Systematic Review with Meta-Analysis Catherine M Calvin, Ian J Deary, Candida Fenton, Beverly Roberts, Geoff Der, Nicola Leckenby, David Batty

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Systematic Review with Meta-Analysis Catherine M Calvin, Ian J Deary, Candida Fenton, Beverly Roberts, Geoff Der, Nicola Leckenby, David Batty Intelligence in youth and all-cause-mortality: systematic review with meta-analysis Catherine M Calvin, Ian J Deary, Candida Fenton, Beverly Roberts, Geoff Der, Nicola Leckenby, David Batty To cite this version: Catherine M Calvin, Ian J Deary, Candida Fenton, Beverly Roberts, Geoff Der, et al.. Intelligence in youth and all-cause-mortality: systematic review with meta-analysis. International Journal of Epidemiology, Oxford University Press (OUP), 2010, 10.1093/ije/DYQ190. hal-00637030 HAL Id: hal-00637030 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00637030 Submitted on 29 Oct 2011 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Intelligence in youth and all-cause-mortality: systematic review with meta-analysis For Review Only Journal: International Journal of Epidemiology Manuscript ID: IJE-2010-05-0505.R1 Manuscript Type: Original Article Date Submitted by the 26-Aug-2010 Author: Complete List of Authors: Calvin, Catherine; University of Edinburgh, Dept of Psychology Deary, Ian; University of Edinburgh, Dept of Psychology Fenton, Candida; University of Glasgow, Medical Research Council Social & Public Health Sciences Unit Roberts, Beverly; University of Edinburgh, Dept of Psychology Der, Geoff; University of Glasgow Leckenby, Nicola; University of Edinburgh, Dept of Psychology Batty, David; University of Edinburgh, Dept of Psychology; University of Glasgow, Medical Research Council Social & Public Health Sciences Unit intelligence, mortality, meta-analysis, socioeconomic factors, Key Words: systematic review Page 1 of 49 1 2 3 Intelligence in youth and all-cause-mortality: systematic review with meta- 4 5 6 analysis 7 8 9 10 a 11 Catherine M. Calvin 12 a 13 5 Ian J. Deary 14 15 Candida Fenton b 16 17 a 18 Beverly Roberts 19 20 Geoff Der b For Review Only 21 22 Nicola Leckenby a 23 24 a, b, c 25 10 G. David Batty 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 aCentre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology (CCACE), Department of 35 36 37 15 Psychology, University of Edinburgh, UK 38 b 39 Medical Research Council Social & Public Health Sciences Unit, Glasgow, UK 40 41 cDepartment of Epidemiology & Public Health, University College London, 1-19 42 43 44 Torrington Place, London WC1E 6BT 45 46 47 48 20 -------- 49 50 51 Correspondence to Ian Deary, Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive 52 53 Epidemiology, Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh EH8 54 55 9JZ, UK . T. + 44 131 650 3452. E. [email protected] 56 57 58 59 60 25 Word count: 6022 1 Page 2 of 49 1 2 3 4 Abstract 5 6 Background. A number of prospective cohort studies have examined the association 7 8 9 between intelligence in childhood or youth and life expectancy in adulthood; 10 11 however, the effect size of this association have yet to be quantified and previous 12 13 30 reviews require updating. 14 15 16 Methods. The systematic review included an electronic search of EMBASE, 17 18 MEDLINE and PSYCHINFO databases. This yielded 16 unrelated studies that met 19 20 inclusion criteria, Forcomprising Review 22,453 deaths among Only 1,107,022 participants. 21 22 23 Heterogeneity was assessed, and fixed effects models were applied to the aggregate 24 25 35 data. Publication bias was evaluated, and sensitivity analyses were conducted. 26 27 Results. A one SD advantage in cognitive test scores was associated with a 24% (95% 28 29 30 CI 23-25%) lower risk of death, during a 17 to 69 year follow-up. There was little 31 32 evidence of publication bias (Egger’s intercept = 0.10, p = 0.81), and the intelligence- 33 34 35 mortality association was similar for men and women. Adjustment for childhood 36 37 40 socioeconomic status (SES) in the nine studies containing these data had almost no 38 39 impact on this relationship, suggesting that this is not a confounder of the intelligence- 40 41 42 mortality association. Controlling for adult SES in five studies and for education in 43 44 six studies attenuated the intelligence-mortality hazard ratios by 34% and 54%, 45 46 respectively. 47 48 49 45 Conclusions. Future investigations should address the extent to which attenuation of 50 51 the intelligence-mortality link by adult SES indicators is due to mediation, over- 52 53 adjustment, and/or confounding. The explanation(s) for association between higher 54 55 56 early life intelligence and lower risk of adult mortality require further elucidation. 57 58 Key words: Intelligence, Mortality , Meta-analysis, Socioceconomic Factors, 59 60 50 Systematic review . 