Pinniped Vocal Communication: an Introduction

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Pinniped Vocal Communication: an Introduction AquaticMammals 2003, 29.2, 177–180 Pinniped vocalcommunication: an introduction Ronald J.Schusterman 1 andSo e M.VanParijs 2 1LongMarine Laboratory, University of CaliforniaSanta Cruz, Santa Cruz, California, 95060, USA 2Institutefor Aquatic Biology, Norwegian College of FisheriesScience, University of Tromsø, Tromsø, Norway Communicationforms the fabric of animalsocial life . ..infact, looking at the way in which animals spend their time,it is striking how much of it they spend either in uencing, or being in uenced by, the behavior of other animals—inother words, in some form of communication. —Dawkins,Unraveling Animal Behavior, p. 71 Thepinnipeds evolved and diverged tens of millions otherdescriptors. The variability in pinniped vocal ofyears ago and spread throughout the oceans of signallingcan be assessed at the level of theindivid- theworld, colonizing isolated shores and feeding ual,as well asby age and gender class, emotive innearby productive waters. While the thirty-three state,geographic location, and species, and the livingspecies of seals, sea lions, and walruses diVerencesdocumented thus far re ect a degreeof inhabitunique behavioural and ecological niches, adaptationin vocal signalling that is rare among onesigni cant commonality amongst these animals mammals.The acoustic characteristics of the sig- isthe apparent structural complexity of theirvocal- nalsproduced by pinnepeds are well-designed to izationsand the degree to which they use these gainthe attention of conspeci cs— incidentally, vocalizationsin communicative contexts. Some thesesounds have also captured the interest and species,including the sea lions and some land- imaginationof humanlisteners. breedingseals, congregate seasonally on crowded Forcenturies, seals have been signi cant in the rookerieswhere the repetitive calls of thousands of culturesof people living in close proximity to them. individualsswell into an incessant din. Other Notsurprisingly, many of the legends and stories species,including most of the aquatically breeding featuringthese animals are concerned with their seals,appear more reserved on their haul-outs, vocalcommunication. In these stories, seals may rarelyproducing audible sounds except for the takeon humanform, talk and sing with people and occasionalguttural threats of adults and the bleats withone another, and give warning of impending ofdependent pups. Many of theseaquatic breeders disaster(Maxwell, 1967; Thomson, 1914/ 1965). wereonce thought to be relatively silent, but Manyof these observations have been published as accumulatingobservations have shown them to factualaccounts. For example, Maxwell (1967) haveunusual and complex underwater vocal reper- refersto a 1920’s newspaperarticle which states toires.Among the pinnipeds, the walruses are that ‘...thereis undoubtedtestimony that the great themost amphibious callers of all, producing fre- greyseals of the Atlantic who visit the islands have quentand assorted aerial and underwater sounds. beenheard singing— for no other word applies to Intriguingly,all pinnipeds appear capable of sound theirvery human voices. ’Manyyears later, a ‘talk- productionon land and in water, and some even ing’harbour seal named Hoover at the New emitcalls simultaneously above and below the EnglandAquarium rekindled the notion that seals water’s surface. couldlearn to speak (Ralls, et al., 1985). Inaddition to the quantity of vocalizations emit- Inthe 1960’ s, scienti c explorationof pinniped tedby pinnipeds is the surprising diversity of the vocalcommunication began in earnest, led by soundsthemselves, which have been categorized ahandfulof dedicated American investigators, bylisteners as clicks, creaks, bleats, growls, barks, includingThomas Poulter, Bill Scheville,Carlton whinnies,buzzes, grunts, snorts, songs, pulses, trills, Ray,Burney Le Bouef,Dick Peterson, Nicholas thumps,blasts, groans, sirens, sweeps, yelps, roars, Collias,George Bartholomew, Ian Stirling, and one hums,chirps, belches, squeals, bells, whistles, ofthe editors of thisissue [RJS]. This early research knocks,mews, chucks, glugs, coos, gongs, clacks, tendedto emphasize structural descriptions of claps,drones, trumpets, whimpers, pu Vs, burps, aerialand underwater sounds emitted by readily snarls,hisses, warbles, moans, and bellows, among accessiblesea lions, fur seals, and elephant seals. In ? 