What Is “Brain Training”?

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

What Is “Brain Training”? “Train Your Brain” for Cognitive Gain? A Systematic Review and Meta- Analysis of Commercial Brain Training Programs Presented by Lan Nguyen Griffith University Cognitive Ageing 472,000+ People living with dementia in Australia (2021) Projection of dementia +227% prevalence rates (2021-58) dementia.org.au Projection of cognitive & brain training market value +356% (2020-25) USD 3.2billion to 11.4billion Growth of people aged 65+ in Australia “Brain Training” What is “brain training”? ▪ commercial apps/programs used on smart devices ▪ subscriptions can cost hundreds of dollars/year ▪ aim to improve cognition and daily functioning ▪ mini-games targeting different cognitive abilities Claims Made by the Industry Lumosity Brain HQ CogniFit “sharpen the skills you “improve [your] driving “strengthen the cognitive use every day” safety and confidence state of people who are behind the wheel” beginning to suffer cognitive pathology” Posit Science Personal Zen NeuroNation “think faster, focus better, “reduce stress and “used successfully in and remember more” anxiety within minutes” Dementia prevention as well as for stress and burnout prevention” -“Scientifically validated!” - “Results are proven!” “Brain Training” Studies Research Evaluating “brain training” ▪ pre-test → training → post-test ▪ assess variety of measures (affective, cognitive, neural) ▪ evaluate improvement on measures compared to control group How does “brain training” work? Founded on the premise that: training → neuroplasticity* → efficient cognitive processes = better performance & real-world functioning Current Meta-Analytic Investigation Aim: To evaluate the efficacy of commercial “brain training” programs in improving cognitive functioning for older adults with and without mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Rationale: A synthesis of the equivocal findings is needed. Researchers & general public need to be better informed. RQs: Are “brain training” programs effective in improving cognitive functioning in older adults? Systematic Literature Search ▪ Initial Google search to identify available commercial programs ▪ Systematic database search: o CINAHL o Embase o ProQuest o PsycINFO o PubMed o Scopus o Web of Science o Other sources (books, theses & dissertations database, etc.) Inclusion Criteria DESIGN PARTICIPANTS MEASURES ▪ pre-test—training—post- test design ▪ older adults (with or ▪ objective measures of without MCI) cognitive functioning ▪ commercial brain training (no other components) ▪ mean sample age at least ▪ subjective measures of 60 years cognitive/daily functioning ▪ control group needed Commercial Brain Training Programs Identified: 1. BrainGymmer 5. Dakim 2. BrainHQ 6. Lumosity 3. CogMed 7. MyBrainTrainer 4. CogniFit Data Analysis Effect of Brain Training ▪ Hedges’ g o Direction of effect (+) training > control (–) control > training o Magnitude of effect g = 0.20 (small effect) Planned Analyses g = 0.50 (moderate effect) Training efficacy (healthy & MCI) g = 0.80 (large effect) ▪ Corrected for publication bias ▪ attention ▪ memory ▪ everyday functioning ▪ processing speed ▪ executive functioning ▪ visuospatial ability ▪ fluid intelligence Results – Healthy Older Adults PROCESSING SPEED MEMORY EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING Sig. small training effect Sig. small training effect but Sig. small training effect but ✓ 18 studies nonsig. adjusting for bias nonsig. adjusting for bias ✓ g = 0.18, p = .045 ‒ 25 studies ‒ 28 studies ‒ g = 0.26, p < .001 ‒ g = 0.19, p < .001 ‒ gadjusted = 0.07, p > .05 ‒ gadjusted = 0.11, p > .05 EVERYDAY FUNCTIONING FLUID INTELLIGENCE OTHER DOMAINS Objective nonsig. No training effect (nonsig.) No training effect (nonsig.) x 7 studies x 7 studies x Attention (13) x g = 0.12, p = 064 x g = -0.06, p = .578 x Language (3) Subjective sig. small effect x Visuospatial ability (7) ✓ 10 studies ✓ g = 0.22, p = .001 Healthy Older Adults— Everyday Functioning (objective) Effect size of individual studies Line of “no effect” Average effect size Healthy Older Adults— Everyday Functioning (subjective) Results – Older Adults with MCI MEMORY EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING OTHER DOMAINS No training effect (nonsig.) No training effect (nonsig.) Insufficient studies for analysis x 5 studies x 5 studies x Attention x g = 0.33, p = .058 x g = 0.04, p = .749 x Everyday functioning x Fluid Intelligence x Language x Processing speed x Visuospatial ability Summary of Findings HEALTHY MCI ATTENTION ns — EVERYDAY FUNCTIONING (OBJECTIVE) ns — EVERYDAY FUNCTIONING (SUBJECTIVE) sig — EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING sigx ns FLUID INTELLIGENCE ns — MEMORY sigx ns PROCESSING SPEED sig — VISUOSPATIAL ABILITY ns — Note: sigx = nonsignificant when adjusting for publication bias Limitations & Future Directions ➢ Publication bias ➢ Very few studies involving older adults with MCI ➢ Very few studies examining ‘real-world’ outcomes ➢ Programs could be better adapted to improve everyday functioning Note: “Brain training” does not represent all cognitive training or cognitive remediation interventions. Conclusion ▪ Currently no compelling evidence to substantiate brain training companies’ claims. ▪ May confer some benefits but more evidence is needed. “Brain training” – and any other intervention – is not a silver bullet. A balance of healthy cognitive, physical, and social activities is important for maintaining a healthy and active lifestyle. Thank you.
