Obituary the Nine Lives of Richard D. Alexander

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Obituary the Nine Lives of Richard D. Alexander Evolution and Human Behavior 40 (2019) 133–139 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Evolution and Human Behavior journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ens Obituary: The nine lives of Richard D. Alexander T ⁎ Kyle Summersa, , David Lahtib, Stanton Braudec, Beverly Strassmannd, Joan Strassmannc a Department of Biology, East Carolina University, Greenville, NC 27858, United States b Department of Biology, Queens College, City University of New York, Flushing, NY, 11367, United States c Department of Biology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO 63130, United States d Department of Anthropology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48104, United States Richard D. Alexander, T.H. Hubbell Distinguished University carver. In addition to his professional scientific writings, he was a Professor of Biology at the University of Michigan, Curator of Insects at prolific author of many kinds of books, including children's stories, the Museum of Zoology there, a member of the National Academy of biographical texts and practical guides, especially on horse training Sciences, and a pioneer in the study of the evolutionary basis of human (Fig. 2). behavior, died on the 20th of August 2018 at age 88. Alexander is survived by his wife of 68 years Lorraine Kearnes Alexander; his brother 1. Evolutionary entomology Noel (Donna); his daughters Susan (Sarita) and Nancy; his grand- children Morgan, Lydia, Lincoln, and Winona; and his great-grandson Alexander began his professional career in the bosom of entomology, Ezekiel; several nieces and nephews; and “young” Tom Pyle who lived with a firm focus on the inter-related fields of acoustic communication, with Alexander's family for years while growing up (Fig. 1). systematics and speciation. It was his expertise in those fields that Alexander's life is an iconic American success story. He rose from equipped him to become a major contributor to our understanding of the humble beginnings, having been born on November 18th, 1929 and evolution of cooperation in general and a leading thinker on the evolu- raised on a small, single-family farm in rural Illinois, without electricity tion of the social behavior of that “uniquely unique” species – humans. or indoor plumbing. Although his early schooling took place in a one His early studies of insect behavior convinced him that behavior and room school house, and he had no thought of attending college, his communication are clearly evolved phenomena, underlain by genes as good grades and keen intellect enabled him to attend Blackburn College certainly as morphology is, an insight he came to partly because of the and then Illinois State Normal University, obtaining his bachelor's de- utility of behavior in distinguishing species. This understanding led him gree in 1950. He served in the army during the Korean War, stationed at to realize the importance of behavior in adaptive evolution, leading Fort Knox, then went on complete a PhD in Entomology at Ohio State naturally to further insights into social evolution. The communication University in 1956. As a graduate student he pioneered the use of new systems of crickets had consequences for diversification and speciation, acoustic recording technology, developed in WWII, to study insect be- and the social behavior of insects like wasps that benefitted offspring, as havior, revolutionizing the study of acoustic communication and spe- well as more distant kin, via kin selection and mutualism – were all ciation in the process. In 1957, he was hired by the University of comprehensible in terms of Darwinian selection and likewise applicable Michigan, where he spent his career. At Michigan Alexander became a to humans (Fig. 3). national leader in evolutionary biology. His early career awards include From beginnings in evolutionary entomology, Richard Alexander the Newcomb Cleveland Prize (1961) from the American Association developed multiple, ground-breaking theories concerning the evolution for the Advancement of Science for his paper “The role of behavioral and development of key human social traits, including monogamy, ju- study in cricket classification” and the Daniel Giraud Eliot medal (1971) venile helplessness (altriciality), parental and alloparental care, incest from the National Academy of Sciences for “outstanding fundamental and cousin-marriage, cooperation in increasingly large social groups work on the systematics, evolution, and behavior of crickets.” and the associated problems of warfare, deceit and self-deception, The “nine lives” in the title of this obituary does not refer to a language and scenario-building, music and the arts, humor, religion, particular fondness for cats on Alexander's part (in fact, he was partial and even science as a human endeavor (Fig. 4). to horses), or to some series of near death experiences (although he had Alexander was instrumental in founding the multi-disciplinary some of those). Rather, it refers to the many different interests that Human Behavior and Evolution Program at the University of Michigan, Alexander pursued during his lifetime. Growing up, he worked as a alumni of which founded the Human Behavior and Evolution Society. farmer, rancher and horse trainer, activities he continued throughout Long before “sociobiology” or “evolutionary psychology” were widely his life. He was also a talented poet, songwriter, musician, and wood- discussed, Alexander had already written many profoundly influential ⁎ Corresponding author. E-mail address: [email protected] (K. Summers). