank space to write in your own set. 3 The venue and the town We’re assuming you’ll navigate using electronic devices, but here’s some basic info. Here’s a good campus map: http://map.wisc.edu/. The conference will be in Union South, in red below, except for Saturday talks, which will be in the Brogden Psychology Building, just across Johnson Street to the northeast on the map. There are a few places to grab a bite or a drink near Union South and the big concentration of places is on and near State Street, a pedestrian zone that runs east from Memorial Library (top right). 4 The program 5 NWAV 46 2017 Madison, WI Thursday, November 2nd, 2017 12:00 Registration – 5th Quarter Room, Union South pm-6:00 pm Industry Landmark Northwoods Agriculture 1:00- Progress in regression: Discourse analysis for Sociolinguistics and Texts as data 3:00 Statistical and practical variationists forensic speech sources for improvements to Rbrul science: Knowledge-
    [Show full text]
  • UC Berkeley Dissertations, Department of Linguistics
    UC Berkeley Dissertations, Department of Linguistics Title The Aeroacoustics of Nasalized Fricatives Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/00h9g9gg Author Shosted, Ryan K Publication Date 2006 eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California The Aeroacoustics of Nasalized Fricatives by Ryan Keith Shosted B.A. (Brigham Young University) 2000 M.A. (University of California, Berkeley) 2003 A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics in the GRADUATE DIVISION of the UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA, BERKELEY Committee in charge: John J. Ohala, Chair Keith Johnson Milton M. Azevedo Fall 2006 The dissertation of Ryan Keith Shosted is approved: Chair Date Date Date University of California, Berkeley Fall 2006 The Aeroacoustics of Nasalized Fricatives Copyright 2006 by Ryan Keith Shosted 1 Abstract The Aeroacoustics of Nasalized Fricatives by Ryan Keith Shosted Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics University of California, Berkeley John J. Ohala, Chair Understanding the relationship of aerodynamic laws to the unique geometry of the hu- man vocal tract allows us to make phonological and typological predictions about speech sounds typified by particular aerodynamic regimes. For example, some have argued that the realization of nasalized fricatives is improbable because fricatives and nasals have an- tagonistic aerodynamic specifications. Fricatives require high pressure behind the suprala- ryngeal constriction as a precondition for high particle velocity. Nasalization, on the other hand, vents back pressure by allowing air to escape through the velopharyngeal orifice. This implies that an open velopharyngeal port will reduce oral particle velocity, thereby potentially extinguishing frication. By using a mechanical model of the vocal tract and spoken fricatives that have undergone coarticulatory nasalization, it is shown that nasal- ization must alter the spectral characteristics of fricatives, e.g.
    [Show full text]
  • Part 1: Introduction to The
    PREVIEW OF THE IPA HANDBOOK Handbook of the International Phonetic Association: A guide to the use of the International Phonetic Alphabet PARTI Introduction to the IPA 1. What is the International Phonetic Alphabet? The aim of the International Phonetic Association is to promote the scientific study of phonetics and the various practical applications of that science. For both these it is necessary to have a consistent way of representing the sounds of language in written form. From its foundation in 1886 the Association has been concerned to develop a system of notation which would be convenient to use, but comprehensive enough to cope with the wide variety of sounds found in the languages of the world; and to encourage the use of thjs notation as widely as possible among those concerned with language. The system is generally known as the International Phonetic Alphabet. Both the Association and its Alphabet are widely referred to by the abbreviation IPA, but here 'IPA' will be used only for the Alphabet. The IPA is based on the Roman alphabet, which has the advantage of being widely familiar, but also includes letters and additional symbols from a variety of other sources. These additions are necessary because the variety of sounds in languages is much greater than the number of letters in the Roman alphabet. The use of sequences of phonetic symbols to represent speech is known as transcription. The IPA can be used for many different purposes. For instance, it can be used as a way to show pronunciation in a dictionary, to record a language in linguistic fieldwork, to form the basis of a writing system for a language, or to annotate acoustic and other displays in the analysis of speech.
