Interpersonal Effects of Expressed Anger and Sorrow in Morally Charged Negotiation

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Interpersonal Effects of Expressed Anger and Sorrow in Morally Charged Negotiation Judgment and Decision Making, Vol. 9, No. 2, March 2014, pp. 104–113 Interpersonal effects of expressed anger and sorrow in morally charged negotiation Morteza Dehghani∗ Peter J. Carnevale † Jonathan Gratch ‡ Abstract The expression of emotion can play a significant role in strategic decision-making. In this study, we hypothesized that emotion expression alters behavior in morally charged negotiation. We investigated the impact of facial displays of discrete emotions, specifically anger and sadness, in a morally charged multi-issue negotiation task. Our results indicate that if a negotiator associated moral significance to the object of the negotiation, displays of anger resulted in reduced concession making whereas displays of sadness increased concession making. Moral significance of the issues fostered an emotional matching mechanism of sorrow, where a sorrow expression from one party elicited a sorrow expression from the other. Taken together, the results indicate that emotional expressions can affect morally charged negotiation in ways that can inhibit as well as promote cooperation. Keywords: emotion, sacred values, moral decision-making, negotiation. 1 Introduction cred domains. In this study, we investigated the impact of facial dis- Recent research into sacred values reveals a consistent pat- plays of discrete emotions, specifically sadness and anger, tern: when confronted with possible threats to moral or in a morally charged multi-issue negotiation task in which sacred concerns, people tend to react emotionally (e.g., one of the issues entailed the moral matter of saving the Ginges, Atran, Medin, & Shikaki, 2007), become uncom- life of the negotiator’s child. We suggest that interper- promising (e.g., Tetlock, 2003), and act in ways contrary sonal effects of emotion unfold differently in moral con- to traditional formalizations of rational self-interest (e.g., texts and with morally charged issues in negotiation. This Atran, 2010). This article offers some hope in this oth- idea stems from work showing that people who have a sa- erwise gloomy picture. Building on findings from both cred value tend to reject tradeoffs with other values (es- moral decision-making and the interpersonal effects of pecially with secular issues such as money) and express emotion, we show that emotion, in certain circumstances, anger when such tradeoffs arise (e.g., Tetlock, Kristel, El- may foster cooperation in value-laden conflicts. We show son, Green, & Lerner, 2000). that well-established interpersonal effects of emotion un- fold differently in moral domains. For example, displays We based our predictions on the notion that anger con- of sadness can convey weakness (Tiedens, 2001; Tiedens, notes a violation of autonomous individual rights (Rozin, Ellsworth, & Mesquita, 2000) and promote exploitation in Lowery, Imada, & Haidt, 1999), whereas sadness connotes non-moral negotiations (Van Kleef, De Dreu, & Manstead, sympathy, need, weakness and concern (Horbeg, Oveis 2006). This effect reverses in strategic decision making & Keltner, 2011). We expected that when an object is over sacred issues. Our findings have potential important perceived as a sacred (or protected) value, with intrin- implications for negotiation and conflict resolution in sa- sic moral significance (Tetlock, 2003; Baron & Spranca, 1997), angry or sad facial displays expressed by an oppo- nent will have opposing effects on the negotiator. Thus, This research was supported by AFOSR Young Investigator, Program we hypothesized that perceiving different emotional ex- AFOSR FA9550-09-1-0507, NSF IIS-1211064, and NSF SES-0836004. The software developed for the experiments reported here is free and pressions in others would influence moral cognition by available by contacting Morteza Dehghani at [email protected] shifting interpretive-frames. In other words, we suggest Copyright: © 2013. The authors license this article under the terms of that the interpretation of a moral issue can be affected by the Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License. the emotional expressions conveyed by the opposing ne- ∗Brain and Creativity Institute, University of Southern California, 3620A McClintock Avenue; Los Angeles, CA 90089, USA Email: mde- gotiator in the negotiation, in addition to the emotion ex- [email protected] perienced by the negotiator, as shown in past work. † Marshall School of Business, University of Southern California, Specifically, we hypothesize that when facing an an- 3670 Trousdale Parkway, Los Angeles, CA 90089-0808, USA ‡Institute for Creative Technologies, University of Southern Califor- gry opponent, a negotiator dealing with a sacred-value nia, 1205 Waterfront Dr., Playa Vista, CA 90094-2536, USA issue—a morally significant issue—would show rejection 104 Judgment and Decision Making, Vol. 9, No. 2, March 2014 Interpersonal effects of expressed anger and sorrow 105 of tradeoffs and concede very little, as their concerns about emotional display. For example, Van Kleef, De Dreu and their sacred values would be amplified by the anger ex- Manstead (2004) showed that negotiators conceded more pressed in the other party. That is, expressed anger would to an angry opponent than to a happy one and further pro- lessen the likelihood of concessions for sacred-value is- vided evidence that participants used expressed emotion sues. However, when interacting with an opponent who to infer the other party’s limits and adjusted their offers displays sadness, the morality of sympathy, need, weak- accordingly. This line of research argues that anger com- ness and concern become salient, and participants may municates that a party has high aspirations and that con- thus make greater concessions. For non-sacred-value par- cessions are thus required to reach an agreement. ticipants we expected to see the known pattern of in- Although less studied than other emotions, sadness is creased concession due to perceived anger, consistent with another emotion that naturally arises in intergroup con- findings of Van Kleef and others in non-moral domains flicts. Sadness can communicate weakness or low status (e.g., Van Kleef, et al., 2004; Sinaceur & Tiedens, 2006). (Tiedens, 2001; Tiedens, Ellsworth & Mesquita, 2000). Our hypothesize is built on Horberg, Oveis and Kelt- From a functional perspective, sadness can serve as an ap- ner’s (2011) theory that different moral concerns get pri- peal for support (Tomkins, 1963) that aims to elicit empa- oritized based on the distinct emotions that are experi- thy and helping behavior (Yee & Greenberg, 1998). How- enced. This theory, rooted in social-functional framework ever, there is no clear consensus on the impact of sadness of emotion (Keltner & Haidt, 1999; Frijda & Masquita, on negotiations. As a signal of weakness, one might ex- 1994), argues that our perception of the permissibility of pect sadness to provoke tough stances by the other party to actions in moral situations is affected by the emotions ex- strengthen their negotiation position (Tiedens, 2001; Van perienced. For example, disgust has been linked to viola- Kleef, De Dreu & Manstead, 2006). On the other hand, as tions of purity-sanctity (Rozin, et al., 1999); experimen- an elicitor of support and sympathy, sadness might trigger tally predisposing individuals to disgust increases their feelings of compassion and greater concessions. Few stud- tendency to focus on purity related issues (e.g., sexual- ies have examined this issue directly, although one line of ity) as opposed to other moral concerns such as justice studies by Van Kleef and colleagues (2006) suggests that (e.g., Tapias, Glaser, Keltner, Vasquez & Wickens, 2007). displays of disappointment (which in some contexts re- In a related vein, DeSteno, Dasgupta, Bartlett, and Cajdric lates to sadness) will tend to elicit greater concessions in (2004) report that the display of anger can evoke automatic non-sacred domains. bias directed toward out-groups. In contrast to the large volume of research into both However, the research on emotion-related moral ap- moral-decision making and the interpersonal function of praisals has been for the most part limited to the intrap- emotions in negotiations (mainly focusing on issue that ersonal effects of emotion in decision-making. In con- may be of interest to people but have no sentimental or trast, research on negotiation and conflict resolution has moral significance to them), almost no studies exist in the considered both the intrapersonal as well as the interper- intersection of these two fields. Yet several new findings sonal effects of emotion (e.g., Carnevale, 2008; Carnevale hint that studying this intersection may provide new