Convolutional Neural Network Models of V1 Responses to Complex Patterns

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Convolutional Neural Network Models of V1 Responses to Complex Patterns bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/296301; this version posted April 6, 2018. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license. Journal of Computational Neuroscience manuscript No. (will be inserted by the editor) Convolutional neural network models of V1 responses to complex patterns Yimeng Zhang∗ · Tai Sing Lee · Ming Li · Fang Liu · Shiming Tang∗ Received: date / Accepted: date Abstract In this study, we evaluated the convolutional and Wiesel, 1968, 1959, 1962). V1 neurons are tradi- neural network (CNN) method for modeling V1 neu- tionally classified as simple and complex cells, which rons of awake macaque monkeys in response to a large are modeled by linear-nonlinear (LN) models (Heeger, set of complex pattern stimuli. CNN models outper- 1992) and energy models (Adelson and Bergen, 1985), formed all the other baseline models, such as Gabor- respectively. However, a considerable gap between the based standard models for V1 cells and various vari- standard theory of V1 neurons and reality has been ants of generalized linear models. We then systemati- demonstrated repeatedly, at least from two aspects. First, cally dissected different components of the CNN and although standard models explain neural responses to found two key factors that made CNNs outperform simple stimuli such as gratings well, they cannot explain other models: thresholding nonlinearity and convolu- satisfactorily neural responses to more complex stimuli, tion. In addition, we fitted our data using a pre-trained such as natural images and complex shapes (David and deep CNN via transfer learning. The deep CNN's higher Gallant, 2005; Victor et al., 2006; Hegd´eand Van Es- layers, which encode more complex patterns, outper- sen, 2007; K¨osterand Olshausen, 2013). Second, more formed lower ones, and this result was consistent with sophisticated analysis techniques have revealed richer our earlier work on the complexity of V1 neural code. structures in V1 neurons than those dictated by stan- Our study systematically evaluates the relative merits dard models (Rust et al., 2005; Carandini et al., 2005). of different CNN components in the context of V1 neu- As an additional yet novel demonstration of this gap, ron modeling. using large-scale calcium imaging techniques, we (Li et al., 2017; Tang et al., 2018) have recently discovered Keywords convolutional neural network V1 · · that a large percentage of neurons in the superficial lay- nonlinear regression system identification · ers of V1 of awake macaque monkeys respond strongly to highly specific complex features; this finding suggests that some V1 neurons act as complex pattern detectors 1 Introduction rather than Gabor-based edge detectors as dictated by classical studies (Jones and Palmer, 1987a; Dayan and There has been great interest in the primary visual cor- Abbott, 2001). tex (V1) since pioneering studies decades ago (Hubel While our previous work (Tang et al., 2018) has ∗ YZ and ST are co-corresponding authors. shown the existence of complex pattern detector neu- YZ and TL rons in V1, a quantitative understanding of the rela- Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition and Computer Sci- tionship between input stimuli and neural responses for ence Department, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213 those neurons has been lacking. One way to better un- derstand these neurons quantitatively is to build com- ML, FL, and ST Peking University School of Life Sciences and Peking- putational models that predict their responses given in- Tsinghua Center for Life Sciences, Beijing 100871, China put stimuli (Wu et al., 2006). If we can find a model that IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research at Peking Uni- accurately predicts neural responses to (testing) stim- versity, Beijing 100871, China uli not used during training, a careful analysis of that bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/296301; this version posted April 6, 2018. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license. 