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.12.002 Received 5 December 2018; Accepted 11 December 2018 1090-5138/ © 2018 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. K. Summers et al. Evolution and Human Behavior 40 (2019) 133–139 Fig. 1. Richard D. Alexander (photo by Mark O'Brien). Fig. 4. A meeting in 1987 of several of the thinkers who were responsible for the modern unification of evolutionary and behavioral science. Left to right, in the back: David Buss, George C. Williams, Martin Daly, and Mildred Dickemann. In the front, William D. Hamilton, Napoleon Chagnon, and Richard D. Alexander. Three other people who profoundly influenced Alexander's thinking are Robert Trivers, William Irons, and Margo Wilson. Photo from richarddalexander.com maintained by David Lahti. works on evolution and social behavior. His review article entitled The Evolution of Social Behavior, published in 1974 in the Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, has been cited over 4000 times. He continued to publish significant contributions on the evolution of social behavior on a regular basis throughout the rest of his career. A volume in his honor celebrating and summarizing many of these contributions was published in Summers and Crespi, 2013 (Human Social Evolution: The Foundational Works of Richard D. Alexander (K. Summers and B. Crespi, eds), 2013, Oxford University Press). Alexander's (1974) review of the evolution of social behavior pre- Fig. 2. Richard Alexander and friend in 2015 (photo by David Lahti). sented a comprehensive theoretical framework for understanding eu- sociality and social systems in insects, birds, and mammals. The 58 page synthetic review laid out hypotheses that a generation of biologists and anthropologists have spent their lives testing. When asked why it was so frequently cited, Alexander offered an uncharacteristically self-depre- cating answer: ‘because I got so many things wrong and every hot-shot, young scientist wants to make his reputation by showing me up’. One criticism of the review led Alexander to one of his best known discoveries, eusociality – societies with sterile workers – in the naked mole-rat: why had eusociality evolved many times among the hyme- noptera and only once in all other insects? William Hamilton had re- cently suggested that kin selection might help answer the question since hymenoptera (ants, bees, wasps) are haplodiploid which means sisters are more closely related to each other than they are to brothers or their own offspring, something that favors sisters becoming workers and rearing more sisters instead of their own young. It turned out that the factors driving the evolution of sterile workers are more complicated than simple genetic relatedness, but the insight of this finding is still important. In his 1974 review, Alexander argued that parental care (not asymmetric relatedness) was the essential ancestral character in the evolution of insect eusociality. Unlike Hamilton's haplodiploid hy- pothesis, Alexander's included the Isoptera (termites), a diploid group that evolved from ancestral wood roaches with parental care. One cri- ticism of Alexander's parental care hypothesis was that there are many Fig. 3. Cricket by R.D. Alexander. other taxa with parental care, particularly within vertebrates, where eusociality has failed to emerge. Alexander took this criticism seriously and explored it by posing the 134 K. Summers et al. Evolution and Human Behavior 40 (2019) 133–139 inaugurating the modern evolutionary study of human behavior. Alexander developed a new, synthetic approach to understanding human psychology, rooted firmly in the fresh understanding of adap- tation that had emerged in the mid-1960s thanks especially to inclusive fitness theory as developed by William D. Hamilton and subsequently applied and enriched by George C. Williams, Robert Trivers and John Maynard Smith. With such a foundation, along with a thorough study of ethnographic works, the behavioral sciences, and the lifestyles of ani- mals, Alexander drew together
Recommended publications
  • THE DESCENT of MADNESS: Evolutionary Origins of Psychosis
    THE DESCENT OF MADNESS Drawing on evidence from across the behavioural and natural sciences, this book advances a radical new hypothesis: that madness exists as a costly consequence of the evolution of a sophisticated social brain in Homo sapiens. Having explained the rationale for an evolutionary approach to psych- osis, the author makes a case for psychotic illness in our living ape relatives, as well as in human ancestors. He then reviews existing evolutionary theor- ies of psychosis, before introducing his own thesis: that the same genes causing madness are responsible for the evolution of our highly social brain. Jonathan Burns’ novel Darwinian analysis of the importance of psychosis for human survival provides some meaning for this form of suffering. It also spurs us on to a renewed commitment to changing our societies in a way that allows the mentally ill the opportunity of living. The Descent of Madness will be of interest to those in the fields of psychiatry, psychology, sociology and anthropology, and is also accessible to the general reader. Jonathan Burns is chief specialist psychiatrist at the Nelson Mandela School of Medicine. His main areas of research include psychotic illnesses, human brain evolution and evolutionary origins of psychosis. THE DESCENT OF MADNESS Evolutionary Origins of Psychosis and the Social Brain Jonathan Burns First published 2007 by Routledge 27 Church Road, Hove, East Sussex BN3 2FA Simultaneously published in the USA and Canada by Routledge 270 Madison Ave, New York, NY 10016 Routledge is an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business © 2007 Jonathan Burns This edition published in the Taylor & Francis e-Library, 2007.