in- & Isen, 1985; O’Quin & Aronoff, 1981). Carnevale and sights into the mechanisms underlying emotion’s impact Isen (1985) found that positive mood had an impact on on social behavior. For example, Harinck and Van Kleef negotiation, that is, it increased problem solving and co- (2012) found that an expression of anger can have opposite operation, but not when a physical barrier separated the effects on reactions to an authority who denied a request negotiators so that they could hear but not see one another, depending on the value-context of the request.1 Before thus preventing them from seeing one another’s facial ex- 1 pressions. This suggests that intrapersonal emotion may Harinck and Van Kleef (2012) found that the expression of anger had opposite effects on reactions to an authority who denied a request de- influence interpersonal emotion via facial expression or pending on
Recommended publications
  • Your Sadness, My Sorrow: Negative Emotion Concordance Within Married Couples in China 1. Introduction
    Your Sadness, My Sorrow: Negative Emotion Concordance within Married Couples in China Wanru Xiong Office of Population Research Princeton University Abstract: This paper finds significant evidence for negative emotion concordance within married couples age 45 and above in China using CHARLS data. To understand the mechanism of such concordance and test the hypothesis of emotional contagion effect, I applied instrument variable method, structure equation model and fixed effect model in this dyadic relation context. The results confirm the effect of spouse’s negative emotion on the respondent’s and support the emotional contagion effect hypothesis. In general, wives tend to be more sensitive to the emotional condition of their husband. Keywords: dyadic relation; negative emotion; concordance; emotional contagion effect 1. Introduction “I will laugh with you and cry with you ... in sickness and in health, till death do us apart.” Couples mean it when they take their wedding vows. There is evidence of health concordance within married couples, which means couples’ health conditions, both physical and mental, tend to converge after marriage (Meyler, Stimpson and Peek, 2007). This phenomenon of health concordance within couples is drawing increasing research interests. Published findings of physical health concordance within couples include concordance in blood pressure (Beresford, 1976; Speers et al., 1986; Peek and Markides, 2003; Stimpson and Peek, 2005), chronic conditions (Cheraskin et al., 1968; Bookwala and Schulz, 1996; Ubeda, 1998; Stimpson and Peek, 2005) and obesity (Inoue et al., 1996; Knuiman et al., 1996). Among the growing number of papers on health concordance within couples, studies on depressive symptoms and distress are the most prevalent (Meyler, Stimpson and Peek, 2007).
    [Show full text]
  • About Emotions There Are 8 Primary Emotions. You Are Born with These
    About Emotions There are 8 primary emotions. You are born with these emotions wired into your brain. That wiring causes your body to react in certain ways and for you to have certain urges when the emotion arises. Here is a list of primary emotions: Eight Primary Emotions Anger: fury, outrage, wrath, irritability, hostility, resentment and violence. Sadness: grief, sorrow, gloom, melancholy, despair, loneliness, and depression. Fear: anxiety, apprehension, nervousness, dread, fright, and panic. Joy: enjoyment, happiness, relief, bliss, delight, pride, thrill, and ecstasy. Interest: acceptance, friendliness, trust, kindness, affection, love, and devotion. Surprise: shock, astonishment, amazement, astound, and wonder. Disgust: contempt, disdain, scorn, aversion, distaste, and revulsion. Shame: guilt, embarrassment, chagrin, remorse, regret, and contrition. All other emotions are made up by combining these basic 8 emotions. Sometimes we have secondary emotions, an emotional reaction to an emotion. We learn these. Some examples of these are: o Feeling shame when you get angry. o Feeling angry when you have a shame response (e.g., hurt feelings). o Feeling fear when you get angry (maybe you’ve been punished for anger). There are many more. These are NOT wired into our bodies and brains, but are learned from our families, our culture, and others. When you have a secondary emotion, the key is to figure out what the primary emotion, the feeling at the root of your reaction is, so that you can take an action that is most helpful. .