2 Yimeng Zhang∗ et al. model should give us insights into the computational literature, we believe that this is the first study that mechanisms of the modeled neuron(s). For example, we systematically evaluates the relative merits of different can directly examine different components of the model CNN components in the context of V1 neuron model- (McIntosh et al., 2017; McFarland et al., 2013; Prenger ing. et al., 2004), find stimuli that maximize the model out- put (Kindel et al., 2017; Olah et al., 2017), and decom- pose model parameters into simpler, interpretable parts 2 Stimuli and neural recordings (Rowekamp and Sharpee, 2017; Park et al., 2013). A large number of methods have been applied to 2.1 Stimuli model V1 neural responses, such as ordinary least squares (Theunissen et al., 2001; David and Gallant, 2005), Using two-photon calcium imaging techniques, we col- spike-triggered average (Theunissen et al., 2001), spike- lected neural population data in response to a large set triggered covariance (Touryan et al., 2005; Rust et al., of complex artificial \pattern" stimuli. The \pattern" 2005), generalized linear models (GLMs) (Kelly et al., stimulus set contains 9500 binary (black and white) im- 2010; Pillow et al., 2008), nested GLMs (McFarland ages of about 90 px by 90 px from five major categories: et al., 2013), subunit models (Vintch et al., 2015), and orientation stimuli (OT; bars and gratings), curvature artificial neural networks (Prenger et al., 2004). Com- stimuli (CV; curves, solid disks, and concentric rings), pared to more classical methods, convolutional neural corner stimuli (CN; line or solid corners), cross stim- networks (CNNs) have recently been found to be more uli (CX; lines crossing one another), and composition effective for modeling retinal neurons (Kindel et al., stimuli (CO; patterns created by combining multiple el- 2017) and V1 neurons in two studies concurrent to ours ements from the first four categories). The last four cat- (McIntosh et al., 2017; Cadena et al., 2017). In addi- egories are also collectively called non-orientation stim- tion, CNNs have been used for explaining inferotempo- uli (nonOT). See Figure 1 for some example stimuli. In ral cortex and some other areas (Yamins et al., 2013; this study, the central 40 px by 40 px parts of the stim- Kriegeskorte, 2015; Yamins and DiCarlo, 2016). Never- uli were used as model input as 40 pixels translated to theless, existing studies mostly treat the CNN as a black 1:33 degrees in visual angle for our experiments and all box without analyzing much the reasons underlying its recorded neurons had classical receptive fields of diam- success relative to other models, and we are trying to eters well below one degree in visual angle around the fill that knowledge gap explicitly in this study. stimulus center (Tang et al., 2018). The cropped stimuli were further downsampled to 20 px by 20 px for compu- To understand the CNN's success better, we first tational efficiency. Later, we use x to represent the t-th evaluated the performance of CNN models, Gabor-based t stimulus as a 20 by 20 matrix, with 0 for background standard models for simple and complex cells, and vari- and 1 for foreground (there can be intermediate values ous variants of GLMs on modeling V1 neurons of awake due to downsampling), and x~ to denote the vectorized macaque monkeys in response to a large set of complex t version of x as a 400-dimensional vector. pattern stimuli (Tang et al., 2018). We found that CNN t models outperformed all the other models, especially for neurons that acted more like complex pattern de- Stimulus type Previous work modeling V1 neurons mostly tectors than Gabor-based edge detectors. We then sys- used natural images or natural movies (Kindel et al., tematically explored different variants of CNN models 2017; Cadena et al., 2017; David and Gallant, 2005), in terms of their nonlinear structural components, and while we used artificial pattern images (Tang et al., found that thresholding nonlinearity and max pooling, 2018). While neural responses to natural stimuli ar- especially the former, were important for the CNN's guably reflect neurons' true nature better, it has the performance. We also found that convolution (spatially following problems in our current study: 1) public data shifted filters with shared weights) in the CNN was ef- sets (Coen-Cagli et al., 2015) of V1 neurons typically fective for increasing model performance. Finally, we have much fewer images and neurons than our data set, used a pre-trained deep CNN (Simonyan and Zisser- and limited data may introduce bias on the results; 2) man, 2014) to model our neurons via transfer learn- artificially generated images can be easily classified and ing (Cadena et al., 2017), and found that the deep parameterized, and this convenience allows us to clas- CNN's higher layers, which encode more complex pat- sify neurons and compare models over different neu- terns, outperformed lower ones; the result was consis- ron classes separately (Section 2.2). While white noise tent with our earlier work (Tang et al., 2018) on the stimuli (Rust et al., 2005; McIntosh et al., 2017) are complexity of V1 neural code. While some of our ob- another option, we empirically found that white noise servations have been stated in alternative forms in the stimuli (when limited) would not be feasible for finding bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/296301; this version posted April 6, 2018.