2003 EAAM 178 R.J.Schusterman and S. M.VanParijs additionto recording and describing vocalizations, largelybecause of thebringing together of con ict- thesescientists attempted to establish hypotheses ingterms along with con icting de nitions and regardingthe use of these sounds in socially unworkablemetaphors. These de nitionsand meta- communicativecontexts, as well asin auto- phorsare largely unworkable because of con icts communicativecontexts, namely foraging and navi- betweengroup selectionist and sel sh-genetheory gationvia echolocation. Over the next 40 years,the viewsof communication. While ethologists and possibilityof pinniped echolocation diminished as psychologistsgenerally consider communication, observationsand experiments on their sensory sys- alongwith other aspects of social behaviour, in a temsand their underwater-emitted sounds revealed Darwinianevolutionary framework, many investi- thatspecialized echolocation abilities were lacking gatorsstill opt to describe communication among inmost if not all species (Schusterman et al. 2000). animalsas involving the cooperative sharing of However,ideas about the social functions of their information. vocalizationswere transformed into eld and lab Thisnotion, as described by W. J.Smith(1977), investigationsas moreinformation about the vocal hashad strong support among workers in the eld behaviourof various species began to accumulate. ofmarine mammalogy (Miller, 1991; Tyack & These eVortswere later elevated and expanded by Miller,2002; Dudzinski et al.,2002).From our theadvent of new recording technologies and im- pointof view, the greatest di Ycultythat such an provedgeographic accessibility, which facilitated information-transfermodel has is the confusing of theacoustic monitoring of animalsdiving, foraging, theseparable roles and tnessinterests of signallers andsocializing at sea. andperceivers. For example, in a recentencyclo- Presently,we knowsomething about the vocal paedicreview of communication in marine mam- repertoiresof over three-quarters of pinniped mals,Dudzinski et al.2002state that during an species,including terrestrial and aquatic breeders, exchangeof information, both sender and receiver andpolar, temperate, and tropical species. Early dependon the ‘accurateinterpretation of signalsto researchershave trained and stimulated new meetcommon group challenges such as repro- generationsof scientists, and these investigators duction,predator defense, foraging, and parental continueto re ne,increase, and synthesize our care’ (p.249). The authors also go on to say that understandingof the relationship between sound communicationamong marine mammals promotes productionand behavioural ecology in pinnipeds. groupcohesiveness. It is di Ycultto reconcile such At thestart of the new millennium, researchers aviewof communication with an evolutionary representingseveral generations met at the 2001 approach.According to Darwinian evolution, a meetingof the Society for Marine Mammalogy, in communicativeact should be bene cial to the Vancouver,Canada, to participate in a special sender,but not necessarily the receiver (this is symposiumorganized by the guest editors of particularlytrue of con ictsituations or contests thisissue [SVP and RJS]. This issue of Aquatic overresources where there is often a costincurred Mammals isthe result of that symposium, with bythe receiver). In addition, a Darwinianapproach contributorsincluding the speakers at that meet- suggeststhat communicative acts provide indirect ingand others working on original research in bene tsto receivers only when signals function to the eld. contributeto the welfare of kin, or as part of Followingthe Vancouver symposium in 2001, reciprocatedaltruism occurring among non-kin andour decision to co-edit this volume, we had (Trivers,1985). avarietyof discussions with one another, the Approachinganimal communication, particu- contributors,reviewers, and our colleagues about larlyvocal communication, without using con- pinnipedvocal communication. The topics of structsgleaned from information transfer theory, theseinteractions ranged from editorial issues to Owings& Morton(1998) and Owren &Rendall technologygaps to philosophical points of view. (1997)have taken a similarapproach to dis- Betweenus, one issue about animal communication entanglingthe roles of sender and receiver in emergedas particularly controversial and signi - evolutionary-basedmodels of communication. cant,and we wouldlike to brie youtlineit in this