    [Show full text]
  • Libertarian Marxism Mao-Spontex Open Marxism Popular Assembly Sovereign Citizen Movement Spontaneism Sui Iuris
    Autonomist Marxist Theory and Practice in the Current Crisis Brian Marks1 University of Arizona School of Geography and Development [email protected] Abstract Autonomist Marxism is a political tendency premised on the autonomy of the proletariat. Working class autonomy is manifested in the self-activity of the working class independent of formal organizations and representations, the multiplicity of forms that struggles take, and the role of class composition in shaping the overall balance of power in capitalist societies, not least in the relationship of class struggles to the character of capitalist crises. Class composition analysis is applied here to narrate the recent history of capitalism leading up to the current crisis, giving particular attention to China and the United States. A global wave of struggles in the mid-2000s was constituitive of the kinds of working class responses to the crisis that unfolded in 2008-10. The circulation of those struggles and resultant trends of recomposition and/or decomposition are argued to be important factors in the balance of political forces across the varied geography of the present crisis. The whirlwind of crises and the autonomist perspective The whirlwind of crises (Marks, 2010) that swept the world in 2008, financial panic upon food crisis upon energy shock upon inflationary spiral, receded temporarily only to surge forward again, leaving us in a turbulent world, full of possibility and peril. Is this the end of Neoliberalism or its retrenchment? A new 1 Published under the Creative Commons licence: Attribution-Noncommercial-No Derivative Works Autonomist Marxist Theory and Practice in the Current Crisis 468 New Deal or a new Great Depression? The end of American hegemony or the rise of an “imperialism with Chinese characteristics?” Or all of those at once? This paper brings the political tendency known as autonomist Marxism (H.
    [Show full text]
  • Raising-Darwins-Consciousness.Pdf
    RAISING DARWIN'S CONSCIOUSNESS Female Sexuality and the Prehominid Origins of Patriarchy Sarah Blaffer Hrdy University of California, Davis Sociobiologists and feminists agree that men in patriarchal social systems seek to control females, but sociobiologists go further, using Darwin's theory of sexual selection and Trivers's ideas on parental investment to explain why males should attempt to control female sexuality. From this perspective, the stage for the development under some conditions of patriarchal social systems was set over the course of primate evolution. Sexual selection encompasses both competition between males and female choice. But in applying this theory to our "lower origins" (pre- hominid ancestors), Darwin assumed that choices were made by essen- tially "coy" females. I argue here that female solicitation of multiple males (either simultaneously or sequentially, depending on the breeding system) characterized prehominid females; this prehominid legacy of cy- clical sexual assertiveness, itself possibly a female counter-strategy to male efforts to control the timing of female reproduction, generated fur- ther male counter-strategies. This dialectic had important implications for emerging hominid mating systems, human evolution, and the devel- opment of patriarchal arrangements in some human societies. For homi- nid males who will invest in offspring, there would be powerful selection for emotions, behaviors, and customs that ensure them certainty of pater- nity. The sexual modesty that so struck Darwin can be explained as a recent evolved or learned (perhaps both) adaptation in women to avoid penalties imposed by patrilines on daughters and mates who failed to conform to the patriline's prevailing norms for their sex.