Recommended publications
  • Visual Physiology
    Visual Physiology Perception and Attention Graham Hole Problems confronting the visual system: Retinal image contains a huge amount of information - which must be processed quickly. Retinal image is dim, blurry and distorted. Light levels vary enormously. Solutions: Enhance the important information (edges and contours). Process relative intensities, not absolute light levels. Use two systems, one for bright light and another for twilight (humans). The primary visual pathways: The right visual field is represented in the left hemisphere. The left visual field is represented in the right hemisphere The eye: 50% of light entering the eye is lost by absorption & scattering (diffraction). The rest strikes retinal photoreceptors. Photoreceptors: Rod Outer Segment Cone The retina contains two kinds of photoreceptors, rods and cones. Inner Segment Pedicle Why are two kinds of photoreceptor needed? 2 S Surface Luminance (cd/m ) c o t o White paper in starlight 0.05 p i c White paper in moonlight 0.5 White paper in artificial light 250 P ho t Computer monitor, TV 100 o p i White paper in sunlight 30,000 c Ambient light levels can vary hugely, by a factor of 10,000,000. Pupil diameter can vary retinal illumination by only a factor of 16. Rods respond at low (scotopic) light levels. Cones respond at high (photopic) light levels. Differences between rods and cones: Sensitivity Number Retinal distribution Visual Pigment Number and distribution of receptors: There are about 120 million rods, and 8 million cones. Cones are concentrated in central vision, around the fovea. Rods cover the entire retina, with the exception of the fovea.
    [Show full text]
  • A Linear Systems View to the Concept of Strfs Mounya Elhilali(1)
    A linear systems view to the concept of STRFs Mounya Elhilali (1) , Shihab Shamma (2,3) , Jonathan Z Simon (2,3,4) , Jonathan B. Fritz (3) (1) Department of Electrical & Computer Engineering, Johns Hopkins University (2) Department of Electrical & Computer Engineering, University of Maryland, College Park (3)Institute for Systems Research, University of Maryland, College Park (4)Department of Biology, University of Maryland, College Park 1. Introduction A key requirement in the study of sensory nervous systems is the ability to characterize effectively neuronal response selectivity. In the visual system, striving for this objective yielded remarkable progress in understanding the functional organization of the visual cortex and its implications to perception. For instance, the discovery of ordered feature representations of retinal space, color, ocular dominance, orientation, and motion direction in various cortical fields has catalyzed extensive studies into the anatomical bases of this organization, its developmental course, and the changes it undergoes during learning or following injury. In the primary auditory cortex (A1), response selectivity of the neurons (also called their receptive fields ) is ordered topographically according to the frequency they are most tuned to, an organization inherited from the cochlea. Beyond their tuning, however, A1 responses and receptive fields exhibit a bewildering variety of dynamics, frequency bandwidths, response thresholds, and patterns of excitatory and inhibitory regions. All this has been learned over decades of testing with a large variety of acoustic stimuli (tones, clicks, noise, and natural sounds) and response measures (tuning curves, rate-level functions, and binaural maps). A response measure that has proven particularly useful beyond the feature- specific measures enumerated earlier is the notion of a generalized spatiotemporal , or equivalently for the auditory system, the spectro-temporal receptive field (STRF).