2 Page 3 of 49 1 2 3 4 Introduction 5 6 Individual differences in intelligence (cognitive ability, mental ability) test scores, as 7 8 9 measured by standardised IQ-type tests in childhood, show an inverse association 10 11 with risk of death from all causes throughout adulthood. That is, higher intelligence 12 13 55 appears to confer protection. This finding is replicated in prospective cohorts from 14 15 1 2 16 several Westernized countries , across different ranges of intelligence , and in follow- 17 18 up periods from early through to late adulthood. 2-4 19 20 IntelligenceFor and somatic Review health may be inextricably Only linked throughout the life 21 22 23 course. However, longitudinal studies help to establish causal pathway models of the 24 25 60 effects of one upon the other. For example, morbidities such as diabetes, cancer, 26 27 stroke and peripheral atherosclerosis, and/or their treatments, are reported to cause a 28 29 5-10 30 decline in cognitive function after longitudinal follow-up. This illness-to-cognitive 31 32 ability direction of association is a commonplace finding. The reverse direction of 33 34 35 association is studied less often, and has only recently come to be recognized under 36 11, 12 37 65 the term ‘cognitive epidemiology’. That is, mental ability scores from early life 38 39 associated with later adulthood morbidities, and before any somatic symptoms or risk 40 41 42 factors of disease are manifest, provide evidence that cognitive abilities may be 43 44 predictive of later health outcomes. 45 46 The association between pre-morbid intelligence and adult all-cause mortality 47 48 1 49 70 was the subject of a systematic review , in which all nine studies that met the 50 51 inclusion criteria demonstrated an inverse relationship between intelligence and risk 52 53 of dying by follow-up. The review did not quantify the association. Furthermore, there 54 55 56 were insufficient studies to address comprehensively a number of pertinent questions 57 58 from this research domain. One issue is whether or not the association between 59 60 75 intelligence and mortality is the same in women as in men. For example, it is possible 3 Page 4 of 49 1 2 3 that sex differences in the incidence, age at onset of health behaviours, and the extent 4 5 13, 14 6 to which these act as risk factors for disease , could produce sex-specific 7 8 intelligence-mortality gradients. Data from many more men than women have been 9 10 11 included in intelligence-mortality cohort studies to date, mainly due to some studies 12 13 80 using military conscript databases. Moreover, when mixed-sex cohorts report 14 15 mortality risk as predicted by intelligence for men and women separately, they rarely 16 17 18 test for statistical difference but, rather, report the observed trend. With more studies 19 20 now reporting hazardFor ratios (HRs)Review for mortality by Onlysex, there is an opportunity to 21 22 quantify the predictive effects of intelligence on mortality separately for men and 23 24 25 85 women. 26 27 A second issue yet to be evaluated systematically is the extent to which 28 29 intelligence as a predictor of mortality is confounded by early life environmental 30 31 32 influences including socioeconomic factors. Socioeconomic status (SES) is 33 34 established as an important determinant of public health inequalities,15-18 including 35 36 37 90 risk of mortality, and it can carry influence in childhood, via factors such as family 38 39 income and parental education, to predict individual differences in childhood 40 41 intelligence. 19, 20 In this context, therefore, intelligence may be considered a mediating 42 43 44 variable on the pathway between early life influences and adult health outcomes. If 45 46 early social factors substantially confound the link between intelligence and longevity, 47 48 95 then adjusting for childhood SES would sizeably attenuate the effect size of the 49 50 1 51 association between intelligence and mortality. In their systematic review Batty et al 52 53 identified three out of nine studies that adjusted for childhood SES: one of these 54 55 showed no change from an unadjusted model, and two had modest attenuating effects, 56 57 58 suggesting that intelligence has independent effects on risk of mortality from those of 59 60 4 Page 5 of 49 1 2 3 100 early socioeconomic influences. Due to this small number of studies, the role of 4 5 6 childhood SES in the intelligence-mortality link requires further investigation. 7 8 One explanation why intelligence may exert an influence on life expectancy is 9 10 21 22 11 its ability to predict educational outcomes and occupational class, which can both 12 13 affect health outcomes via a number of mechanisms, for example the knowledge and 14 15 105 living conditions that contribute to better personal health risk assessment, behaviours 16 17 23 18 and management.