    [Show full text]
  • Iciumv LEADERS in ANIMAL BEHAVIOR the Second Generation
    UO1BJ3U3D iciumv LEADERS IN ANIMAL BEHAVIOR The Second Generation Edited by Lee C. Drickamer Northern Arizona University Donald A. Dewsbury University of Florida CAMBRIDGE UNIVERSITY PRESS 13 Myths, monkeys, and motherhood: a compromising life SARAH BLAFFER HRDY Definition of an Anthropologist: "(Someone) who studies human nature in all its diversity." Carmelo Lison-Tolosana (1966) Maternal effects (1946-64) From a young age, I was interested in why humans do what they do. With little exposure to science, certainly no inkling that there might be people in the world who studied other animals in order to better understand our species, I decided to become a novelist. Born in Leaders in Animal Behavior: The Setond Generation, ed. L. C. Drickamcr & D. A. Dewsbury. Published by Cambridge University Press. CO Cambridge University Press 2010. 344 Sarah Bluffer Hrdy Texas in 1946, right at the start of the postwar baby boom, I was the third of five children - Speedway. Prevailin four daughters and finally the long-awaited son. My father's father, R. L. Blaffer, had come segregation, and pi to Texas from Hamburg via New Orleans in 1901 at the time oil was discovered at interested in the e1 Spindletop. He recognized that fortunes would be made in the oil business. He married inheritance, female Sarah Campbell from Lampasas, whose father was in that business. I was named for her, the women's moven Sarah Campbell Blaffer II. My mother's father's ancestors, the Hardins, French Huguenots Reared by a suo from Tennessee, arrived earlier, in 1825, before Texas was even a state.
    [Show full text]
  • Cinderella Effect Facts
    The “Cinderella effect”: Elevated mistreatment of stepchildren in comparison to those living with genetic parents. Martin Daly & Margo Wilson Department of Psychology, Neuroscience & Behaviour McMaster University Hamilton, Ontario, Canada L8S 4K1 <[email protected]> <[email protected]> Theory Parents commit a huge amount of time, attention and material resources to the care of their children, as well as incurring life-threatening risks to defend them and bodily depletion to nourish them. Why are parents motivated to invest so heavily in their children? From an evolutionary perspective, the answer is surely that natural selection has favoured intensive parental care in our lineage. Those ancestral genotypes and phenotypes that best succeeded in raising children to become reproducing adults were the ones that persisted and proliferated. If the psychological underpinnings of parental care have indeed evolved by natural selection, we may furthermore anticipate that parental feeling and action will not typically be elicited by just any random conspecific juvenile. Instead, care-providing animals may be expected to direct their care selectively towards young who are (a) their own genetic offspring rather than those of their reproductive rivals, and (b) able to convert parental investment into increased prospects for survival and reproduction. This is the kernel of the theory of discriminative parental solicitude, which (notwithstanding some interesting twists and caveats) has been abundantly verified in a broad range of care-giving species
    [Show full text]
  • Steps to an Evolutionary Ecology of Mind and Morality
    This page intentionally left blank Death, Hope and Sex Steps to an Evolutionary Ecology of Mind and Morality By showing how and why human nature is what is is, evolutionary theory can help us see better what we need to do to improve the human condition. Following evolutionary theory to its logical conclusion, Death, Hope, and Sex uses life history theory and attachment theory to construct a model of human nature in which critical features are understood in terms of the development of alternative reproductive strategies contingent on environmental risk and uncertainty. James Chisholm examines the implication of this model for perspectives on concerns associated with human reproduction, including teen pregnancy, and young male violence. He thus develops new approaches for thorny issues such as the nature–nurture and mind–body dichotomies. Bridging the gap between the social and biological sciences, this far-reaching volume will be a source of inspiration, debate, and discussion for all those interested in the evolution of human nature and the potential for an evolutionary humanism. james s. chisholm is Associate Professor in the Department of Anatomy and Human Biology at the University of Western Australia in Perth, Australia. His previous publications include Navajo Infancy: An Ethological Study of Child Development (1983). Death, Hope and Sex Steps to an Evolutionary Ecology of Mind and Morality James S. Chisholm The Pitt Building, Trumpington Street, Cambridge, United Kingdom The Edinburgh Building, Cambridge CB2 2RU, UK 40 West 20th Street, New York, NY 10011-4211, USA 477 Williamstown Road, Port Melbourne, VIC 3207, Australia Ruiz de Alarcón 13, 28014 Madrid, Spain Dock House, The Waterfront, Cape Town 8001, South Africa http://www.cambridge.org © James S.