    [Show full text]
  • The Neocognitron As a System for Handavritten Character Recognition: Limitations and Improvements
    The Neocognitron as a System for HandAvritten Character Recognition: Limitations and Improvements David R. Lovell A thesis submitted for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering University of Queensland March 14, 1994 THEUliW^^ This document was prepared using T^X and WT^^. Figures were prepared using tgif which is copyright © 1992 William Chia-Wei Cheng (william(Dcs .UCLA. edu). Graphs were produced with gnuplot which is copyright © 1991 Thomas Williams and Colin Kelley. T^ is a trademark of the American Mathematical Society. Statement of Originality The work presented in this thesis is, to the best of my knowledge and belief, original, except as acknowledged in the text, and the material has not been subnaitted, either in whole or in part, for a degree at this or any other university. David R. Lovell, March 14, 1994 Abstract This thesis is about the neocognitron, a neural network that was proposed by Fuku- shima in 1979. Inspired by Hubel and Wiesel's serial model of processing in the visual cortex, the neocognitron was initially intended as a self-organizing model of vision, however, we are concerned with the supervised version of the network, put forward by Fukushima in 1983. Through "training with a teacher", Fukushima hoped to obtain a character recognition system that was tolerant of shifts and deformations in input images. Until now though, it has not been clear whether Fukushima's ap- proach has resulted in a network that can rival the performance of other recognition systems. In the first three chapters of this thesis, the biological basis, operational principles and mathematical implementation of the supervised neocognitron are presented in detail.
    [Show full text]
  • Different Roles for Simple-Cell and Complex-Cell Inhibition in V1
    The Journal of Neuroscience, November 12, 2003 • 23(32):10201–10213 • 10201 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Different Roles for Simple-Cell and Complex-Cell Inhibition in V1 Thomas Z. Lauritzen1,4 and Kenneth D. Miller1,2,3,4 1Graduate Group in Biophysics, 2Departments of Physiology and Otolaryngology, 3Sloan-Swartz Center for Theoretical Neurobiology, and 4W. M. Keck Center for Integrative Neuroscience, University of California, San Francisco, California 94143-0444 Previously, we proposed a model of the circuitry underlying simple-cell responses in cat primary visual cortex (V1) layer 4. We argued that the ordered arrangement of lateral geniculate nucleus inputs to a simple cell must be supplemented by a component of feedforward inhibition that is untuned for orientation and responds to high temporal frequencies to explain the sharp contrast-invariant orientation tuning and low-pass temporal frequency tuning of simple cells. The temporal tuning also requires a significant NMDA component in geniculocortical synapses. Recent experiments have revealed cat V1 layer 4 inhibitory neurons with two distinct types of receptive fields (RFs): complex RFs with mixed ON/OFF responses lacking in orientation tuning, and simple RFs with normal, sharp-orientation tuning (although, some respond to all orientations). We show that complex inhibitory neurons can provide the inhibition needed to explain simple-cell response properties. Given this complex cell inhibition, antiphase or “push-pull” inhibition from tuned simple inhibitory neurons acts to sharpen
    [Show full text]
  • Development of Spatial Coarse-To-Fine Processing in the Visual Pathway
    Development of spatial coarse-to-fine processing in the visual pathway Jasmine A. Nirody ∗ July 16, 2013 Abstract The sequential analysis of information in a coarse-to-fine manner is a fundamental mode of processing in the visual pathway. Spatial frequency (SF) tuning, arguably the most fundamental feature of spatial vision, provides particular intuition within the coarse-to-fine framework: low spatial frequencies convey global information about an image (e.g., general orientation), while high spatial frequencies carry more detailed information (e.g., edges). In this paper, we study the development of cortical spatial frequency tuning. As feedforward input from the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) has been shown to have significant influence on cortical coarse-to-fine processing, we present a firing-rate based thalamocortical model which includes both feedforward and feedback components. We analyze the relationship between various model parameters (including cortical feedback strength) and responses. We confirm the importance of the antagonistic relationship between the center and surround responses in thalamic relay cell receptive fields (RFs), and further characterize how specific structural LGN RF parameters affect cortical coarse-to-fine processing. Our results also indicate that the effect of cortical feedback on spatial frequency tuning is age-dependent: in particular, cortical feedback more strongly affects coarse-to-fine processing in kittens than in adults. We use our results to propose an experimentally testable hypothesis for the function of the extensive feedback in the corticothalamic circuit. 1 Introduction The mode of information processing in neural sensory systems has been the subject of many experimental and computational studies. It is intuitive that visual information is processed sequentially in a coarse-to-fine manner—when looking quickly at a scene, we process the general features before focusing on individual objects.