Basically,these theoreticians suggest that vocal introduction. communicationis a re ectionof the fundamental processof signallers regulating the behaviour of Thequote underscoring the importance of com- receivers,and receivers assessing the characteristics municationthat appears at the start of this intro- ofsignallers. From this sel sh-geneperspective, ductionappears in a textbookon animalbehaviour signallerscontrol the behaviour of receiversto their byMarian Stamp Dawkins (1995). In this text, own bene t,and not necessarily to the bene t of Dawkinspoints out that, despite intensive study listeners.Perceivers, on the other hand, receive the andthoughtful theory, the whole subject
Recommended publications
  • 56. Otariidae and Phocidae
    FAUNA of AUSTRALIA 56. OTARIIDAE AND PHOCIDAE JUDITH E. KING 1 Australian Sea-lion–Neophoca cinerea [G. Ross] Southern Elephant Seal–Mirounga leonina [G. Ross] Ross Seal, with pup–Ommatophoca rossii [J. Libke] Australian Sea-lion–Neophoca cinerea [G. Ross] Weddell Seal–Leptonychotes weddellii [P. Shaughnessy] New Zealand Fur-seal–Arctocephalus forsteri [G. Ross] Crab-eater Seal–Lobodon carcinophagus [P. Shaughnessy] 56. OTARIIDAE AND PHOCIDAE DEFINITION AND GENERAL DESCRIPTION Pinnipeds are aquatic carnivores. They differ from other mammals in their streamlined shape, reduction of pinnae and adaptation of both fore and hind feet to form flippers. In the skull, the orbits are enlarged, the lacrimal bones are absent or indistinct and there are never more than three upper and two lower incisors. The cheek teeth are nearly homodont and some conditions of the ear that are very distinctive (Repenning 1972). Both superfamilies of pinnipeds, Phocoidea and Otarioidea, are represented in Australian waters by a number of species (Table 56.1). The various superfamilies and families may be distinguished by important and/or easily observed characters (Table 56.2). King (1983b) provided more detailed lists and references. These and other differences between the above two groups are not regarded as being of great significance, especially as an undoubted fur seal (Australian Fur-seal Arctocephalus pusillus) is as big as some of the sea lions and has some characters of the skull, teeth and behaviour which are rather more like sea lions (Repenning, Peterson & Hubbs 1971; Warneke & Shaughnessy 1985). The Phocoidea includes the single Family Phocidae – the ‘true seals’, distinguished from the Otariidae by the absence of a pinna and by the position of the hind flippers (Fig.
    [Show full text]
  • Hunting and Social Behaviour of Leopard Seals (Hydrurga Leptonyx) at Seal Island, South Shetland Islands, Antarctica
    University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Publications, Agencies and Staff of the U.S. Department of Commerce U.S. Department of Commerce 1999 Hunting and social behaviour of leopard seals (Hydrurga leptonyx) at Seal Island, South Shetland Islands, Antarctica Lisa M. Hiruki National Marine Mammal Laboratory, Alaska Fisheries Science Center, National Marine Fisheries Service, National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration, [email protected] Michael K. Schwartz National Marine Mammal Laboratory, Alaska Fisheries Science Center, National Marine Fisheries Service, National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration Peter L. Boveng National Marine Mammal Laboratory, Alaska Fisheries Science Center, National Marine Fisheries Service, National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/usdeptcommercepub Part of the Environmental Sciences Commons Hiruki, Lisa M.; Schwartz, Michael K.; and Boveng, Peter L., "Hunting and social behaviour of leopard seals (Hydrurga leptonyx) at Seal Island, South Shetland Islands, Antarctica" (1999). Publications, Agencies and Staff of the U.S. Department of Commerce. 151. https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/usdeptcommercepub/151 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the U.S. Department of Commerce at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. It has been accepted for inclusion in Publications, Agencies and Staff of the U.S. Department of Commerce by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. J. Zool., Lond. (1999) 249, 97±109 # 1999 The Zoological Society of London Printed in the United Kingdom Hunting and social behaviour of leopard seals (Hydrurga leptonyx) at Seal Island, South Shetland Islands, Antarctica Lisa M. Hiruki*, Michael K. Schwartz{ and Peter L.