    [Show full text]
  • The Theory of Moral Sentiments
    The Theory of Moral Sentiments Adam Smith Sixth Edition (1790) pΜεταLibriq x y c 2005 Sálvio Marcelo Soares (apply only to edition, not to text) 1st Edition Version a A . Esta obra está disponível para uso privado e individual. Não pode ser vendida nem mantida em sistema de banco de dados, em qualquer forma ou meio, sem prévia autorização escrita do detentor do copyright. Apenas este e as pessoas por ele autorizadas por escrito têm direito de reproduzir esta obra ou transmití-la eletronicamente ou por qualquer outro meio. Published by ΜεταLibri [email protected] Obra editada e publicada no Brasil. São Paulo, May 15, 2006. Contents A PART I Of the P of A S I Of the S of P . p. 4 C.I Of S . 4 C. II Of the Pleasure of mutual Sympathy. 9 C. III Of the manner in which we judge of the propriety or impropriety of the affections of other men, by their concord or dissonance with our own. 11 C. IV The same subject continued . 14 C.V Of the amiable and respectable virtues . 18 S II Of the Degrees of the different Passions which are consistent with Propriety . 22 I. 22 C.I Of the Passions which take their origin from the body . 22 C. II Of those Passions which take their origin from a particular turn or habit of the Imagination. 26 C. III Of the unsocial Passions . 29 C. IV Of the social Passions . 33 C.V Of the selfish Passions. 35 S III Of the Effects of Prosperity and Adversity upon the Judgment of Mankind with regard to the Propriety of Action; and why it is more easy to obtain their Approbation in the one state than in the other .
    [Show full text]
  • Emotions.Pdf
    April 2011 Forthcoming in R. Crisp ed, The Oxford Handbook to the History of Ethics Emotion and the Emotions Susan Sauvé Meyer Adrienne M. Martin A prominent theme in twentieth and early twenty-first century moral philosophy is that a full and accurate picture of the ethical life must include an important role for the emotions. The neglect of the emotions has been a major point of criticism raised against the dominant consequentialist, Kantian, and contractualist theories by virtue ethicists such as G.E.M. Anscombe, Alisdair MacIntyre, Martha Nussbaum, and Michael Stocker. Bernard Williams and Susan Wolf also develop a similar line of criticism as part of their arguments against the supremacy or priority of moral values as conceived by utilitarianism and other “impartialist” theories. There are a number of reasons why it might be a mistake for moral philosophy to neglect the emotions. To name just three: 1. It seems obvious that emotions have an important influence on motivation, and that proper cultivation of the emotions is helpful, perhaps essential, to our ability to lead ethical lives. 2. It is also arguable that emotions are objects of moral evaluation, not only because of their influence on action, but in themselves. In other words, it is a plausible thesis that an ethical life involves feeling certain ways in certain circumstances and acting from certain feelings in certain circumstances. 3. Finally, a more contentious thesis, but certainly worth considering, is that some emotions are forms of ethical perception, judgment, or even knowledge. The bulk of this chapter surveys the Ancient ethical tradition that inspires the virtue ethicist’s critique, revealing versions of each of these three theses in one guise or another.