Recommended publications
  • Is Lumosity an Effective Brain Training Program?: a Meta-Analysis of the Existing Research
    IS LUMOSITY AN EFFECTIVE BRAIN TRAINING PROGRAM?: A META-ANALYSIS OF THE EXISTING RESEARCH A thesis presented to the faculty of the Graduate School of Western Carolina University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Specialist in School Psychology. Kristen Nicole Hyman Director: Dr. Candace Boan-Lenzo, Department of Psychology Committee Members: Dr. Ethan Schilling, Department of Psychology Dr. Mickey Randolph, Department of Psychology April 2017 TABLE OF CONTENTS Title Page ......................................................................................................................................... i Table of Contents ............................................................................................................................ ii List of Tables ................................................................................................................................. iii List of Figures ................................................................................................................................ iv Abstract .......................................................................................................................................... v Chapter One: Introduction .............................................................................................................. 1 Chapter Two: Literature Review .................................................................................................... 2 Definition of Working Memory ................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Behavioural Interventions to Remediate Learning Disorders: a Technical Report
    Behavioural Interventions to Remediate Learning Disorders: A Technical Report 23 March 2015 George Dawson & Stephanie D'Souza Supervised by: Dr. David Moreau PhD & A/Prof. Karen Waldie PhD Centre for Brain Research and School of Psychology The University of Auckland Auckland, New Zealand Contact: [email protected] Behavioural interventions to remediate learning disorders: A technical report TABLE OF CONTENTS I Introduction ............................................................................................................ 3 II Audited Programmes .............................................................................................. 4 A Arrowsmith Programme ........................................................................................ 4 B Brain Gym ........................................................................................................... 8 C Cellfield ............................................................................................................. 17 D Cogmed Working Memory Training ........................................................................ 21 E Coloured Overlays and Lenses (including those from Irlen and The Institute of Optometry) ............................................................................................................. 30 F Danks Davis Dyslexia Tutoring .............................................................................. 34 G Davis Dyslexia ...................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • A CASE STUDY of the LEARNING DISABILITIES ASSOCIATION of SASKATCHEWAN (LDAS) ARROWSMITH PROGRAM a Thesis Submitted to the Colleg
    A CASE STUDY OF THE LEARNING DISABILITIES ASSOCIATION OF SASKATCHEWAN (LDAS) ARROWSMITH PROGRAM A Thesis submitted to the College of Graduate Studies and Research in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Department of Educational Psychology & Special Education University of Saskatchewan Saskatoon by Debra Kemp-Koo © Debra Kemp-Koo, November 2013. All rights reserved. Permission to Use In presenting this thesis in partial fulfillment of the requirements for a Postgraduate degree from the University of Saskatchewan, I agree that the libraries of this university may make it freely available for inspection. I further agree that permissions for copying of this thesis in a manner, in whole or in part, for scholarly purposes may be granted by the professor who supervised my thesis work, or in his absence, by the Dean of the College in which the work was done. It is understood that any copying, publication, or use of this thesis, or parts thereof, for financial gain shall not be allowed without my written permission. It is also understood that due recognition shall be given to me and to the University of Saskatchewan in any scholarly use that may be made of any material in my thesis. Request for permission to copy or to make other use of materials in this thesis in whole or in part should be addressed to: Department Head, Educational Psychology and Special Education, College of Education, University of Saskatchewan 28 Campus Drive Saskatoon, SK, S&N 0X1 i Acknowledgements This completed dissertation is the culmination of a long held but greatly delayed goal.