Recommended publications
  • Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology
    Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology Cognitive decline is the single most feared aspect of growing old. It is also the most costly, in terms of the financial, the personal and societal burden. Many organisations—governmental, academic, research-funding, professional and charitable—have announced that is has to be addressed, that too little progress has been made in the field, and that more integrated research is needed to understand the mechanisms of cognitive ageing . The University of Edinburgh Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology will answer this call directly by working on identifying the risk factors for, and mechanisms of, individual differences in age-related cognitive decline—cognitive ageing—and its relationship to morbidity and mortality in later life—cognitive epidemiology. The Centre’s objectives The Centre will initially run from 2008 to 2013. It will enhance the University of Edinburgh’s research on lifelong health and wellbeing, especially with regard to cognition. It will have a doctoral training programme and clinical research fellowship. The Centre’s objectives are: • To explore lifecourse influences on cognitive ageing and pathways whereby cognitive ability in early life affects later health—cognitive epidemiology. As a contribution to this it will maintain, develop and exploit the unique long-term human cohort studies assembled in Scotland as new national resources. • To advance knowledge by research into biological, neurological, genetic, social, economic, and psychological aspects of cognitive ageing in humans and lifecourse mammalian model systems. • To develop and evaluate methods in psychological, genetic, other biological, and brain imaging science to assess, monitor, and prevent or ameliorate decline in mental functions with a view to providing a rational basis for translating this into potential interventions.
    [Show full text]
  • September 2020 Lothian Birth Cohorts News
    Sharing results of the LBC1936 COVID-19 The Disconnected Mind aims to understand how questionnaire with Public Health Scotland changes in the brain’s white matter – its connectivity – In another exciting development, the team was contribute to age-related cognitive decline in humans. contacted by Public Health Scotland (PHS), and we prepared a special report including a privileged first Newsletter 51: September 2020 view of the COVID-19 questionnaire results which Welcome to the Autumn 2020 Disconnected Mind Prof Ian Deary presented to one of the working newsletter. This issue includes news about the groups. Following the presentation, epidemiologist Disconnected Mind/Lothian Birth Cohorts (LBC) Markéta Keller, on behalf of PHS, had this to say: team, our latest publications, and the events we have participated in recently. A message from our “We all are currently exposed to an unprecedented colleagues at Age UK is also included on page 7. situation where a fast and sophisticated approach to generate evidence is essential to formulate informed For further information about this newsletter or to public health decisions. Given the long-term contribute to future issues, please contact us using wonderful work conducted by Professor Deary and the details on page 8. the Lothian Birth Cohorts team, it comes as no surprise that they responded to the current COVID- Lothian Birth Cohorts News 19 situation in a pragmatic and wise manner. LBC1936 COVID-19 questionnaire update The LBC1936 COVID-19 Questionnaire has You might remember that, in our last edition, we produced most interesting insights into older announced the launch of our new LBC1936 COVID- people’s understanding of, feeling in, and responses 19 questionnaire, developed by our testing team to to the current situation.