    [Show full text]
  • Chronic Sorrow – from Grieving to Gratitude
    Chronic sorrow – from grieving to gratitude Dr Kirsty Ross Senior Clinical Psychologist and Senior Lecturer Massey University, Palmerston North [email protected] Outline What do we know about long term health conditions Patients Siblings Families Parents Key Definitions Loss Grief Depression Chronic Sorrow Chronic Sorrow Triggers Coping Resilience The impact of the LTC Patient Key variables age of diagnosis developmental stage With growing understanding and comprehension, complex emotions emerge, without necessarily the skills to manage them Identity formation affected by a LTC Sense of mortality and different perspective on life (maturity) Social isolation Physical changes and adjustments Autonomy and independence can be thwarted or regression can occur Siblings May exhibit social and academic problems Research suggests possible difficulties with poor self-concept, depression, grief, anxiety and loneliness Feeling the need to ‘live a big life’ (survivor guilt) Knowledge increases understanding and empathy Increased independence may be hastened Feeling unable to talk about own problems and issues – not important enough when family dealing with major illness Social support important Outcomes overall suggest positive adjustment – increased empathy, compassion, maturity Families Marital satisfaction and cohesion: different coping styles between genders Changes in roles and responsibilities Division of time; two sub-units can emerge Impact on parental relationship and sibling relationships Family functioning
    [Show full text]
  • Hurricane Season and Silence Orkansæsonen Og Stilheden by Anne-Cathrine Riebnitzsky
    Hurricane Season and Silence Orkansæsonen og stilheden by Anne-Cathrine Riebnitzsky When Beate wakes up in the morning, she turns towards the empty space in bed beside her, slowly remembering that Thomas is really dead. Her sense of loss floods back, hitting her so hard that she’s almost pinned to the mattress. She pulls herself together before she goes into the office and focuses on her job as a defence lawyer. Her close friend Monica is a rather unusual priest, outspoken and with a no-nonsense approach to the parishioners who seek her advice. Monica travels to Guatemala, where she meets a mafioso, a Guatemalan teenage girl who grew up in a children’s home, and a young pot dealer whom Beate also encounters in her capacity as a lawyer. The book follows the two women as they weave in and out of people’s lives, grappling with issues of faith, justice, vigilantism, sorrow, infatuation, forgiveness of murder and – greatest of all – love. Anne-Cathrine Riebnitzsky was born in 1974 and graduated from Copenhagen’s Writer’s School in 1998. Having completed her Foreign Language Officer training in Moscow in 2003, she worked at, the Danish Embassy in Russia, before going to Afghanistan in 2007, first as a soldier and then as a Foreign Ministry advisor. In 2010 she was awarded the Anders Lassen Award for “significant military and civilian achievements during SPECIFICATIONS Pages: 346 deployment ”. Publication Date: 06. januar 2016 Rights sold to Her first book was The Women’s War (Kvindernes krig) in 2010, which was a FRANCE / Gaïa documentary about her work with the Afghan women.
    [Show full text]
  • The Enlightenment of the Buddha
    The Enlightenment of the Buddha The Buddha’s followers later recorded the episodes associated with his attainment of enlightenment, his early teaching and the development of a community of his followers. These writings became an important part of the Buddhist scriptures that accumulated in the years after the Buddha’s death in 483 B.C.E. The following selections recount the Buddha’s enlightenment and his first sermon near the city of Benares, in northern India. Points to Consider: 1. What is the “Law of Dharma”? 2. What does the Buddha mean by the “middle path”? 3. What similarities and differences do you see between the religious ideas of Jesus and Gautama Buddha? The Buddha gave himself up to meditation. All the miseries of the world, the evils produced by evil deeds and the sufferings arising from them, passed before his mental eye, and he thought: "Surely if living creatures saw the results of all their evil deeds, they would turn away from them in disgust. But selfhood blinds them, and they cling to their obnoxious desires. "They crave pleasure for themselves and they cause pain to others; when death destroys their individuality, they find no peace; their thirst for existence abides and their selfhood reappears in new births. "Thus they find no escape from the hell of their own making. And how empty are their pleasures, how vain are their endeavors! "The world is full of evil and sorrow, because it is full of lust. Men go astray because they think that delusion is better than truth. Rather than truth they follow error, which is pleasant to look at in the beginning but in the end causes anxiety, tribulation, and misery." The Law of Dharma The Enlightened One began to expound the Dharma.