    [Show full text]
  • Training the Brain: Fact and Fad in Cognitive and Behavioral Remediation
    Brain and Cognition 79 (2012) 159–179 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Brain and Cognition journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/b&c Special Invited Review Training the brain: Fact and fad in cognitive and behavioral remediation ⇑ Sheida Rabipour a, Amir Raz a,b, a Department of Neurology & Neurosurgery, McGill University, Canada b Departments of Psychiatry & Psychology, McGill University, Canada article info abstract Article history: Putatively safe and effective for improving cognitive performance in both health and disease, products Accepted 13 February 2012 purported to train the brain appeal to consumers and healthcare practitioners. In an increasingly Available online 30 March 2012 health-centered society, these applications constitute a burgeoning commercial market. Sparse evidence coupled with lack of scientific rigor, however, leaves claims concerning the impact and duration of such Keywords: brain training largely unsubstantiated. On the other hand, at least some scientific findings seem to sup- Attention port the effectiveness and sustainability of training for higher brain functions such as attention and work- Brain training ing memory. In the present paper we provide a tectonic integration and synthesis of cognitive training Cognitive remediation approaches. Specifically, we sketch the relative merits and shortcomings of these programs, which often Computerized training programs Neuroplasticity appeal to parents who must choose between side-effect-laden medication and other less conventional Working memory options. Here we examine how neuroplasticity allows the healthy as well the impaired to benefit from cognitive training programs. We evaluate the evidence and consider whether brain training can be a stand-alone treatment or an adjunct to pharmacotherapy, outline promising future prospects, and high- light what training outcomes are plausible in line with available data.
    [Show full text]
  • A CASE STUDY of the LEARNING DISABILITIES ASSOCIATION of SASKATCHEWAN (LDAS) ARROWSMITH PROGRAM a Thesis Submitted to the Colleg
    A CASE STUDY OF THE LEARNING DISABILITIES ASSOCIATION OF SASKATCHEWAN (LDAS) ARROWSMITH PROGRAM A Thesis submitted to the College of Graduate Studies and Research in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Department of Educational Psychology & Special Education University of Saskatchewan Saskatoon by Debra Kemp-Koo © Debra Kemp-Koo, November 2013. All rights reserved. Permission to Use In presenting this thesis in partial fulfillment of the requirements for a Postgraduate degree from the University of Saskatchewan, I agree that the libraries of this university may make it freely available for inspection. I further agree that permissions for copying of this thesis in a manner, in whole or in part, for scholarly purposes may be granted by the professor who supervised my thesis work, or in his absence, by the Dean of the College in which the work was done. It is understood that any copying, publication, or use of this thesis, or parts thereof, for financial gain shall not be allowed without my written permission. It is also understood that due recognition shall be given to me and to the University of Saskatchewan in any scholarly use that may be made of any material in my thesis. Request for permission to copy or to make other use of materials in this thesis in whole or in part should be addressed to: Department Head, Educational Psychology and Special Education, College of Education, University of Saskatchewan 28 Campus Drive Saskatoon, SK, S&N 0X1 i Acknowledgements This completed dissertation is the culmination of a long held but greatly delayed goal.