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolution of Human Vocal Emotion
    EMR0010.1177/1754073920930791Emotion ReviewBryant 930791research-article2020 SPECIAL SECTION: EMOTION IN THE VOICE Emotion Review Vol. 13, No. 1 (January 2021) 25 –33 © The Author(s) 2020 ISSN 1754-0739 DOI:https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073920930791 10.1177/1754073920930791 The Evolution of Human Vocal Emotion https://journals.sagepub.com/home/emr Gregory A. Bryant Department of Communication, Center for Behavior, Evolution, and Culture, University of California, Los Angeles, USA Abstract Vocal affect is a subcomponent of emotion programs that coordinate a variety of physiological and psychological systems. Emotional vocalizations comprise a suite of vocal behaviors shaped by evolution to solve adaptive social communication problems. The acoustic forms of vocal emotions are often explicable with reference to the communicative functions they serve. An adaptationist approach to vocal emotions requires that we distinguish between evolved signals and byproduct cues, and understand vocal affect as a collection of multiple strategic communicative systems subject to the evolutionary dynamics described by signaling theory. We should expect variability across disparate societies in vocal emotion according to culturally evolved pragmatic rules, and universals in vocal production and perception to the extent that form–function relationships are present. Keywords emotion, evolution, signaling, vocal affect Emotional communication is central to social life for many ani- 2001; Pisanski, Cartei, McGettigan, Raine, & Reby, 2016; mals. Beginning with Darwin
    [Show full text]
  • Science News | December 19, 2009 Feature | Humans Wonder, Anybody Home?
    FEATURE | HUMANS WONDER, ANYBODY HOME? To prevent Shania the octopus from becom- Humans wonder, ing bored, keepers at the National Aquarium in Washington, D.C., gave her a Mr. Potato Head filled with fish to snuggle. Researchers anybody are now looking beyond behavior into the brain for signs of awareness in birds and invertebrates. home? Brain structure and circuitry offer clues to consciousness in nonmammals By Susan Gaidos ne afternoon while participat­ except for the fact that Betty was a New ing in studies in a University Caledonian crow. of Oxford lab, Abel snatched Betty isn’t the only crow with such a hook away from Betty, leav­ conceptual ingenuity. Nor are crows the Oing her without a tool to complete a task. only members of the animal kingdom to Spying a piece of straight wire nearby, exhibit similar mental powers. Animals POST WASHINGTON THE she picked it up, bent one end into a can do all sorts of clever things: Studies EARY/ L ’ hook and used it to finish the job. Noth­ of chimpanzees, gorillas, dolphins and O ILL ing about this story was remarkable, birds have found that some can add, B 22 | SCIENCE NEWS | December 19, 2009 www.sciencenews.org FEATURE | HUMANS WONDER, ANYBODY HOME? subtract, create sentences, plan ahead objects in their visual field. “This raises or deceive others. the intriguing question whether con­ Brain-on-brain comparisons To carry out such tasks, these animals scious experience requires the specific In an effort to find signs of conscious- ness, scientists are identifying analo- must be drawing on past experiences and structure of human or primate brains,” gous structures (same coloring) among then using them along with immediate biologist Donald Griffin wrote inAnimal the brains of humans, birds, octopuses perceptions to make sense of it all.
    [Show full text]
  • Going Beyond Just-So Stories Response to Commentary on Key on Fish Pain
    Animal Sentience 2016.022: Response to Commentary on Key on Fish Pain Going beyond just-so stories Response to Commentary on Key on Fish Pain Brian Key School of Biomedical Sciences University of Queensland Abstract: Colloquial arguments for fish feeling pain are deeply rooted in anthropometric tendencies that confuse escape responses to noxious stimuli with evidence for consciousness. More developed arguments often rely on just-so stories of fish displaying complex behaviours as proof of consciousness. In response to commentaries on the idea that fish do not feel pain, I raise the need to go beyond just-so stories and to rigorously analyse the neural circuitry responsible for specific behaviours using new and emerging technologies in neuroscience. By deciphering the causal relationship between neural information processing and conscious behaviour, it should be possible to assess cogently the likelihood of whether a vertebrate species has the neural hardware necessary to — at least — support the feeling of pain. Brian Key [email protected] is Head of the Brain Growth and Regeneration Lab at the University of Queensland. He is dedicated to understanding the principles of stem cell biology, differentiation, axon guidance, plasticity, regeneration and development of the brain. http://www.uq.edu.au/sbms/staff/brian-key Just-so stories In this response to commentaries on the target article “Fish do not feel pain” (Key), I do not plan to interrogate either the putative behavioural evidence or the just-so stories previously proposed as supportive of fish feeling pain (Balcombe; Braithwaite & Droege; Broom; Brown; Dinets; Ng). These claims have been adequately addressed and refuted in recent literature (Key, 2015a and 2015b; Rose et al., 2014).