    [Show full text]
  • How Simple Cells Are Made in a Nonlinear Network Model of the Visual Cortex
    The Journal of Neuroscience, July 15, 2001, 21(14):5203–5211 How Simple Cells Are Made in a Nonlinear Network Model of the Visual Cortex D. J. Wielaard, Michael Shelley, David McLaughlin, and Robert Shapley Center for Neural Science and Courant Institute of Mathematical Sciences, New York University, New York, New York 10012 Simple cells in the striate cortex respond to visual stimuli in an of the dependence of membrane potential on synaptic conduc- approximately linear manner, although the LGN input to the tances, and computer simulations, reveal that the nonlinearity striate cortex, and the cortical network itself, are highly nonlin- of corticocortical inhibition cancels the nonlinear excitatory ear. Although simple cells are vital for visual perception, there input from the LGN. This interaction produces linearized re- has been no satisfactory explanation of how they are produced sponses that agree with both extracellular and intracellular in the cortex. To examine this question, we have developed a measurements. The model correctly accounts for experimental large-scale neuronal network model of layer 4C␣ in V1 of the results about the time course of simple cell responses and also macaque cortex that is based on, and constrained by, realistic generates testable predictions about variation in linearity with cortical anatomy and physiology. This paper has two aims: (1) position in the cortex, and the effect on the linearity of signal to show that neurons in the model respond like simple cells. (2) summation, caused by unbalancing the relative strengths of To identify how the model generates this linearized response in excitation and inhibition pharmacologically or with extrinsic a nonlinear network.
    [Show full text]
  • The Hermann Grid Illusion Revisited
    Perception, 2005, volume 34, pages 1375 ^ 1397 DOI:10.1068/p5447 The Hermann grid illusion revisited Peter H Schiller, Christina E Carvey Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA; e-mail: [email protected] Received 12 October 2004, in revised form 12 January 2005; published online 23 September 2005 Abstract. The Hermann grid illusion consists of smudges perceived at the intersections of a white grid presented on a black background. In 1960 the effect was first explained by a theory advanced by Baumgartner suggesting the illusory effect is due to differences in the discharge characteristics of retinal ganglion cells when their receptive fields fall along the intersections versus when they fall along non-intersecting regions of the grid. Since then, others have claimed that this theory might not be adequate, suggesting that a model based on cortical mechanisms is necessary [Lingelbach et al, 1985 Perception 14(1) A7; Spillmann, 1994 Perception 23 691 ^ 708; Geier et al, 2004 Perception 33 Supplement, 53; Westheimer, 2004 Vision Research 44 2457 ^ 2465]. We present in this paper the following evidence to show that the retinal ganglion cell theory is untenable: (i) varying the makeup of the grid in a manner that does not materially affect the putative differ- ential responses of the ganglion cells can reduce or eliminate the illusory effect; (ii) varying the grid such as to affect the putative differential responses of the ganglion cells does not eliminate the illusory effect; and (iii) the actual spatial layout of the retinal ganglion cell receptive fields is other than that assumed by the theory.