    [Show full text]
  • Neuroplasticity and Neuroscience in Education
    NEUROPLASTICITY AND NEUROSCIENCE IN EDUCATION Ricoh Fellowship 2016/7 Eden Chapman NEUROPLASTICITY AND NEUROSCIENCE IN EDUCATION NEUROPLASTICITY AND NEUROSCIENCE IN EDUCATION RICOH FELLOWSHIP 201 6/7 EDEN CHAPMAN Contents Applying Neuroplasticity to Education = Neuroeducation ____________________________________ 2 How it differs from what we currently do __________________________________________________________ 2 How can we apply it in New Zealand schools? ______________________________________________________ 3 Summary of Findings __________________________________________________________________________ 3 Recommendations ____________________________________________________________________________ 3 Arrowsmith Programme _____________________________________________________________ 5 Barbara Arrowsmith Young _____________________________________________________________________ 5 Fulltime Programme ___________________________________________________________________________ 5 Exercise Examples (2 of 8 exercises) ______________________________________________________________ 5 School visits – Melbourne Australia ______________________________________________________ 6 St Peter’s Primary School _______________________________________________________________________ 6 Oakleigh Grammar School _____________________________________________________________________ 6 Plenty Valley Christian College __________________________________________________________________ 6 School visits – Christchurch New Zealand _________________________________________________
    [Show full text]
  • News Coverage
    NEWS COVERAGE Oldest adults may have much to gain from social technology, according to Stanford research By ALEX SHASHKEVICH NOVEMBER 28, 2016 The oldest of the adults in the U.S. who use the internet, cellphones and other communication technology report feeling less lonely, more satisfied, and more physically fit. Adults over 80 who use information and communication technology are more likely to report mental and physical well-being, according to Stanford research. “Critics say that people might not be able to connect with others as well as they used to because of the spread of new technologies,” said Tamara Sims, a research scientist at the Stanford Center on Longevity. “But there really is this bright side of technology, especially for older people, who may not have the opportunity to connect with many family members to the extent they want to due to physical limitations or geographical separation.” The study specifically found that adults over the age of 80 were likely to report using technology because it helps them connect with friends and family. They also found that those who reported using technology to primarily connect with loved ones reported higher mental well-being. Those who said they used technology mostly to learn new information reported being more physically fit. Sims was a lead author on the research paper, which was published in the Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences. She conducted the study along with Andrew Reed, a former Stanford postdoctoral fellow, and Dawn Carr, an assistant professor of sociology at Florida State University. Research gap on oldest of the old The population of people age 80 and older is the fastest growing segment in the United States, but there are relatively few scientific studies about them compared to other age groups, said Sims, a former Stanford psychology postdoctoral fellow.
    [Show full text]
  • Do “Brain-Training” Programs Work? 2016, Vol
    PSIXXX10.1177/1529100616661983Simons et al.Brain Training 661983research-article2016 Psychological Science in the Public Interest Do “Brain-Training” Programs Work? 2016, Vol. 17(3) 103–186 © The Author(s) 2016 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/1529100616661983 pspi.sagepub.com Daniel J. Simons1, Walter R. Boot2, Neil Charness2,3, Susan E. Gathercole4,5, Christopher F. Chabris6,7, David Z. Hambrick8, and Elizabeth A. L. Stine-Morrow9,10 1Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign; 2Department of Psychology, Florida State University; 3Institute for Successful Longevity, Florida State University; 4Medical Research Council Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK; 5School of Clinical Medicine, University of Cambridge; 6Department of Psychology, Union College; 7Geisinger Health System, Danville, PA; 8Department of Psychology, Michigan State University; 9Department of Educational Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign; and 10Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign Summary In 2014, two groups of scientists published open letters on the efficacy of brain-training interventions, or “brain games,” for improving cognition. The first letter, a consensus statement from an international group of more than 70 scientists, claimed that brain games do not provide a scientifically grounded way to improve cognitive functioning or to stave off cognitive decline. Several months later, an international group of 133 scientists and practitioners countered that the literature is replete with demonstrations of the benefits of brain training for a wide variety of cognitive and everyday activities. How could two teams of scientists examine the same literature and come to conflicting “consensus” views about the effectiveness of brain training? In part, the disagreement might result from different standards used when evaluating the evidence.
    [Show full text]
  • Fluid Intelligence and Lumosity Training Schedule – Any Effect?
