Learning Degree Zero Dismiss Its Remains As Yet Another Nasty Gob of Formless D

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Learning Degree Zero Dismiss Its Remains As Yet Another Nasty Gob of Formless D a quarterly of art and culture Issue 39 learnIng c US $12 canada $12 uK £7 3 0 5 6 3 5 8 9 8 9 6 6 5 0 Aplysia californica inking. Courtesy Genevieve Anderson. learning Degree Zero dismiss its remains as yet another nasty gob of formless D. Graham Burnett protoplasm coughed up by the deep. But in continuing your stroll, you would have walked past the creature Aplysia californica is a soft-bodied marine invertebrate upon which much of the modern scientific understand- that makes its living nibbling red algae in the kelp beds ing of learning has been built. and rocky reefs of the Pacific coast of the united States. For close to fifty years, Aplysia californica they are meaty, sometimes weighing more than ten has served as the irreplaceable model organism at pounds, and the deep hues of their food give their the center of what is arguably the most significant translucent flesh the mottled marbling of polished tor- twentieth-century research program in the sciences toiseshell. When scrunched up contentedly, they look of mind and behavior. In fact, on the afternoon of 8 a bit like rabbits, on account of two earlike tentacular December 2000, when the distinguished neuroscien- horns that sit atop their faceless heads—a resemblance tist eric Kandel stepped to the podium of Sweden’s that long ago gave rise to the common name for the Karolinska Institute to present his nobel Prize lecture, whole taxon, known as the “sea hares” since antiquity. he flashed up on the screen behind him a huge, photo- When irritated, A. californica release a cloud of acrid, shopped image of a robust Aplysia sporting around its mucosal, purple ink from a shockingly labial cleft that extended neck Kandel’s golden medallion—the most runs down the length of their backs and shelters the coveted prize in science. the mollusk, it was agreed, delicate folds of a feathery, blood-rich gill. they move deserved a little bling on the occasion. feelingly and with a certain dignity, like large slugs— So how did a herbivorous hermaphroditic gas- which is exactly what they are. tropod end up as a Stockholm honoree? the answer Stumbling on a defunct Aplysia in the tide-washed requires a quick turn through the twentieth-century flotsam of a California beach, you would be inclined to science of learning and memory. 59 to begin with, then, ask the most basic question: the product. and for this reason questions about how What is “learning,” anyway? a recent handbook in the we learn have long been entangled with the question of field offers the following definition: how we form and maintain memories. across the first half of the twentieth century, a Learning is the process by which relatively permanent variety of physiologists and experimental psychologists changes occur in behavior potential as a result of went after these problems in different ways. Some, experience.1 like the German psychologist hermann ebbinghaus, worked directly with human subjects and tried to Which is to say, learning is a little like an accelerated identify consistent patterns in the dynamics of informa- mechanism for evolutionary adaptation, one that tion acquisition. In a series of classic experiments that works at the scale of the individual. If you can figure out involved memorizing long sequences of nonsense syl- how reliably to convert new experiences into revised lables, ebbinghaus eventually codified what we now behavior, you’ve just gotten a serious leg up on the busi- call “the learning curve”—a time-sequence plot of the ness of being here, since you’ve effectively acquired decreasing effort needed to master a given task. Other the capacity to redeem some measure of the general investigators, like the russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov unpleasantness of existence—you can now turn your and the american psychologist edward thorndike, bad breaks into lessons. left humans aside and made fundamental contribu- true, there is an existential price to be paid for this tions to the understanding of learning by studying trick, in that now you are stuck with memory, against animals. Pavlov’s dogs, famously, learned to associate which nietzsche (among others) railed very persua- one stimulus (say, a bell) with another (say, a zap in sively. But no memory, no learning, since if you can’t the foot) in what has come to be known as “classical store information, you’ve got no way to make your past conditioning”—a behavioral phenomenon that has bear on your future. Learning is the process, memory is served ever since, for better or worse, as a basic Forty-nine-day-old Aplysia juvenile, nicknamed Claire, that has just completed metamorphosis. Courtesy the National Resource for Aplysia at the University of Miami. 