    [Show full text]
  • David H. Hubel 1926–2013
    David H. Hubel 1926–2013 A Biographical Memoir by Robert H. Wurtz ©2014 National Academy of Sciences. Any opinions expressed in this memoir are those of the author and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Academy of Sciences. DAVID HUNTER HUBEL February 27, 1926–September 22, 2013 Elected to the NAS, 1971 David Hunter Hubel was one of the great neuroscientists of the 20th century. His experiments revolutionized our understanding of the brain mechanisms underlying vision. His 25-year collaboration with Torsten N. Wiesel revealed the beautifully ordered activity of single neurons in the visual cortex, how innate and learned factors shape its development, and how these neurons might be assem- bled to ultimately produce vision. Their work ushered in the current era of analyses of neurons at multiple levels of the cerebral cortex that seek to parse out the functional brain circuits underlying behavior. For these achieve- ments, David H. Hubel and Torsten N. Wiesel, along with Roger W. Sperry, shared the Nobel Prize for Physiology or Medicine in 1981. By Robert H. Wurtz Early life: growing up in Canada David Hubel was born on February 27 1926 in Windsor Ontario. Both of his parents were American citizens, born and raised in Detroit, but because he was born in Canada, he also held Canadian citizenship. His father was a chemical engineer, and his parents moved to Windsor because his father had a job with the Windsor Salt company. His mother, Elsie Hubel, was independent minded, with an interest in electricity and a regret that she had not attended college to study it.
    [Show full text]
  • The Receptive-Field Organization of Simple Cells in Primary Visual Cortex of Ferrets Under Natural Scene Stimulation
    4746 • The Journal of Neuroscience, June 1, 2003 • 23(11):4746–4759 The Receptive-Field Organization of Simple Cells in Primary Visual Cortex of Ferrets under Natural Scene Stimulation Darragh Smyth,1 Ben Willmore,2 Gary E. Baker,1 Ian D. Thompson,1 and David J. Tolhurst2 1Laboratory of Physiology, Oxford University, Oxford OX1 3PT, United Kingdom, and 2Department of Physiology, Cambridge University, Cambridge CB2 3EG, United Kingdom The responses of simple cells in primary visual cortex to sinusoidal gratings can primarily be predicted from their spatial receptive fields, as mapped using spots or bars. Although this quasilinearity is well documented, it is not clear whether it holds for complex natural stimuli. We recorded from simple cells in the primary visual cortex of anesthetized ferrets while stimulating with flashed digitized photographs of natural scenes. We applied standard reverse-correlation methods to quantify the average natural stimulus that invokes a neuronal response. Although these maps cannot be the receptive fields, we find that they still predict the preferred orientation of grating for each cell very well (r ϭ 0.91); they do not predict the spatial-frequency tuning. Using a novel application of the linear reconstruction method called regularized pseudoinverse, we were able to recover high-resolution receptive-field maps from the responses to a relatively small number of natural scenes. These receptive-field maps not only predict the optimum orientation of each cell (r ϭ 0.96) but also the spatial-frequency optimum (r ϭ 0.89); the maps also predict the tuning bandwidths of many cells. Therefore, our first conclusion is that the tuning preferences of the cells are primarily linear and constant across stimulus type.
    [Show full text]
  • Learning the Invariance Properties of Complex Cells from Their Responses to Natural Stimuli
    European Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 15, pp. 475±486, 2002 ã Federation of European Neuroscience Societies Learning the invariance properties of complex cells from their responses to natural stimuli Wolfgang EinhaÈuser, Christoph Kayser, Peter KoÈnig and Konrad P. KoÈrding Institute of Neuroinformatics, University of ZuÈrich and ETH ZuÈrich, Winterthurerstr. 190, 8057 ZuÈrich, Switzerland Keywords: development, primary visual cortex, temporal continuity, unsupervised learning Abstract Neurons in primary visual cortex are typically classi®ed as either simple or complex. Whereas simple cells respond strongly to grating and bar stimuli displayed at a certain phase and visual ®eld location, complex cell responses are insensitive to small translations of the stimulus within the receptive ®eld [Hubel & Wiesel (1962) J. Physiol. (Lond.), 160, 106±154; Kjaer et al. (1997) J. Neurophysiol., 78, 3187±3197]. This constancy in the response to variations of the stimuli is commonly called invariance. Hubel and Wiesel's classical model of the primary visual cortex proposes a connectivity scheme which successfully describes simple and complex cell response properties. However, the question as to how this connectivity arises during normal development is left open. Based on their work and inspired by recent physiological ®ndings we suggest a network model capable of learning from natural stimuli and developing receptive ®eld properties which match those of cortical simple and complex cells. Stimuli are drawn from videos obtained by a camera mounted to a cat's head, so they should approximate the natural input to the cat's visual system. The network uses a competitive scheme to learn simple and complex cell response properties. Employing delayed signals to learn connections between simple and complex cells enables the model to utilize temporal properties of the input.