    [Show full text]
  • SSC: the Science of Talking
    SSC: The Science of Talking (for year 1 students of medicine) Week 3: Sounds of the World’s Languages (vowels and consonants) Michael Ashby, Senior Lecturer in Phonetics, UCL PLIN1101 Introduction to Phonetics and Phonology A Lecture 4 page 1 Vowel Description Essential reading: Ashby & Maidment, Chapter 5 4.1 Aim: To introduce the basics of vowel description and the main characteristics of the vowels of RP English. 4.2 Definition of vowel: Vowels are produced without any major obstruction of the airflow; the intra-oral pressure stays low, and vowels are therefore sonorant sounds. Vowels are normally voiced. Vowels are articulated by raising some part of the tongue body (that is the front or the back of the tongue notnot the tip or blade) towards the roof of the oral cavity (see Figure 1). 4.3 Front vowels are produced by raising the front of the tongue towards the hard palate. Back vowels are produced by raising the back of the tongue towards the soft palate. Central vowels are produced by raising the centre part of the tongue towards the junction of the hard and soft palates. 4.4 The height of a vowel refers to the degree of raising of the relevant part of the tongue. If the tongue is raised so as to be close to the roof of the oral cavity then a close or high vowel is produced. If the tongue is only slightly raised, so that there is a wide gap between its highest point and the roof of the oral cavity, then an open or lowlowlow vowel results.
    [Show full text]
  • Kent Academic Repository Full Text Document (Pdf)
    Kent Academic Repository Full text document (pdf) Citation for published version Rehman, Ishrat (2019) Urdu Vowel System and Perception of English Vowels by Punjabi-Urdu Speakers. Doctor of Philosophy (PhD) thesis, University of Kent,. DOI Link to record in KAR https://kar.kent.ac.uk/80850/ Document Version UNSPECIFIED Copyright & reuse Content in the Kent Academic Repository is made available for research purposes. Unless otherwise stated all content is protected by copyright and in the absence of an open licence (eg Creative Commons), permissions for further reuse of content should be sought from the publisher, author or other copyright holder. Versions of research The version in the Kent Academic Repository may differ from the final published version. Users are advised to check http://kar.kent.ac.uk for the status of the paper. Users should always cite the published version of record. Enquiries For any further enquiries regarding the licence status of this document, please contact: [email protected] If you believe this document infringes copyright then please contact the KAR admin team with the take-down information provided at http://kar.kent.ac.uk/contact.html Urdu Vowel System and Perception of English Vowels by Punjabi-Urdu Speakers Thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy to Department of English Language and Linguistics (ELL) School of European Culture and Languages (SECL) University of Kent, Canterbury by Ishrat Rehman 2019 Abstract A well-defined vocalic and consonantal system is a prerequisite when investigating the perception and production of a second language. The lack of a well-defined Urdu vowel system in the multilingual context of Pakistan motivated investigation of the acoustic and phonetic properties of Urdu vowels.
    [Show full text]
  • Word Study Guide for Parents
    Descriptions of Word Study Patterns/Concepts Purpose of Word Study • It teaches students to examine words to discover the regularities, patterns, and conventions of the English language in order to read and spell. • It increases specific knowledge of words – the spelling and meaning of individual words. 3 Layers of word study • Alphabet – learning the relationship between letters and sounds • Pattern – learning specific groupings of letters and their sounds • Meaning – learning the meaning of groups of letters such as prefixes, suffixes, and roots. Vocabulary increases at this layer. Sorting – organizing words into groups based on similarities in their patterns or meaning. Oddballs – words that cannot be grouped into any of the identified categories of a sort. Students should be taught that there are always words that “break the rules” and do not follow the general pattern. Sound marks / / - Sound marks around a letter or pattern tell the student to focus only on the sound rather than the actual letters. (example: the word gem could be grouped into the /j/ category because it sounds like j at the beginning). Vowel (represented by V) – one of 6 letters causing the mouth to open when vocalized (a, e, i, o, u, and usually y). A single vowel sound is heard in every syllable of a word. Consonants (represented by C) – all letters other than the vowels. Consonant sounds are blocked by the lips, tongue, or teeth during articulation. Concepts in Order of Instruction Initial and final consonants – students are instructed to look at pictures and identify the beginning sound. They then connect the beginning sound to the letter that makes that sound.
    [Show full text]