    [Show full text]
  • The Theoretical Foundations of Evolutionary Psychology
    3GC01 06/09/2015 12:40:42 Page 3 Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (2015). The theoretical foundations of evolutionary psychology. In Buss, D. M. (Ed.), The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology, Second edition. Volume 1: Foundations. (pp. 3-87). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. CHAPTER 1 The Theoretical Foundations of Evolutionary Psychology JOHN TOOBY and LEDA COSMIDES THE EMERGENCE OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY: WHAT IS AT STAKE? HE THEORY OF evolution by natural selection has revolutionary implications for understanding the design of the human mind and brain, as Darwin himself was Tthe first to recognize (Darwin, 1859). Indeed, a principled understanding of the network of causation that built the functional architecture of the human species offers the possibility of transforming the study of humanity into a natural science capable of precision and rapid progress. Yet, more than a century and a half after The Origin of Species was published, many of the psychological, social, and behavioral sciences continue to be grounded on assumptions that evolutionarily informed researchers know to be false; the rest have only in the past few decades set to work on the radical reformulations of their disciplines necessary to make them consistent with findings in the evolutionary sciences, information theory, computer science, physics, the neuro- sciences, molecular and cellular biology, genetics, behavioral ecology, hunter-gatherer studies, biological anthropology, primatology, and so on (Pinker, 1997, 2002; Tooby & Cosmides, 1992). Evolutionary psychology is the long-forestalled scientific attempt to assemble out of the disjointed, fragmentary, and mutually contradictory human disciplines a single, logically integrated research framework for the psychological, social, and behavioral sciences—a framework that not only incorporates the evolu- tionary sciences and information theory on a full and equal basis, but that systemati- cally works out all the revisions in existing belief and research practice that such a synthesis requires (Tooby & Cosmides, 1992).
    [Show full text]
  • The Selfish Gene by Richard Dawkins Is Another
    BOOKS & ARTS COMMENT ooks about science tend to fall into two categories: those that explain it to lay people in the hope of cultivat- Bing a wide readership, and those that try to persuade fellow scientists to support a new theory, usually with equations. Books that achieve both — changing science and reach- ing the public — are rare. Charles Darwin’s On the Origin of Species (1859) was one. The Selfish Gene by Richard Dawkins is another. From the moment of its publication 40 years ago, it has been a sparkling best-seller and a TERRY SMITH/THE LIFE IMAGES COLLECTION/GETTY SMITH/THE LIFE IMAGES TERRY scientific game-changer. The gene-centred view of evolution that Dawkins championed and crystallized is now central both to evolutionary theoriz- ing and to lay commentaries on natural history such as wildlife documentaries. A bird or a bee risks its life and health to bring its offspring into the world not to help itself, and certainly not to help its species — the prevailing, lazy thinking of the 1960s, even among luminaries of evolution such as Julian Huxley and Konrad Lorenz — but (uncon- sciously) so that its genes go on. Genes that cause birds and bees to breed survive at the expense of other genes. No other explana- tion makes sense, although some insist that there are other ways to tell the story (see K. Laland et al. Nature 514, 161–164; 2014). What stood out was Dawkins’s radical insistence that the digital information in a gene is effectively immortal and must be the primary unit of selection.
    [Show full text]
  • Academic Freedom and Controversial Speech About Campus Governance — Rogers Brubaker
    Academic Freedom and Controversial Speech about Campus Governance — Rogers Brubaker — Rogers Brubaker is Professor of Sociology, University of California, Los Angeles and UCLA Foundation Chair; Visiting Professor, CEU. Classical definitions of academic freedom focused on freedom of re- search and teaching. In the American context, the right of professors to speak freely as citizens outside the university has also been emphasized. But many recent controversies over academic freedom in the US—and I limit my comments to the US—have turned on speech inside the univer- sity yet outside the traditional domains of research and teaching. Research and teaching continue of course to be central to the defense of academic freedom in the face of external pressures, notably from private and public funders, government regulators, and the populist right. But I have been asked to address internal threats to academic freedom. And while some internal controversies have focused on the freedom of research and teaching, many have focused on other issues. The most widely discussed of these controversies have concerned invita- tions to controversial outside speakers. Public attention has focused on efforts by the campus left to “disinvite” or “de-platform” speakers such as Charles Murray at Middlebury and Milo Yiannopoulos and Ann Coulter 93 i6 AF - 00 Book.indb 93 2017.11.22. 9:01 at Berkeley. These widely publicized incidents have already generat- ed a substantial backlash: several conservative state legislatures have passed campus speech bills.1 The campus right has also sought to pre- vent or disrupt events involving controversial (especially pro-Palestinian) outside speakers. But there is another kind of internal academic freedom controversy that I would like to highlight.