60 laboratory proxy for learning writ large. thorndike, how to run a given maze, and then surgically remove working at Columbia university, studied cats as they various bits of their brains, and they still generally man- tried to figure out how to escape from a locked box. aged to find their way to the cheese. In the end, their Whereas in Pavlovian classical conditioning there performance appeared to have more to do with how is an arbitrary relationship between the stimuli to much of their brains you removed—not what part. But be associated (Bell? electrocution? no logical con- how could one investigate the basic biology of learning nection), in thorndike’s experiments the cats were and memory if it was so difficult to specify where to learning to make associations that weren’t arbitrary look? this sort of thing depressed physiologists, who at all (pull latch, get out of box). this kind of learn- like to find organs for different functions. meanwhile, ing—learning, via payoffs, about the world of causes the psychologists largely took up versions of what and effects—came to be called “operant conditioning.” is called “behaviorism,” a paradigm of mind science Philosophically oriented wags have tended to wonder that black-boxed cognitive activity, and treated organ- just how different these two kinds of conditioning really isms like input/output systems—which did not make are (isn’t classical conditioning simply the operant for much progress on the internal biology of learning. conditioning of animals forced to live in the “environ- Surveying the field in the early 1950s, Lashley, old and ment” of a laboratory?), but put this knotty matter aside. resigned, summed up the situation on a paradoxical What was significant was that all of these learning note: “I sometimes feel, in reviewing the evidence… situations were shown to share many consistent fea- that the necessary conclusion is that learning is just not tures: practice makes perfect; time off tends to reduce possible.”2 competency; reacquisition of capacities is faster than and yet, of course, it was. the question was: how at the first go. and these sorts of generalities could be to learn about learning? quantified, parameterized, and shown to possess reli- In an important book published in 2007, Science able statistical signatures that were conserved across without Laws, angela Creager and a number of other experimental animals and scenarios. historians of science drew attention to the importance By mid-century, then, there was broad consensus of “model organisms” in the history of twentieth- among researchers about the descriptive features of century biology. there are whole fields of investigation the basic behavioral dynamics of learning. But there that seem inextricable from particular creatures, which was nothing even remotely consistent about the various for various reasons (rational foresight, historical con- theories proposed to explain the underlying physiology tingency) end up functioning as obligatory passage of the process. how did learning actually happen? What points for disciplinary development. What would the was going on in the brain that accounted for these twentieth-century study of genetics have been without observable changes in the doings of dogs, cats, mon- Drosophila (the fruit fly)? Inconceivable. What about keys, mice, and army recruits? (the latter were great virology absent TMV (the tobacco mosaic Virus)? Simi- experimental subjects—a perfectly captive audience.) larly difficult to imagine. So profound is this linkage no one could say with any satisfaction. Yes, people between certain kinds of science and certain specific knew a fair bit about nerves and how they transmitted beasts that it has been dignified with a name, the Kuf- signals around the body. Yes, there was some evidence fler-Grundfest principle: “For every biological problem that regions of the mammalian brain were specialized there is a suitable organism in which to study it.”3 to manage particular cognitive, sensory, and motor When, in the early 1960s, Kandel—then an ambi- activities. and yes, there were even some ideas about tious young medical doctor—decided to turn his how to tie brain activity to behavior (one thinks here of research attentions to the neurophysiology of learn- the work of both Donald hebb and Karl Pribram). ing and memory, he took this principle to heart. Cats? nevertheless, exactly how the nervous system Dogs? rats? too complicated. the learned behaviors acquired and stored new information was hotly dis- in question? too intricate. Despite his Viennese origins puted, and even the best arguments remained largely and expansive interest in the nuance and complexity of speculative. among other difficulties, the available Freudian psychoanalysis, Kandel had a biophysicist’s experimental data were exceedingly hard to interpret. appetite for radical reduction. he wanted to study For instance, in a fascinating and discouraging set of learning in an animal built for the very simplest kinds investigations in this area, the most important ameri- of information acquisition and storage. an animal that can learning researcher in the period, Karl Lashley, could be understood as a little laboratory learning- demonstrated that you could take rats that had learned machine: limited behavioral repertoire; large, simple 61 wiring; a resilient metabolism; and, ideally, small teeth scientists were down at the level where a living crea- (no one likes getting chomped by lab animals).