    [Show full text]
  • Spatial Vision: from Spots to Stripes Clickchapter to Edit 3 Spatial Master Vision: Title Style from Spots to Stripes
    3 Spatial Vision: From Spots to Stripes ClickChapter to edit 3 Spatial Master Vision: title style From Spots to Stripes • Visual Acuity: Oh Say, Can You See? • Retinal Ganglion Cells and Stripes • The Lateral Geniculate Nucleus • The Striate Cortex • Receptive Fields in Striate Cortex • Columns and Hypercolumns • Selective Adaptation: The Psychologist’s Electrode • The Development of Spatial Vision ClickVisual to Acuity: edit Master Oh Say, title Can style You See? The King said, “I haven’t sent the two Messengers, either. They’re both gone to the town. Just look along the road, and tell me if you can see either of them.” “I see nobody on the road,” said Alice. “I only wish I had such eyes,” the King remarked in a fretful tone. “To be able to see Nobody! And at that distance, too!” Lewis Carroll, Through the Looking Glass Figure 3.1 Cortical visual pathways (Part 1) Figure 3.1 Cortical visual pathways (Part 2) Figure 3.1 Cortical visual pathways (Part 3) ClickVisual to Acuity: edit Master Oh Say, title Can style You See? What is the path of image processing from the eyeball to the brain? • Eye (vertical path) . Photoreceptors . Bipolar cells . Retinal ganglion cells • Lateral geniculate nucleus • Striate cortex ClickVisual to Acuity: edit Master Oh Say, title Can style You See? Acuity: The smallest spatial detail that can be resolved. ClickVisual to Acuity: edit Master Oh Say, title Can style You See? The Snellen E test • Herman Snellen invented this method for designating visual acuity in 1862. • Notice that the strokes on the E form a small grating pattern.
    [Show full text]
  • A Unified Theory of Early Visual Representa- Tions from Retina to Cortex Through Anatomi- Cally Constrained Deep Cnns
    Published as a conference paper at ICLR 2019 A UNIFIED THEORY OF EARLY VISUAL REPRESENTA- TIONS FROM RETINA TO CORTEX THROUGH ANATOMI- CALLY CONSTRAINED DEEP CNNS Jack Lindsey∗y, Samuel A. Ocko∗, Surya Ganguli1, Stephane Denyy Department of Applied Physics, Stanford and 1Google Brain, Mountain View, CA ABSTRACT The vertebrate visual system is hierarchically organized to process visual infor- mation in successive stages. Neural representations vary drastically across the first stages of visual processing: at the output of the retina, ganglion cell receptive fields (RFs) exhibit a clear antagonistic center-surround structure, whereas in the primary visual cortex (V1), typical RFs are sharply tuned to a precise orientation. There is currently no unified theory explaining these differences in representa- tions across layers. Here, using a deep convolutional neural network trained on image recognition as a model of the visual system, we show that such differences in representation can emerge as a direct consequence of different neural resource constraints on the retinal and cortical networks, and for the first time we find a sin- gle model from which both geometries spontaneously emerge at the appropriate stages of visual processing. The key constraint is a reduced number of neurons at the retinal output, consistent with the anatomy of the optic nerve as a strin- gent bottleneck. Second, we find that, for simple downstream cortical networks, visual representations at the retinal output emerge as nonlinear and lossy feature detectors, whereas they emerge as linear and faithful encoders of the visual scene for more complex cortical networks. This result predicts that the retinas of small vertebrates (e.g.
    [Show full text]