    [Show full text]
  • First Record of a Galapagos Fur Seal (Arctocephalus Galapagoensis) In
    Quintana-Rizzo et al. Marine Biodiversity Records (2017) 10:24 DOI 10.1186/s41200-017-0126-x MARINERECORD Open Access First record of a Galapagos fur seal (Arctocephalus galapagoensis) in Guatemala Ester Quintana-Rizzo1,4* , Lucia Garcia2, Rudy José López1, Susana Tobar-Hurtado3,5 and Airam López-Roulet1 Abstract Background: Galapagos fur seals, Arctocephalus galapagoensis, inhabit the Galapagos Archipelago. The species is considered to be non-migratory, remaining in the Galapagos year-round. On 9 June 2016, a live fur seal stranded on the Pacific coast of Guatemala. Results: Morphological characteristics (slightly rounded crown, short pointed muzzle with a lighter colored pelage extending into the face and over the eyes forming a small pale mask, tan ventrum contrasting with grizzled gray-brown fur on the back and sides) were used to identify the pinniped as a Galapagos fur seal, Arctocephalus galapagoensis. Additionally, the fur seal was estimated to be a subadult male based on body size and the presence of testicles. Total body length was estimated to be 140 cm and the weight was estimated to be 90 lb. For comparison, adult males of A. galapagoensis measured to date are slightly larger with an average length and weight equal to 150–160 cm and 132–150 lb., respectively. The species identification and age-class determination were confirmed by pinniped specialists. Conclusion: This is the first record of Galapagos fur seal in the Pacific coast of Guatemala and in the Central America region. Keywords: Arctocephalus galapagoensis, Galapagos fur seal, Guatemala, Stranding Background In the Americas, the distribution of Arctocephalus is The marine mammal fauna of Guatemala is comprised of limited to four species inhabiting localized areas: A.
    [Show full text]
  • A Novel Reinforcement Model of Birdsong Vocalization Learning
    A Novel Reinforcement Model of Birdsong Vocalization Learning Kenji Doya Terrence J. Sejnowski ATR Human Infonnation Processing Howard Hughes Medical Institute Research Laboratories UCSD and Salk Institute, 2-2 Hikaridai, Seika, Kyoto 619-02, Japan San Diego, CA 92186-5800, USA Abstract Songbirds learn to imitate a tutor song through auditory and motor learn­ ing. We have developed a theoretical framework for song learning that accounts for response properties of neurons that have been observed in many of the nuclei that are involved in song learning. Specifically, we suggest that the anteriorforebrain pathway, which is not needed for song production in the adult but is essential for song acquisition, provides synaptic perturbations and adaptive evaluations for syllable vocalization learning. A computer model based on reinforcement learning was con­ structed that could replicate a real zebra finch song with 90% accuracy based on a spectrographic measure. The second generation of the bird­ song model replicated the tutor song with 96% accuracy. 1 INTRODUCTION Studies of motor pattern generation have generally focussed on innate motor behaviors that are genetically preprogrammed and fine-tuned by adaptive mechanisms (Harris-Warrick et al., 1992). Birdsong learning provides a favorable opportunity for investigating the neuronal mechanisms for the acquisition of complex motor patterns. Much is known about the neuroethology of bird song and its neuroanatomical substrate (see Nottebohm, 1991 and Doupe, 1993 for reviews), but relatively little is known about the overall system from a computational viewpoint. We propose a set of hypotheses for the functions of the brain nuclei in the song system and explore their computational strength in a model based on biological constraints.