    International Journal of Humanities Social Sciences and Education (IJHSSE) Volume 5, Issue 10, October 2018, PP 113-144 ISSN 2349-0373 (Print) & ISSN 2349-0381 (Online) http://dx.doi.org/10.20431/2349-0381.0510011 www.arcjournals.org Fluid Intelligence and Lumosity Training Schedule – Any Effect? Peter James Kpolovie Room 216, Education Building, Department of Educational Psychology, Guidance & Counselling, Faculty of Education, University of Port Harcourt, P.M.B. 5323, Port Harcourt; Nigeria *Corresponding Author: Peter James Kpolovie, Room 216, Education Building, Department of Educational Psychology, Guidance & Counselling, Faculty of Education, University of Port Harcourt, P.M.B. 5323, Port Harcourt; Nigeria Abstract: The most needed asset in the ever-competitive 21st century and beyond is an extraordinarily enhanced intelligence and unlimited memory. Cognitive science application to real-world learning is undergoing new unquenchable technological fire. Results of investigations that online brain training can improve fluid intelligence in healthy individuals is debated fiercely by neuroscientists, psychologists, geneticists, dietitians, ICT specialists, researchers and experts in various works of life. Theories that underlie brain features and intelligence are critically re-examined with a view to better understand the phenomena of behavioural training and the diverse electrical stimulations that are being proposed as a function of the radically changing world of information and communication technology. The current experiment investigated the effect of Lumosity training schedule on fluid intelligence, measured with Culture Fair Intelligence Test. In all, 60 Lumosity brain enrichment games, covering seven skills (attention, memory, problem solving, flexibility, processing speed, math and language), served as experimental intervention conditions. The experiment lasted for 60 days with 150 healthy 15-year old students randomized into 4 experimental and 1 control groups.
    [Show full text]
  • Do “Brain-Training” Programs Work? 2016, Vol
    PSIXXX10.1177/1529100616661983Simons et al.Brain Training 661983research-article2016 Psychological Science in the Public Interest Do “Brain-Training” Programs Work? 2016, Vol. 17(3) 103–186 © The Author(s) 2016 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/1529100616661983 pspi.sagepub.com Daniel J. Simons1, Walter R. Boot2, Neil Charness2,3, Susan E. Gathercole4,5, Christopher F. Chabris6,7, David Z. Hambrick8, and Elizabeth A. L. Stine-Morrow9,10 1Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign; 2Department of Psychology, Florida State University; 3Institute for Successful Longevity, Florida State University; 4Medical Research Council Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK; 5School of Clinical Medicine, University of Cambridge; 6Department of Psychology, Union College; 7Geisinger Health System, Danville, PA; 8Department of Psychology, Michigan State University; 9Department of Educational Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign; and 10Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign Summary In 2014, two groups of scientists published open letters on the efficacy of brain-training interventions, or “brain games,” for improving cognition. The first letter, a consensus statement from an international group of more than 70 scientists, claimed that brain games do not provide a scientifically grounded way to improve cognitive functioning or to stave off cognitive decline. Several months later, an international group of 133 scientists and practitioners countered that the literature is replete with demonstrations of the benefits of brain training for a wide variety of cognitive and everyday activities. How could two teams of scientists examine the same literature and come to conflicting “consensus” views about the effectiveness of brain training? In part, the disagreement might result from different standards used when evaluating the evidence.
    [Show full text]
  • Training the Brain: Fact and Fad in Cognitive and Behavioral Remediation
    This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution and sharing with colleagues. Other uses, including reproduction and distribution, or selling or licensing copies, or posting to personal, institutional or third party websites are prohibited. In most cases authors are permitted to post their version of the article (e.g. in Word or Tex form) to their personal website or institutional repository. Authors requiring further information regarding Elsevier’s archiving and manuscript policies are encouraged to visit: http://www.elsevier.com/copyright Author's personal copy Brain and Cognition 79 (2012) 159–179 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Brain and Cognition journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/b&c Special Invited Review Training the brain: Fact and fad in cognitive and behavioral remediation ⇑ Sheida Rabipour a, Amir Raz a,b, a Department of Neurology & Neurosurgery, McGill University, Canada b Departments of Psychiatry & Psychology, McGill University, Canada article info abstract Article history: Putatively safe and effective for improving cognitive performance in both health and disease, products Accepted 13 February 2012 purported to train the brain appeal to consumers and healthcare practitioners. In an increasingly Available online 30 March 2012 health-centered society, these applications constitute a burgeoning commercial market. Sparse evidence coupled with lack of scientific rigor, however, leaves claims concerning the impact and duration of such Keywords: brain training largely unsubstantiated. On the other hand, at least some scientific findings seem to sup- Attention port the effectiveness and sustainability of training for higher brain functions such as attention and work- Brain training ing memory.
    [Show full text]