    [Show full text]
  • Starting Young Decades-Old IQ Test Records from Scottish Children Have Opened a Unique Window on How the Brain Ages by Emily Underwood
    Starting young Decades-old IQ test records from Scottish children have opened a unique window on how the brain ages By Emily Underwood n 4 June in 1947, just before being decoding cyphers. There were 71 questions ing and to address fears that the average released for the summer holiday, in all, with only 45 minutes to finish. Scottish intelligence was dropping as pro- 11-year-old Sheila McGowan sat down More than 70,000 other 11-year-olds took fessional families had fewer children. at her desk with a pencil and paper to the same test that day, as part of one of McGowan remembers the test “very take an intelligence test at a state-run the first efforts to measure the intelligence well,” because her mother was gravely ill school in Glasgow, Scotland. It began of an age cohort across an entire nation. when she took it. After her mom died the Oeasily enough, with simple analogies: “A Called the Scottish Mental Surveys, the following April, McGowan lived alone with man is to skin as what—coat, animal, bird, tests, including an earlier survey admin- her father, who had to work night shifts at skin or cloth—is to fur?” for example. The istered in 1932, were originally aimed at the local shipyard to make ends meet. She test quickly progressed to more difficult determining how many children were too had scored high on the intelligence test, challenges: spatial puzzles, arithmetic, and “mentally defective” to benefit from school- but like many poor Scottish teenagers at EDINBURGH OF ROBERTSON/UNIVERSITY DOUGLAS PHOTO: EPIDEMIOLOGY AND COGNITIVE AGEING COGNITIVE CENTRE FOR 568 31 OCTOBER 2014 • VOL 346 ISSUE 6209 sciencemag.org SCIENCE Published by AAAS THE AGING BRAIN SPECIAL SECTION In April, psychologist Ian Deary (front, center) gathered not exercise, education, or any other virtu- trove of documents, having spent decades surviving members of the Lothian Birth Cohorts to ous activity, but rather simply an individu- studying the cognitive and biological ba- hear about how their brains are aging.
    [Show full text]
  • Introduction to the Special Issue on Cognitive Epidemiology
    Intelligence 37 (2009) 517–519 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Intelligence Introduction to the special issue on cognitive epidemiology Ian J. Deary Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, 7 George Square, Edinburgh EH8 9JZ, United Kingdom article info abstract Article history: This is an introduction to a special issue of the journal Intelligence on cognitive epidemiology. Received 29 April 2009 Cognitive epidemiology is a new field of study, which examines the associations between Received in revised form 30 April 2009 intelligence—usually from early in life—and later morbidity (physical and mental) and Accepted 5 May 2009 mortality. In addition to exploring and establishing associations, studies within cognitive Available online 23 May 2009 epidemiology attempt to explain them, by testing possible confounders and mediators, and complex pathways, of intelligence–health associations. Popular among mediators are health Keywords: Intelligence behaviours and education, and the well-known risk factors for chronic illnesses such as fi Mortality cardiovascular disease. In this special issue, readers will nd advances in all of these matters. Health Thirteen new empirical studies, all involving large cohorts of humans, provide novel Illness associations between intelligence and mortality, morbidity, and health behaviours and risk Morbidity factors. New hypotheses of these associations are tested. This is the largest collection of IQ cognitive epidemiology studies to date. Together, they will take the field forward by a quantum Cognitive epidemiology jump. This is a feast of cognitive epidemiology, establishing that, beside education and occupation, health outcomes contribute to the impressive predictive validity of intelligence differences. © 2009 Elsevier Inc.