    [Show full text]
  • THE PINNIPEDS of the CALIFORNIA CURRENT California
    ANTONELIS AND FISCUS: PINNIPEDS OF THE CALIFORNIA CURRENT CalCOFI Rep., Vol. XXI, 1980 THE PINNIPEDS OF THE CALIFORNIA CURRENT GEORGE A. ANTONELIS. JR. AND CLIFFORD H. FISCUS Marine Mammal Division Northwest and Alaska Fisheries Center National Marine Fisheries Service National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration 7600 Sand Point Way, N.E. Seattle, WA 981 15 ABSTRACT 10s pequenos peces en 10s cardumenes y peces ana- There are six species of pinnipeds-California sea dromos. Los dos focidos, otra vez con ciertas excep- lion, Zalophus californianus; northern sea lion, Eume- ciones, predan especies diferentes. Aparentemente, el topias jubatus; northern fur seal, Callorhinus ursinus; elefante marino se alimenta en aguas mas profundas que Guadalupe fur seal, Arctocephalus townsendi; harbor la foca peluda, alimentindose de especies demersales seal, Phoca uitulina richardsi; and northern elephant y benticas, y la foca peluda se alimenta de especiesdemer- seal, Mirounga angustirostris-that inhabit the study sales costeras y neriticas, entrando ocasionalmente en rios area of the California Cooperative Oceanic Fisheries y aguas estuarinas haciendopresa de 10s peces anadromos Investigations (CalCOFI). y otros pequeiios peces que entran regularmente en estas The numbers of animals in each population are given; aguas. the size, distribution, and seasonal movements are de- scribed. The known prey species of the pinnipeds are INTRODUCTION listed for each species. The otariids, with certain excep- The California Current, its components, and the Cali- tions, consume the same kinds of prey, although in slight- fornia Cooperative Oceanic Fisheries Investigations ly different amounts. In general they feed most commonly (CalCOFI) station plan have been described many times on the smaller schooling fishes and squids of the epi- in the past and are well known (Kramer et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Quantification of Developmental Birdsong Learning from The
    Quantification of developmental birdsong learning from the subsyllabic scale to cultural evolution Dina Lipkind1 and Ofer Tchernichovski Department of Psychology, Hunter College, City University of New York, New York, NY 10065 Edited by Donald W. Pfaff, The Rockefeller University, New York, NY, and approved February 4, 2011 (received for review August 31, 2010) Quantitative analysis of behavior plays an important role in bird- The genome of a songbird was sequenced earlier this year (12), song neuroethology, serving as a common denominator in studies motivated in part by the goal of discovering the genetic spanning molecular to system-level investigation of sensory-motor changes (as revealed by comparison with the chicken genome) conversion, developmental learning, and pattern generation in that led to the evolution of vocal learning. In contrast to auditory the brain. In this review, we describe the role of behavioral analysis learning (the ability to discriminate between sounds), vocal in facilitating cross-level integration. Modern sound analysis ap- learning (the ability to imitate complex sounds) is rare in nature, but in birds it probably evolved in three out of the 25–28 known proaches allow investigation of developmental song learning across – multiple time scales. Combined with novel methods that allow orders: songbirds, parrots, and hummingbirds (13 17). Aside experimental control of vocal changes, it is now possible to test from humans, vocal learning in mammals has been conclusively hypotheses about mechanisms of vocal learning. Further, song demonstrated only in dolphins, whales (18), and bats (19), but not in apes. Vocal learners share similar forebrain structures neces- analysis can be done at the population level across generations to sary to produce and acquire their learned vocalization (whether track cultural evolution and multigenerational behavioral processes.
    [Show full text]
  • Diet of a Mediterranean Monk Seal Monachus Monachus in a Transitional Post-Weaning Phase and Its Implications for the Conservation of the Species
    Vol. 39: 315–320, 2019 ENDANGERED SPECIES RESEARCH Published August 22 https://doi.org/10.3354/esr00971 Endang Species Res OPENPEN ACCESSCCESS NOTE Diet of a Mediterranean monk seal Monachus monachus in a transitional post-weaning phase and its implications for the conservation of the species Cem Orkun Kıraç1,*, Meltem Ok2 1Underwater Research Society - Mediterranean Seal Research Group (SAD-AFAG), 06570 Ankara, Turkey 2Middle East Technical University - Institute of Marine Science (METU-IMS), Erdemli, 33731 Mersin, Turkey ABSTRACT: The Mediterranean monk seal Monachus monachus is the most endangered pin- niped in the world and is considered Endangered by the IUCN. Transition from suckling to active feeding is a critical time in the development of all mammal species, and understanding the dietary requirements of seals during this vulnerable period is of value in establishing conservation meas- ures, such as fishery regulations. This study provides unique information on the dietary habits of a moulted monk seal pup, through the opportunistic necropsy of a dead animal encountered at a very early age (5 mo). A total of 6 prey items from 2 families (Octopodidae, 90.8% and Congridae, 8.9%) were identified from stomach contents. The remaining stomach content mass consisted of fish bones from unidentified species (0.3%). The estimated age, low diversity and number of prey items in the stomach contents indicate that this individual may have been in a transition period from suckling to active feeding. The study confirms independent foraging in Mediterranean monk seals at about 5 mo of age. Given the importance of early life survival for maintaining stable Medi- terranean monk seal populations, and the occurrence of an ontogenetic shift in its close relative (Hawaiian monk seal), these findings contribute to the establishment and implementation of suc- cessful conservation and management strategies for this Endangered species.