Recommended publications
  • 'Introspectionism' and the Mythical Origins of Scientific Psychology
    Consciousness and Cognition Consciousness and Cognition 15 (2006) 634–654 www.elsevier.com/locate/concog ‘Introspectionism’ and the mythical origins of scientific psychology Alan Costall Department of Psychology, University of Portsmouth, Portsmouth, Hampshire PO1 2DY, UK Received 1 May 2006 Abstract According to the majority of the textbooks, the history of modern, scientific psychology can be tidily encapsulated in the following three stages. Scientific psychology began with a commitment to the study of mind, but based on the method of introspection. Watson rejected introspectionism as both unreliable and effete, and redefined psychology, instead, as the science of behaviour. The cognitive revolution, in turn, replaced the mind as the subject of study, and rejected both behaviourism and a reliance on introspection. This paper argues that all three stages of this history are largely mythical. Introspectionism was never a dominant movement within modern psychology, and the method of introspection never went away. Furthermore, this version of psychology’s history obscures some deep conceptual problems, not least surrounding the modern conception of ‘‘behaviour,’’ that continues to make the scientific study of consciousness seem so weird. Ó 2006 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Keywords: Introspection; Introspectionism; Behaviourism; Dualism; Watson; Wundt 1. Introduction Probably the most immediate result of the acceptance of the behaviorist’s view will be the elimination of self-observation and of the introspective reports resulting from such a method. (Watson, 1913b, p. 428). The problem of consciousness occupies an analogous position for cognitive psychology as the prob- lem of language behavior does for behaviorism, namely, an unsolved anomaly within the domain of an approach.
    [Show full text]
  • ICS18 - Study Questions
    ICS18 - study questions January 23, 2019 1. What is the broad and what is the narrow notion of cognition? 2. What is representation? Enumenrate examples of representations considerd within different disciplines (cog. psy- chology, neuroscience, AI) and show that they fulfill the definitional requirements 3. Enumerate and characterize briefly key cognitive disciplines 4. Key theses and principles of associationism 5. How to explain a mind: pure vs mixed associationism 6. A mind according to behaviorists 7. Why is behaviorism considered “the scientific psychology”? 8. Why behaviorism (as a theory of mind) was rejected? 9. Find the elements of behaviorism within contemporary cognitive theory of mind (either symbolic or connectionist) 10. Describe Turing Machine as a computational device 11. What is computation? (remember the roll of toilet paper...) 12. The idea of a mind as a “logic machine” (McCulloch, Pitts) 13. What features does a McCulloch-Pitts neuron have? 14. What is the main difference between a McC-P neuron and a perceptron? 15. What is a model? (cf. Kenneth Craik&Minsky) 16. What are the assumptions of phrenology? 17. Localizationist view on a brain (Gall, Spurzheim) 18. Holistic approaches to human brain 19. What does it mean that some part of our body has a “topographic representation” in the brain? 20. The consequences of Broca’s and Wernicke’s discoveries for a cognitive view on a brain 21. What is Brodmann famous for? 22. What did Golgi invent? What consequences had his discovery? 23. Describe Cajal’s discoveries and their influence on neuroscience 24. The key aassumptions of the “neuron doctrine” (Cajal) 25.