    [Show full text]
  • Genetic Foundations of Human Intelligence
    Edinburgh Research Explorer Genetic foundations of human intelligence Citation for published version: Deary, IJ, Johnson, W & Houlihan, LM 2009, 'Genetic foundations of human intelligence', Human Genetics, vol. 126, no. 1, pp. 215-232. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00439-009-0655-4 Digital Object Identifier (DOI): 10.1007/s00439-009-0655-4 Link: Link to publication record in Edinburgh Research Explorer Document Version: Peer reviewed version Published In: Human Genetics Publisher Rights Statement: Deary, I. J., Johnson, W. & Houlihan, L. M. Jul 2009, "Genetic foundations of human intelligence", in Human Genetics. 126, 1, p. 215-232.© Springer-Verlag, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00439-009-0655-4. The final publication is available at link.springer.com General rights Copyright for the publications made accessible via the Edinburgh Research Explorer is retained by the author(s) and / or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing these publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. Take down policy The University of Edinburgh has made every reasonable effort to ensure that Edinburgh Research Explorer content complies with UK legislation. If you believe that the public display of this file breaches copyright please contact [email protected] providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Download date: 24. Sep. 2021 Genetic foundations of human intelligence I. J. Deary, W. Johnson, L. M. Houlihan I. J. Deary, W. Johnson, L. M. Houlihan Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, 7 George Square, Edinburgh EH8 9JZ, Scotland, UK.
    [Show full text]
  • The Wilson Effect: the Increase in Heritability of IQ with Age
    Twin Research and Human Genetics Volume 16 Number 5 pp. 923–930 C The Authors 2013 doi:10.1017/thg.2013.54 The Wilson Effect: The Increase in Heritability of IQ With Age Thomas J. Bouchard Jr. Psychology Department, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, USA Ronald Wilson presented the first clear and compelling evidence that the heritability of IQ increases with age. We propose to call the phenomenon ‘The Wilson Effect’ and we document the effect diagrammatically with key twin and adoption studies, including twins reared apart, that have been carried out at various ages and in a large number of different settings. The results show that the heritability of IQ reaches an asymptote at about 0.80 at 18–20 years of age and continuing at that level well into adulthood. In the aggregate, the studies also confirm that shared environmental influence decreases across age, approximating about 0.10 at 18–20 years of age and continuing at that level into adulthood. These conclusions apply to the Westernized industrial democracies in which most of the studies have been carried out. Keywords IQ, age, intelligence, heritability, twins, adoptees Monozygotic twins became increasingly concordant Figure 1 from Wilson (1983) is a plot of kin correlations with age and also paralleled each other for the spurts across age. The data were gathered by the justly famous and lags in development. Dizygotic twins became less Louisville Longitudinal Twin Study. The plot illustrates the concordant with age and eventually matched their sin- changes in kin correlations that mediate the effect; rela- gleton siblings as closely as one another.
    [Show full text]
  • The Lothian Birth Cohort 1936 from Ages 70 to 73', Longitudinal and Life Course Studies, Vol
    Edinburgh Research Explorer Can we spot deleterious ageing in two waves of data? The Lothian Birth Cohort 1936 from ages 70 to 73 Citation for published version: Johnson, W, Gow, A, Corley, J, Redmond, P, Murray, C, Starr, J & Deary, I 2012, 'Can we spot deleterious ageing in two waves of data? The Lothian Birth Cohort 1936 from ages 70 to 73', Longitudinal and Life Course Studies, vol. 3, no. 3, pp. 312-331. https://doi.org/10.14301/llcs.v3i3.198 Digital Object Identifier (DOI): 10.14301/llcs.v3i3.198 Link: Link to publication record in Edinburgh Research Explorer Document Version: Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Published In: Longitudinal and Life Course Studies Publisher Rights Statement: ©Johnson, W., Gow, A., Corley, J., Redmond, P., Murray, C., Starr, J., & Deary, I. (2012). Can we spot deleterious ageing in two waves of data? The Lothian Birth Cohort 1936 from ages 70 to 73. Longitudinal and Life Course Studies, 3(3), doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.14301/llcs.v3i3.198 General rights Copyright for the publications made accessible via the Edinburgh Research Explorer is retained by the author(s) and / or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing these publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. Take down policy The University of Edinburgh has made every reasonable effort to ensure that Edinburgh Research Explorer content complies with UK legislation. If you believe that the public display of this file breaches copyright please contact [email protected] providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.