    [Show full text]
  • Isochrony, Vocal Learning and the Acquisition of Rhythm and Melody - Andrea Ravignani in Press: Behavioral and Brain Sciences
    Isochrony, vocal learning and the acquisition of rhythm and melody - Andrea Ravignani In press: Behavioral and Brain Sciences Isochrony, vocal learning and the acquisition of rhythm and melody Andrea Ravignani Comparative Bioacoustics Group, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, NL [email protected] ABSTRACT A cross-species perspective can extend and provide testable predictions for Savage et al.’s framework. Rhythm and melody, I argue, could bootstrap each other in the evolution of musicality. Isochrony may function as a temporal grid to support rehearsing and learning modulated, pitched vocalizations. Once this melodic plasticity is acquired, focus can shift back to refining rhythm processing and beat induction. Musicality consists of the lobster rattles to sea lion barks: Patek & (neuro)biological underpinnings to perceive Caldwell, 2006; Schusterman, 1977), and produce music. Research in the evolution autonomously-regulated behavior or of musicality needs cross-species evidence. As (neuro)physiology. Synchrony is widespread a parallel, to understand the evolution of bat but scattered across taxonomic groups wings, one asks why all other mammals lack (Ravignani et al., 2014; Wilson & Cook, 2016). wings and why other flying animals have Vocal learning is rare but potentially arose evolved them. Likewise, our species only multiple times in evolution due to different constitutes one datapoint to construct pressures across species (Nowicki & Searcy, evolutionary hypotheses on musicality. 2014; Garcia & Ravignani, 2020; Martins & Comparisons with other species are necessary Boeckx, 2020). Beat induction has only been to avoid post-hoc explanations of evolutionary found in a few animals, as acknowledged by traits. Savage and colleagues (Kotz et al., 2018; cf.
    [Show full text]
  • Haul-Out Counts and Photo-Identification of Pinnipeds in Chesapeake Bay and Eastern Shore, Virginia: 2018/2019 Annual Progress Report
    Haul-out Counts and Photo-Identification of Pinnipeds in Chesapeake Bay and Eastern Shore, Virginia: 2018/2019 Annual Progress Report Prepared by Danielle V. Jones and Deanna R. Rees Naval Facilities Engineering Command Atlantic Norfolk, Virginia March 5, 2020 Suggested Citation: Jones D.V. and Rees, D.R. 2020. Haul-out Counts and Photo-Identification of Pinnipeds in Chesapeake Bay and Eastern Shore, Virginia: 2018/2019 Annual Progress Report. Final Report. Prepared for U.S. Fleet Forces Command, Norfolk, Virginia. 5 March 2020. Cover Photo Credit: Harbor seals (Phoca vitulina) and a gray seal (Halichoerus grypus atlantica) hauled out at a survey site on the Eastern Shore, Virginia. Cover photo by Naval Facilities Engineering Command, Atlantic. Photo taken by Danielle Jones, Naval Facilities Engineering Command Atlantic, under NMFS General Authorization Permit #19826. This project is funded by United States Fleet Forces Command and carried out by Naval Facilities Engineering Command Atlantic as part of the U.S. Navy’s marine species monitoring program Pinniped Haul-out Counts and Photo-ID, Virginia: 2018/2019 Annual Progress Report Table of Contents Acronyms and Abbreviations .................................................................................................iii 1. Introduction and Background .......................................................................................... 1 2. Methods ............................................................................................................................. 2 2.1
    [Show full text]