    [Show full text]
  • Varieties of Fame in Psychology Research-Article6624572016
    PPSXXX10.1177/1745691616662457RoedigerVarieties of Fame in Psychology 662457research-article2016 Perspectives on Psychological Science 2016, Vol. 11(6) 882 –887 Varieties of Fame in Psychology © The Author(s) 2016 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/1745691616662457 pps.sagepub.com Henry L. Roediger, III Washington University in St. Louis Abstract Fame in psychology, as in all arenas, is a local phenomenon. Psychologists (and probably academics in all fields) often first become well known for studying a subfield of an area (say, the study of attention in cognitive psychology, or even certain tasks used to study attention). Later, the researcher may become famous within cognitive psychology. In a few cases, researchers break out of a discipline to become famous across psychology and (more rarely still) even outside the confines of academe. The progression is slow and uneven. Fame is also temporally constricted. The most famous psychologists today will be forgotten in less than a century, just as the greats from the era of World War I are rarely read or remembered today. Freud and a few others represent exceptions to the rule, but generally fame is fleeting and each generation seems to dispense with the lessons learned by previous ones to claim their place in the sun. Keywords fame in psychology, scientific eminence, history of psychology, forgetting First, please take a quiz. Below is a list of eight names. done. (Hilgard’s, 1987, history text was better known Please look at each name and answer the following three than his scientific work that propelled him to eminence.) questions: (a) Do you recognize this name as belonging Perhaps some of you recognized another name or two to a famous psychologist? (b) If so, what area of study without much knowing why or what they did.
    [Show full text]
  • Using Conceptual Maps in Introductory Psychology
    Using Conceptual Maps in Introductory Psychology •Identify 10-15 key concepts or topics in the course –Classical vs. operant conditioning –Genetics and environmental interactions –Types of forgetting –Theoretical Perspectives –Cognitive, Social and Moral development –Descriptive, Correlational and Experimental Methods –Neuronal vs. synaptic transmission –Processes in human memory –Altered states of consciousness –Theories of dreaming –Schedules of reinforcement –Brain structures and functions Pick 5 to review Write the concept or topic at the top of a piece of blank paper In your own words, write an explanation or definition for each concept Do not use your text or notes Draw a web, or use a chart where possible Include names where important Compare your response to your text or notes and edit Sequence and number each page from 1=most important to 5=least important in terms of your study time Do the whole process again for the next group of concepts Integrate the numbering to guide you in scheduling what to work on first Example of a Conceptual Map Development Prenatal Infancy and Childhood Genetics Environment Attachment XX=girl teratogen biology sets limits XY=boy FAS enviro influences from sperm bio & env interact Cognitive Moral Psychosocial Piaget Vygotsky Kohlberg Erikson Piaget Vygotsky Stages: Continual, gradual process 1. Sensorimotor •Birth to 2 yrs Zone of proximal development •Object perm. -Experience can change 2. Preoper’tl development within limits of •2-7 years biological maturation •Centration 3. Concrete Theory of Mind Operational -understanding other people’s •7-12 years thinking •Conservation -similar to egocentrism 4. Formal operational •>12 years abstract thought Erikson Kohlberg Stages: Stages: 1.Trust/mistrust <1 year Preconventional •1.punishment/obey 2.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Functions of the Brain: Perception, Attention and Memory
    IFM LAB TUTORIAL SERIES # 6, COPYRIGHT c IFM LAB Cognitive Functions of the Brain: Perception, Attention and Memory Jiawei Zhang [email protected] Founder and Director Information Fusion and Mining Laboratory (First Version: May 2019; Revision: May 2019.) Abstract This is a follow-up tutorial article of [17] and [16], in this paper, we will introduce several important cognitive functions of the brain. Brain cognitive functions are the mental processes that allow us to receive, select, store, transform, develop, and recover information that we've received from external stimuli. This process allows us to understand and to relate to the world more effectively. Cognitive functions are brain-based skills we need to carry out any task from the simplest to the most complex. They are related with the mechanisms of how we learn, remember, problem-solve, and pay attention, etc. To be more specific, in this paper, we will talk about the perception, attention and memory functions of the human brain. Several other brain cognitive functions, e.g., arousal, decision making, natural language, motor coordination, planning, problem solving and thinking, will be added to this paper in the later versions, respectively. Many of the materials used in this paper are from wikipedia and several other neuroscience introductory articles, which will be properly cited in this paper. This is the last of the three tutorial articles about the brain. The readers are suggested to read this paper after the previous two tutorial articles on brain structure and functions [17] as well as the brain basic neural units [16]. Keywords: The Brain; Cognitive Function; Consciousness; Attention; Learning; Memory Contents 1 Introduction 2 2 Perception 3 2.1 Detailed Process of Perception .