    [Show full text]
  • The Long Arm of Childhood Intelligence on Terminal Decline: Evidence from the Lothian Birth Cohort 1921 Dorina Cadar University
    The long arm of childhood intelligence on terminal decline: Evidence from the Lothian Birth Cohort 1921 Dorina Cadar University College London Annie Robitaille Université du Québec à Montréal Alison Pattie University of Edinburgh Ian J. Deary University of Edinburgh Graciela Muniz-Terrera University of Edinburgh Author note: Dorina Cadar, Behavioural Science and Health, University College London, London, United Kingdom; Annie Robitaille, Université du Québec à Montréal, Montreal, Quebec, Canada; Alison Pattie, Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom; Ian J. Deary, Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom & Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom; Graciela Muniz-Terrera, Centre for Dementia Prevention, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Dorina Cadar, Department of Behavioural Science and Health, 1-19 Torrington Place, University College London, London, WC1E 6BT, United Kingdom. E-mail: [email protected], T: +44 2076795969. 2 Running head: Childhood intelligence and terminal decline Acknowledgements Dorina Cadar is supported by the National Institute on Aging (Grants 5218182, RO1AG7644-01A1 and RO1AG017644) and the Economic and Social Research Council. Annie Robitaille and Graciela Muniz-Terrera were supported by NIH funding (P01AG043362; 2013-2018). Alison Pattie and the LBC1921 are supported by Age UK (Disconnected Mind programme). Ian Deary is supported by the Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, funded by the Medical Research Council and the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (MR/K026992/1). Disclosure Summary All the authors declare no financial relationships with any organisations that might have an interest in the submitted work; no other relationships or activities that could appear to have influenced the proposed work.
    [Show full text]
  • Introduction to the Special Issue on Cognitive Epidemiology
    Edinburgh Research Explorer Introduction to the special issue on cognitive epidemiology Citation for published version: Deary, IJ 2009, 'Introduction to the special issue on cognitive epidemiology', Intelligence, vol. 37, no. 6, pp. 517-519. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2009.05.001 Digital Object Identifier (DOI): 10.1016/j.intell.2009.05.001 Link: Link to publication record in Edinburgh Research Explorer Document Version: Peer reviewed version Published In: Intelligence Publisher Rights Statement: This is an author's accepted manuscript of the following article: Deary, I. J. (2009), "Introduction to the special issue on cognitive epidemiology", in : Intelligence. 37, 6, p. 517-519. © Elsevier. The final publication is available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2009.05.001 General rights Copyright for the publications made accessible via the Edinburgh Research Explorer is retained by the author(s) and / or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing these publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. Take down policy The University of Edinburgh has made every reasonable effort to ensure that Edinburgh Research Explorer content complies with UK legislation. If you believe that the public display of this file breaches copyright please contact [email protected] providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Download date: 27. Sep. 2021 Introduction to the special issue on cognitive epidemiology Ian J. Deary Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology Department of Psychology University of Edinburgh 7 George Square Edinburgh EH8 9JZ Email: [email protected] 1 Abstract This is an introduction to a special issue of the journal Intelligence on cognitive epidemiology.
    [Show full text]
  • Genetics and Intelligence Differences: Five Special Findings
    Molecular Psychiatry (2015) 20, 98–108 OPEN © 2015 Macmillan Publishers Limited All rights reserved 1359-4184/15 www.nature.com/mp EXPERT REVIEW Genetics and intelligence differences: five special findings R Plomin1 and IJ Deary2,3 Intelligence is a core construct in differential psychology and behavioural genetics, and should be so in cognitive neuroscience. It is one of the best predictors of important life outcomes such as education, occupation, mental and physical health and illness, and mortality. Intelligence is one of the most heritable behavioural traits. Here, we highlight five genetic findings that are special to intelligence differences and that have important implications for its genetic architecture and for gene-hunting expeditions. (i) The heritability of intelligence increases from about 20% in infancy to perhaps 80% in later adulthood. (ii) Intelligence captures genetic effects on diverse cognitive and learning abilities, which correlate phenotypically about 0.30 on average but correlate genetically about 0.60 or higher. (iii) Assortative mating is greater for intelligence (spouse correlations ~0.40) than for other behavioural traits such as personality and psychopathology (~0.10) or physical traits such as height and weight (~0.20). Assortative mating pumps additive genetic variance into the population every generation, contributing to the high narrow heritability (additive genetic variance) of intelligence. (iv) Unlike psychiatric disorders, intelligence is normally distributed with a positive end of exceptional performance that is a model for ‘positive genetics’. (v) Intelligence is associated with education and social class and broadens the causal perspectives on how these three inter-correlated variables contribute to social mobility, and health, illness and mortality differences.
