Open Pandora's Box: Curiosity and Imagination in the Classroom
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Child Development Institute occasional paper series Open Pandora’s Box: Curiosity and Imagination in the Classroom Susan Engel Open Pandora’s Box: Curiosity and Imagination in the Classroom by Susan Engel The Thomas H. Wright Lecture Child Development Institute Sarah Lawrence College Summer 2006 Table of Contents I. Theories Are Lenses ............................................................................................... 1 II. Why Existing Models Are Off-Base ....................................................................... 3 III. A Different View of Children ................................................................................. 3 IV. A Focus on Curiosity.............................................................................................. 6 V. Peeking into Pandora’s Box.................................................................................... 8 References ......................................................................................................................... 10 Theories Are Lenses When you praise a child for being kind to a friend, somewhere within your thinking is the We all look at children through a lens. When idea that children crave social approval, and you watch your daughter, your student, or your such approval can shape the way they interact experimental subject, what you see is shaped by with others. When a teacher leaves a child to your theory of development. Each theory tends play with a bunch of blocks, she’s assuming that to highlight some aspects of a child’s behavior cognitive development occurs spontaneously and mask others. Your theory also guides your when children have a chance to interact with the interpretation of what children do and say. A physical world. A teacher who makes sure her theory can expand and deepen our understand- students see her looking up answers to questions ing of children, but it can also distort and she doesn’t know may believe that children limit our view. imitate the behavior of adults they feel close to. Let me begin by describing a recent conver- When you suggest that four-year-olds should sit sation I had with a friend. The friend was com- quietly in circle time because they will have to plimenting me on my three sons—how well sit quietly at their desks when they are older, you they had “turned out.” She said this with some believe that development is a process of acquir- surprise and admitted that when they were little ing and strengthening habits and behaviors. she thought they were totally wild and out of But it’s not just parents and teachers who control: “You had no rules at all.” I disagreed, carry around implicit models of child develop- suggesting that my rules may not have been ment. Psychologists do, too. The models psy- about things like swearing, or keeping their chologists create have a huge impact not only on clothes on, or saying “thank you” to a grown-up, what they learn about children in their research, but had more to do with working hard at things but on what the rest of us think—particularly and being kind. She thought about that for a about what happens in schools. minute and then said, “But what? Did you Let me describe two powerful metaphors that punish them when they weren’t kind? Did they have shaped the way people have thought about have a consequence when they didn’t throw young children over the past 75 years or so. themselves into things?” Honestly, I was baffled. Then I realized that her implicit model of devel- opment was showing through her questions. The Wild Child Her model of development is clearly based on The earlier of these two metaphors is that of the behaviorism—the traits that will emerge over Wild Child. In his classic book, Emile: Or, On time in a child are the ones that are regularly Education, Jean-Jacques Rousseau put forth the rewarded, while undesirable behaviors that are idea that children are innocent and uncon- punished will disappear. strained by adult conventions (1979). Using a My friend is not odd or unique for thinking darker lens, Anna Freud saw the young child as a about everyday experiences through a theoretical mass of unruly emotions with no control, gov- lens. We all have implicit models of child devel- erned by selfish drives, claiming that if you put a opment, though we rarely articulate what these toddler on any street corner in Cambridge, by are, and even more rarely know how we acquired the time they reached the center of the city they them. I often drive from my home in the Berkshires would have committed every crime known to in Massachusetts to New York City. As I make mankind. my way south, I pass two family day care centers. Innocent or anarchic, the Wild Child view The first, a large old farm house with a fenced-in implies that it is up to adults to constrain young playground outside, has a sign that reads, “My children, train them, and rein them in. Little Angels Day Care.” Right down the road from it is another one, also an old family house. The Little Scientist This one is named “Little Professors.” The two day care directors surely have different models of The second metaphor is that of the Little Scientist, childhood. What people say about and to children a view which can be traced to the Swiss scientist, often offers clues about their implicit model of Jean Piaget (1955). Trained first in biology (he child development. published his first scientific paper on mollusks when he was 13 years old), he wanted to under- stand how a gurgling, kicking, crying baby 1 could become a rational scientist capable of instead of two, babies gaze longer, suggesting abstract thought. The careful observations he that they can detect a discrepancy between how made of his own children, and the ingenious many should be there and how many are there. experiments he conducted with other children, Such research supports our modern view that contributed to what was, at the time, a radical children are eagerly seeking to understand how new view of children, one that persists today. the real world works. We have come to see even In this view, children are eager to understand very young children as cognitively similar to the world, acting on their environment almost adults. In this view, logic about the real world is from the beginning, in their efforts to make everything, and feelings rarely affect how the sense of experience. Piaget viewed development child thinks. So, why does this hyper-rational as the process by which children form ever view of the child matter to teachers? What is at more rational and scientific understandings of stake here? the world. Much of the research done in the past 50 If you watch a child in your classroom years has led us to view the child as overly through Piaget’s eyes, you can see the child rational, ready and eager to learn what grown- testing out hypotheses about how things work. ups have to teach them, with little need for time For instance, imagine a four-year-old child faced and encouragement to explore their own imag- with a set of eight blocks. She may begin build- inative renderings of the world around them. I ing a tower. Then she might try a more sprawl- think this view has led to some serious problems ing shape, and then another one. If we look at with the way we educate and raise our children. this from Piaget’s perspective, we would predict Though Piaget would turn over in his grave, that at some point she will uncover a basic prin- the work of those who followed him has led us to ciple of the physical world—that no matter what view children as incomplete adults, moving in a the shape of the building, if she uses the same neat linear fashion toward knowing the things blocks again and again, the volume of the build- grown-ups know. ing remains constant. The child, we imagine, Take, for example, the answer one often hears from Piaget’s perspective, is sitting alone with when challenging the current practice of assign- some objects, figuring things out, much like the ing homework to kindergarten and first-grade scientist alone in her lab with test tubes. In fact, children—that they will have to do it when they a recent book considered by many to summarize get older, so they should get in the habit when the most up-to-date research on the infant’s they are young. Such an answer reveals an under- mind is titled The Scientist in the Crib (1999). lying theory that development consists of the Of course, research has come a long way acquisition of habits. But such a theory is insid- since Piaget’s first investigations. We have new ious—before you know it, teachers and parents methodologies and have collected data in a much are pleased when young children exhibit adult wider variety of contexts and communities. Iron- behavior, interpreting the behavior as a sign of ically, much of the contemporary research has rapid progress. Taken far enough (and it often is), actually led us away from Piaget’s early emphasis such a view leads adults to misinterpret develop- on the child’s eagerness to find out, and instead has mentally appropriate behavior—they worry or led us to emphasize the child’s ability to master disapprove when a three-year-old behaves in the everyday world—we imagine a goal-oriented three-year-old ways. Consider, for instance, the child whose amazing cognitive skills seem to be beginning of an article published in The Onion in place earlier and earlier. A striking example of some years ago. this is the work of Karen Wynn, who has shown that during the first year of life, babies seem to Day after day, upon arriving home from pre- show some rudimentary understanding of school, Caitlin would retreat into a bizarre addition (Wynn and Chiang, 1998).