    [Show full text]
  • 1. Getting Familiar with Matlab
    THE PREHISTORY OF COGNITIVE SCIENCE THE REACTION AGAINST BEHAVIORISM IN PSYCHOLOGY BEHAVIORISM?? (BEHAVIORAL PSYCHOLOGY) All behaviors are acquired through conditioning. Everything from speech to emotional responses were simply patterns of stimulus and learned response. Only observable behavior should be considered (cognitions, emotions, and moods are too subjective – ignore them all!) John B. Watson “Psychology as the Behaviorist Views It” 1913 "Give me a dozen healthy infants, well-formed, and my own specified world to bring them up in and I'll guarantee to take any one at random and train him to become any type of specialist I might select—doctor, lawyer, artist, merchant-chief and, yes, even beggar-man and thief, regardless of his talents, penchants, tendencies, abilities, vocations, and race of his ancestors." BEHAVIORISM?? (BEHAVIORAL PSYCHOLOGY) Two types of conditioning: Classical conditioning: association between US and CS, CS results in conditioned behavioral response Operant conditioning: learning that occurs through reinforcement and punishment, association between a behavior and a consequence for that behavior CLASSICAL CONDITIONING LITTLE ALBERT EXPERIMENT (PHOBIAS) Watson and Rayner (1920) OPERANT CONDITIONING SKINNER BOX REINFORCEMENT THEORY CLASSICAL CONDITIONING VS. OPERANT CONDITIONING THREE LANDMARK PAPERS 1. Tolman and Honzik (1930): Latent Learning 2. Tolman et al. (1946): Sequential learning? Vs. Cognitive spatial map? 3. Lashley (1951): Importance of planning and organization 1. LATENT LEARNING LATENT LEARNING ▪ Learning in Behaviorism ▪ All learning is the result of conditioning ▪ Conditioning depends upon processes of association and reinforcement ➢ So learning is either reinforcement learning or simpler associative learning RAT MAZE LATENT LEARNING 1. First group: get rewarded every time 2. Second group: never get rewarded 3.
    [Show full text]
  • Organizing Knowledge and Behavior at Yale's Institute of Human Relations Author(S): J
    Organizing Knowledge and Behavior at Yale's Institute of Human Relations Author(s): J. G. Morawski Source: Isis, Vol. 77, No. 2 (Jun., 1986), pp. 219-242 Published by: University of Chicago Press on behalf of History of Science Society Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/232650 Accessed: 22-12-2015 00:42 UTC Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/ info/about/policies/terms.jsp JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. History of Science Society and University of Chicago Press are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Isis. http://www.jstor.org This content downloaded from 129.133.6.95 on Tue, 22 Dec 2015 00:42:52 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions Organizing Knowledge and Behavior at Yale's Institute of Human Relations By J. G. Morawski* IN 1929 JAMES ANGELL, president of Yale, announced plans for a unique teaching and research center for those fields "directly concerned with the problems of man's individual and group conduct. The purpose is to correlate knowledge and coordinate technique in related fields that greater progress may be made in the understanding of human life. The time has certainly come once again to attempt a fruitful synthesis of knowledge." The New York Times described the experiment as dismantling the disciplinary "Great Wall of China" and compared it with the Renaissance transformation of knowledge.1 The Insti- tute of Human Relations (IHR), as the center was named, received over $4.5 million from the Rockefeller Foundation for its first decade of operation.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychologists and Physicians in the Borderlands of Science, 1900-1942
    PSYCHOLOGISTS AND PHYSICIANS IN THE BORDERLANDS OF SCIENCE, 1900-1942 By WADE EDWARD PICKREN A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA 1995 For my mother: WILLIE MERLE PICKREN, and in memoriam, BILL PICKREN, You taught me to love and work. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I wish to express my deepest gratitude to the chairman of my dissertation committee, Donald A. Dewsbury. Dr. Dewsbury has, from the beginning of this long project, been a model of encouragement, kindness, and unfailing generosity. He has shared his time, his great breadth of learning, his editorial ability, and his materials with me. My understanding of the history of psychology has been greatly deepened by our conversations. I also wish to acknowledge that Dr. Dewsbury has helped me to understand that data is a plural! Dr. Wilse B. Webb has also stimulated much thought in me about what I was doing and where I was going with my ideas. Although I did not avail myself of his wisdom as oft as I would have liked, his voice and his sharp eye were always with me. I hope that, in the future, time will allow me a greater opportunity to benefit from his great knowledge and experience. Both near at hand and from afar, Dr. Toby Appel has blessed me with the keenness of her insight . Her acceptance and friendly corrections of my halting efforts to write history have been much appreciated. One of my most pleasant memories of this experience is that of sitting at a table at iii Cafe Gardens talking about the history of biology or psychology, while hoping to hear some Van Morrison on the house music system.