    [Show full text]
  • Implications for Health Policy and Practice
    CELEBRATING THIRTY YEARS OF THE MIDSPAN STUDY: IMPLICATIONS FOR HEALTH POLICY AND PRACTICE Friday 25 November The Teacher Building, 14 St Enoch Square, Glasgow G1 4DB LIST OF CONTRIBUTORS SESSION CHAIRS • Anna Dominiczak , BHF Professor of Cardiovascular Medicine, University of Glasgow • Charles Gillis , retired Professor of Cancer Epidemiology, West of Scottand Cancer Surveillance Unit • Victor Hawthorne , retired Professor of Epidemiology, Ann Arbor, Michigan • Carol Tannahill , Director, Glasgow Centre for Population Health • Graham Watt , Professor of General Practice, University of Glasgow and Chair, MIDSPAN Study Steering Committee; SPEAKERS • Peter Brindle , Welcome Trust Clinical Epidemiology Fellow, University of Bristol • John Connell , Professor of Endocrinology, University of Glasgow • George Davey-Smith , Professor of Epidemiology, University of Bristol • Ian Deary , Professor of Psychology, University of Edinburgh • Phil Hanlon , Professor of Public Health, University of Glasgow • Carole Hart , Senior Research Fellow, University of Glasgow • David Hole , Professor of Epidemiology, University of Glasgow • Kate Hunt , Senior Research Scientist, MRC Social and Public Health Sciences Unit, University of Glasgow • Gordon Lowe , Professor of Vascular Medicine, University of Glasgow • Alex McConnachie , Consultant Statistician, Robertson Centre, University of Glasgow • Naveed Sattar , Professor of Metabolic Medicine, University of Glasgow • Mark Upton , General Practitioner and Epidemiologist, Stockton-on-Tees PROGRAMME 09:30 Registration
    [Show full text]
  • Program 10Th Annual Conference Hotel TRYP Centro Norte (Madrid, SPAIN)
    International Society for Intelligence Research ---December 17-19 2009--- Juan Huarte de San Juan Santiago Ramón y Cajal (1529-1588) (1852-1934) Juan Huarte de San Juan drew a His pioneering investigations of the remarkably cogent picture of microscopic structure of the brain were so individual differences in human original and influential that he is considered by thought. Huarte’s distinction between many to be the greatest neuroscientist of all problem solving by memory or by time [Jacobsen, Marcus (1993). Foundations of imagination is mirrored in neuroscience. Springer. pp. 237] contemporary theories that distinguish between the ability to do abstract reasoning and the ability to apply previously learned solution methods (E.B. Hunt, In Press, On human intelligence. Cambridge). Program 10th Annual Conference Hotel TRYP Centro Norte (Madrid, SPAIN) 1 Acknowledgements Organizers Roberto Colom & Mª Ángeles Quiroga Universidad Autónoma de Madrid & Universidad Complutense de Madrid Advisory Committee Thomas Bouchard University of Minnesota Ian Deary University of Edinburgh Douglas K Detterman Case Western Reserve University Linda Gottfredson University of Delaware Earl Hunt University of Washington, Seattle Wendy Johnson University of Edinburgh, University of Minnesota David Lubinski Vanderbilt University Robert Plomin University of London Con Stough Swinburne University of Technology Thanks to the Templeton Foundation for their support and Elsevier for sponsoring the reception. 2 International Society for Intelligence Research (ISIR) Program
    [Show full text]