    [Show full text]
  • Ideological Diversity Course UNDERGRAD
    Free Speech, Viewpoint Diversity, and Higher Education or Bigots and Snowflakes: Living in a World Where Everyone Else Is Wrong COURSE DESCRIPTION In this course, we will address questions surrounding viewpoint diversity. There are two foundational premises for the course. The first is that there is a lack of diversity along this dimension, particularly within the domain of higher education; recent survey data confirm this trend (goo.gl/ddnGEG). The second assertion is that this lack of diversity damages the mission of higher education (we will talk more about this in the first class). Inherent in the definition of viewpoint diversity is the principle that there is a spectrum of perspectives ranging, politically speaking, from staunchly conservative on one end to radically progressive on the other – and that the voicing of perspectives falling along that spectrum should be without social penalty. *Because the tilt in our current context on campus leans left and the material in the course will often refer to the absence and dismissal of opinions that deviate from an orthodoxy, omitted viewpoints often — though not without exception — coincide with non-left views or leftist views that simply ask uncomfortable questions. Using our two assertions as a starting point, we examine issues that students may consider relevant for their academic studies in the social sciences and on campus more broadly. The overarching goal of the course is to introduce students to concepts that will teach them the importance of examining multiple perspectives dispassionately and to think critically in all aspects of their education. Invariably, it is the case that, to cover this material, we will delve into topics and listen to speakers that some will find distasteful or even abhorrent, particularly — although not exclusively — in the fourth section (described below).
    [Show full text]
  • Daniel C. Dennett
    KINDS OF MINDS Toward an Understanding of Consciousness DANIEL C. DENNETT A Member of the Perseus Books Group -iii- The Science Masters Series is a global publishing venture consisting of original science books written by leading scientists and published by a worldwide team of twenty-six publishers assembled by John Brockman. The series was conceived by Anthony Cheetham of Orion Publishers and John Brockman of Brockman Inc., a New York literary agency, and developed in coordination with BasicBooks. The Science Masters name and marks are owned by and licensed to the publisher by Brockman Inc. Copyright © 1996 by Daniel Dennett. Published by BasicBooks A Member of the Perseus Books Group All rights reserved. Printed in the United States of America. No part of this book may be used or reproduced in any manner whatsoever without written permission except in the case of brief quotations embodied in critical articles and reviews. For information address Basic Books, 10 East 53rd Street, New York, NY 10022-5299. Designed by Joan Greenfield ISBN 0-465-07351-4 (pbk.) 99 00 01 ❖/RRD 10 9 8 7 6 5 -iv- CONTENTS Preface vii 1 What Kinds of Minds Are There? 1 Knowing Your Own Mind 1 We Mind-Havers, We Minders 3 Words and Minds 8 The Problem of Incommunicative Minds 12 2 Intentionality: The Intentional Systems Approach 19 Simple Beginnings: The Birth of Agency 19 Adopting the Intentional Stance 27 The Misguided Goal of Propositional Precision 41 Original and Derived Intentionality 50 3 The Body and Its Minds 57 From Sensitivity to Sentience? 57 The
    [Show full text]
  • States in Mind Anthony C. Lopez, Rose Mcdermott, and Michael
    States in Mind States in Mind Anthony C. Lopez, Rose McDermott, and Evolution, Coalitional Psychology, and Michael Bang International Politics Petersen One of the most com- monly studied puzzles in international politics is the recurrence of coalitional competition and aggression between political groups such as states. Indeed, this pattern constitutes an enduring and central feature of all politics. Yet de- spite the tragic endurance of this leitmotif throughout history, its manifestation varies through time and space. Some wars are fought for honor or revenge, whereas others are ignited for mere opportunism or as a consequence of vari- ous misperceptions, whatever their source. We argue that evolutionary theory provides a theoretical framework that can explain both the stubborn endur- ance and dynamic diversity of coalitional behavior. Debate on the relevance of “human nature” and biological factors for explaining political behavior is not new.1 Yet the comprehensive value of evo- lutionary theory for explaining important aspects of international politics has not been adequately explicated. As we discuss below, this has in part been a consequence of general skepticism about the validity and scope of evolution- ary theory for explaining political behavior. We argue, however, that evolu- tionary psychology can generate falsiªable ex ante predictions that are of central interest to the study of international politics, and we offer several hy- potheses derived from this model to illustrate the depth of this approach. Evo- lutionary psychologists have already generated a large body of work that suggests that the human brain contains webs of psychological mechanisms, or adaptations, each designed to operate in domains relevant to modern politics, and which emerged as a product of natural selection.
    [Show full text]