    [Show full text]
  • Points of View in the Modern History of Psychology
    Points of View in the Modern History of Psychology Edited by Claude E. Buxton Department of Psychology Yale University New Haven, Connecticut 1985 ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. (Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Publishers) Orlando San Diego New York London Toronto Montreal Sydney Tokyo Passages from the following are reprinted by permission of the publishers: Newell, Α., Duncker on Thinking, in S. Koch & D. Leary (Eds.), A Century of Psychology as Science. Copyright 1985 by McGraw-Hill. Neisser, U., Cognitive Psychology. © 1967 by Prentice-Hall. COPYRIGHT © 1985 BY ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. ALL RIGHTS RESERVED. NO PART OF THIS PUBLICATION MAY BE REPRODUCED OR TRANSMITTED IN ANY FORM OR BY ANY MEANS, ELECTRONIC OR MECHANICAL, INCLUDING PHOTOCOPY, RECORDING, OR ANY INFORMATION STORAGE AND RETRIEVAL SYSTEM, WITHOUT PERMISSION IN WRITING FROM THE PUBLISHER. ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. Orlando, Florida 32887 United Kingdom Edition published by ACADEMIC PRESS INC. (LONDON) LTD. 24-28 Oval Road, London NW1 7DX LIBRARY OF CONGRESS CATALOGING IN PUBLICATION DATA Main entry under title: Points of view in the modern history of psychology. Includes indexes. 1. Psychology— History. I. Buxton, Claude E. BF81.P57 1985 150\9 85-4010 ISBN 0-12-148510-2 (alk. paper) PRINTED IN THE UNITED STATES OF AMERICA 85 86 87 88 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Contributors Numbers in parentheses indicate the pages on which the authors' contributions begin. Mitchell G. Ash (295), Department of History, University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa 52242 William Bevan (259), John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation, Chicago, Illinois 60603 Arthur L. Blumenthal (19, 51), Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts at Boston, Boston, Massachusetts 02125 Claude E.
    [Show full text]
  • The Problem of Serial Order in Behavior: Lashley's Legacy
    Human Movement Science 26 (2007) 525–554 www.elsevier.com/locate/humov The problem of serial order in behavior: Lashley’s legacy David A. Rosenbaum a,*, Rajal G. Cohen a, Steven A. Jax b, Daniel J. Weiss a, Robrecht van der Wel a a Department of Psychology, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA 16802, United States b Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute, Philadelphia, PA 19141, United States Available online 14 August 2007 Abstract In a prescient paper Karl Lashley (1951) rejected reflex chaining accounts of the sequencing of behavior and argued instead for a more cognitive account in which behavioral sequences are typi- cally controlled with central plans. An important feature of such plans, according to Lashley, is that they are hierarchical. Lashley offered several sources of evidence for the hierarchical organization for behavioral plans, and others afterward provided more evidence for this hypothesis. We briefly review that evidence here and then shift from a focus on the structure of plans (Lashley’s point of concen- tration) to the processes by which plans are formed in real time. Two principles emerge from the studies we review. One is that plans are not formed from scratch for each successive movement sequence but instead are formed by making whatever changes are needed to distinguish the move- ment sequence to be performed next from the movement sequence that has just been performed. This plan-modification view is supported by two phenomena discovered in our laboratory: the parameter remapping effect, and the handpath priming effect. The other principle we review is that even single movements appear to be controlled with hierarchically organized plans.