    [Show full text]
  • Me, My Self, and Emotion: Identity-Consistent Emotions and Consumption
    Me, My Self, and Emotion: Identity-Consistent Emotions and Consumption NICOLE M. VERROCHI* 2 *Nicole M. Verrochi is a doctoral candidate, Department of Marketing, The Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania. The author is indebted to Patti Williams for her insightful comments; Jonah Berger, Eric Bradlow, Americus Reed, II and Deborah Small for their feedback; Steven Coleman for his musical expertise; and the Wharton Behavioral Laboratory for data collection assistance. Correspondence concerning this paper should be addressed to Nicole M. Verrochi, 700 Jon M. Huntsman Hall, 3730 Walnut Street, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania 19104. Electronic mail may be sent to [email protected]. 3 This research examines the connections between emotion and social identity. Specifically, this project theorizes that identities are associated with discrete emotions, and that these associations give rise to emotion profiles that describe appropriate emotional experiences for individuals with that active identity. The results establish that social identities have associations to specific emotions and that these associations differ between identities. Experiencing emotions consistent with the identity’s emotion profile enhances persuasion, even for identity-unrelated products and advertisements. Further experiments investigate whether individuals engage in emotion regulation to reduce (enhance) their experience of emotions which are inconsistent (consistent) with the identity’s emotion profile. Finally, consequences for the framing and positioning of
    [Show full text]
  • PREGNANCY and INFANT LOSS RESOURCES “We Weep With
    PREGNANCY AND INFANT LOSS RESOURCES “We Weep With Those Who Weep” (Romans 12: 15) To access links in this document, go to https://www.emfgp.org/resources-for-pregnancy-and-infant-loss-8-19/ Reflections for Families and Professionals (From the Archdiocese of Dubuque, IA) ❖ Reflection for Fathers ❖ Reflection for Mothers ❖ Reflection for Couples ❖ Reflection for Young Siblings ❖ Reflection for Older Siblings ❖ Reflection for Grandparents ❖ Reflection for Medical Professionals ❖ Reflection for Lay Ministers Catholic Blessings and Rituals ❖ Blessing of Parents After Miscarriage or Stillbirth ❖ Order for the Naming and Commendation of an Infant Who Died Before Birth – Archdiocese of St. Louis ❖ Naming Ceremony of a Child Lost to Miscarriage or Stillbirth – Diocese of Fargo Catholic and Christian Resources ❖ Emmaus Ministry for Grieving Parents ➢ A Catholic ministry serving the spiritual needs of parents whose children of any age (from conception to old age), who have died by any cause, no matter how long ago. Open to grieving parents of any religion. 1 ❖ Morning Light Ministry ➢ A Catholic ministry for mothers and fathers who have experienced the death of their baby. Also welcomes bereaved parents of other Christian denominations, other faiths, and of no religious affiliation who are struggling with the very notion of faith. ❖ All Embrace ➢ For families who have lost a baby. Provides resources and education to extended family, friends, religious leaders, medical personnel, and volunteers. ❖ Catholic Miscarriage Support ➢ Practical and spiritual
    [Show full text]
  • Does Hope Mediate and Moderate the Relationship Between Happiness Aversion and Depressive Symptoms?
    Open Journal of Depression, 2020, 9, 1-16 https://www.scirp.org/journal/ojd ISSN Online: 2169-9674 ISSN Print: 2169-9658 Does Hope Mediate and Moderate the Relationship between Happiness Aversion and Depressive Symptoms? Rebecca A. Bloore1* , Paul E. Jose1, Mohsen Joshanloo2 1Victoria University of Wellington, Wellington, New Zealand 2Keimung University, Daegu, South Korea How to cite this paper: Bloore, R. A., Jose, Abstract P. E., & Joshanloo, M. (2020). Does Hope Mediate and Moderate the Relationship The literature is beginning to document how and for whom “fear of happi- between Happiness Aversion and Depres- ness” (happiness aversion, i.e. the expectation that being happy can have sive Symptoms? Open Journal of Depres- negative consequences) is predictive of psychological outcomes. We sought to sion, 9, 1-16. https://doi.org/10.4236/ojd.2020.91001 determine whether hope, an important protective factor against depressive symptoms, might mediate and moderate the relationship between happiness Received: December 23, 2019 aversion and depression. In a dataset of 588 undergraduate psychology stu- Accepted: January 18, 2020 Published: January 21, 2020 dents, evidence was found that hope functioned as a mediator as well as a buffer in the relationship between happiness aversion and depression. In ad- Copyright © 2020 by author(s) and dition, exploratory analysis of a small longitudinal dataset (N = 74) suggested Scientific Research Publishing Inc. that hope also played the same roles in the relationship between fear of hap- This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution International piness and depression over time. These findings suggest that interventions License (CC BY 4.0).