    [Show full text]
  • Researchers Are Painting Intricate Pictures of Individual Memories and Learning How the Brain Works in the Process
    PORTRAIT OF A MEMORY Researchers are painting intricate pictures of individual memories and learning how the brain works in the process. BY HELEN SHEN or someone who’s not a Sherlock superfan, cognitive neuroscience in the past decade are enabling researchers to uncover neuroscientist Janice Chen knows the BBC’s hit detective fundamental rules about how individual memories form, organize and drama better than most. With the help of a brain scanner, interact with each other. Using techniques for labelling active neurons, she spies on what happens inside viewers’ heads when they for example, teams have located circuits associated with the memory of watch the first episode of the series and then describe the plot. a painful stimulus in rodents and successfully reactivated those path- FChen, a researcher at Johns Hopkins University in Baltimore, ways to trigger the memory. And in humans, studies have identified Maryland, has heard all sorts of variations on an early scene, when a the signatures of particular recollections, which reveal some of the ways woman flirts with the famously aloof detective in a morgue. Some peo- that the brain organizes and links memories to aid recollection. Such ple find Sherlock Holmes rude while others think he is oblivious to the findings could one day help to reveal why memories fail in old age or woman’s nervous advances. But Chen and her colleagues found some- disease, or how false memories creep into eyewitness testimony. These thing odd when they scanned viewers’ brains: as different people retold insights might also lead to strategies for improved learning and memory.
    [Show full text]
  • The Psychology Book, Big Ideas Simply Explained
    THE PSYCHOLOGY BOOK THE PSYCHOLOGY BOOK LONDON, NEW YORK, MELBOURNE, MUNICH, AND DELHI DK LONDON DK DELHI First American Edition 2012 PROJECT ART EDITOR PROJECT ART EDITOR Published in the United States by Amy Orsborne Shruti Soharia Singh DK Publishing SENIOR EDITORS SENIOR ART EDITOR 375 Hudson Street Sam Atkinson, Sarah Tomley Chhaya Sajwan New York, New York 10014 EDITORS MANAGING ART EDITOR 2 4 6 8 10 9 7 5 3 1 Cecile Landau, Scarlett O’Hara Arunesh Talapatra 001—181320—Feb/2012 US EDITOR SENIOR EDITOR Copyright © 2012 Rebecca G. Warren Monica Saigal Dorling Kindersley Limited MANAGING ART EDITOR EDITORIAL TEAM All rights reserved. Karen Self Sreshtha Bhattacharya, Gaurav Joshi Without limiting the rights under the MANAGING EDITORS PRODUCTION MANAGER copyright reserved above, no part of Esther Ripley, Camilla Hallinan Pankaj Sharma this publication may be reproduced, ART DIRECTOR DTP MANAGER/CTS stored in or introduced into a retrieval Philip Ormerod Balwant Singh system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means (electronic, mechanical, ASSOCIATE DTP DESIGNERS PUBLISHING DIRECTOR Arvind Kumar, Rajesh Singh Adhikari photocopying, recording, or otherwise), Liz Wheeler without the prior written permission of DTP OPERATOR both the copyright owner and the PUBLISHING DIRECTOR Vishal Bhatia above publisher of this book. Jonathan Metcalf Published in Great Britain styling by by Dorling Kindersley Limited. ILLUSTRATIONS STUDIO8 DESIGN A catalog record for this book is James Graham available from the Library of Congress. PICTURE RESEARCH Myriam Megharbi ISBN:978-0-7566-8970-4 DK books are available at special discounts when purchased in bulk for Printed and bound in China PRODUCTION EDITOR sales promotions, premiums, by Leo Paper Products Ltd Tony Phipps fund-raising, or educational use.
    [Show full text]