    [Show full text]
  • Melancholia Before the Twentieth Century: Fear and Sorrow Or Partial Insanity?
    MINI REVIEW ARTICLE published: 03 February 2015 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00081 Melancholia before the twentieth century: fear and sorrow or partial insanity? Diogo Telles-Correia* and João Gama Marques University Clinic of Psychiatry and Psychology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Lisbon, Lisboa, Portugal Edited by: Throughout the history of psychopathology, several meanings have been assigned to the Anne Giersch, Institut National de la term melancholia. The main ones were related to affective disorders (fear and sadness) Santé et de la Recherche Médicale, France and abnormal beliefs. At the time of Hippocrates melancholia was regarded mainly in Reviewed by: its affective component. Since that time, and until the eighteenth century, authors and Mark Elliott, National University of opinions have been divided, with both aspects (affective disorders and abnormal beliefs), Ireland Galway, Ireland being valued. Finally, in the eighteenth to nineteenth centuries, with Pinel at its peak, Alexandre Todorov, Washington melancholia becomes exclusively a synonym of abnormal beliefs. At the turn of the University School of Medicine, USA nineteenth to the twentieth century, the affective component returns as the main aspect Rebekka Lencer, University of characterizing melancholia. Muenster, Germany *Correspondence: Keywords: melancholia, depression, psychopathology, history, eighteenth century, history, nineteenth century Diogo Telles-Correia, University Clinic of Psychiatry and Psychology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Lisbon, Hospital Santa Maria, Avenida Egas Moniz, Lisboa 1649-028, Portugal e-mail: [email protected] INTRODUCTION FROM HIPPOCRATES TO THE EIGHTEENTH CENTURY Throughout the history of psychopathology, many terms have Hippocrates (460–379 BC) attributed the melancholic state been introduced. Some have disappeared, as “Phrenitis” and (as well as other disorders such as dysentery, skin rashes, etc.) “Carum,” while others have survived (Berrios, 1996a).
    [Show full text]
  • Coping with Sorrow, Loss and Grief
    Coping with sorrow, loss and grief The term “Grief” is used to describe reactions and feelings that a person might have to the loss of someone or something that is important to them. The feeling of loss not only covers the immediate loss of a relationship, good health, a job, a way of life or the death of a loved one, but also includes the loss of generations of family bonds, the removal of links to spiritual land rights or neglect of the cultural heritage. What is grief? How long does grief last? Grief is not an illness – it can not be cured or You can’t put a time limit on hurried along. No two people experience grief in grief. The best answer seems to the same way. It is affected by many factors such as be “as long as it takes”. Grief the nature of the loss, the person’s past history, their doesn’t occur in neat stages. It cultural and spiritual beliefs, and their personality. is a chaotic process, which is There is no right or wrong way to experience grief. different for each person. Some common grief reactions include: We don’t recover from grief as • Sadness, crying we might from some physical • Shock, numbness illness. Instead, we gradually learn to adjust to the loss. This • Difficulty accepting the loss adjustment process depends on • Anger, guilt, shame how you express your grief and other feelings, • Isolation, alienation, loneliness your life experience, previous losses, coping style, • Relief personality, physical health and available support • Fear that reactions may not be normal systems.
    [Show full text]