Social Distance and Intergenerational Relations1

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Social Distance and Intergenerational Relations1 Questionnaires were administered to a sample of adults to assess the extent of social distance be- tween people of different ages. The findings suggest that the greater the age difference (younger or older) between people, the greater the social distance they feel. Moreover, people appear to feel more socially distant from the elderly, regardless of their own age. The source of estrangement seems to be the amount of contact with people of different ages: the less the contact, the greater the social distance. Social Distance and Intergenerational Relations1 I.JaneKidwell,MA,2 and Alan Booth. PhD2 The concept of social distance has been studied with his scale (1933). His well-known used extensively over the past 50 years to social distance measure was constructed us- study racial, religious, and ethnic intergroup ing the Thurstone technique. The scale con- sisted of the following items: relations. We believe the social distance con- Downloaded from cept can also add significantly to our under- standing of intergenerational relations. The (1) Would marry purpose of this study is to: (1) construct a (2) Would have as regular friends scale to assess social distance between age (3) Would work beside in an office gerontologist.oxfordjournals.org groups, similar to the one Bogardus used to (4) Would have several families in my neigh- study attitudes between racial and ethnic borhood (5) Would have merely as speaking acquaintances groups; (2) assess the extent of social distance (6) Would have live outside my neighborhood, and between different age groups, and (3) under- (7) Would have live outside my country. stand the role that age segregation has in ex- plaining social distance. The scale has been widely used to assess at New York University on September 30, 2011 social distance, and the studies have added Social Distance significantly to our understanding of inter- The concept of social distance was first de- group relations. While Bogardus' own scale is veloped in the mid-1920s. Although Emory S. seldom used today, scales building on his Bogardus, its first methodological developer, work have influenced research on status dif- is most commonly linked with the beginnings ferential (Westie & Howard, 1954), and the of work on social distance, Robert E. Park acceptance of immigrants into American (1924) actually preceded Bogardus with his society (Duncan & Lieberson, 1959), as well discussion of social distance: as research on grade and sex stratification in the high school community (Langworthy, The concept of "distance" as applied to human, 1959). One of the most frequent uses has as distinguished from spatial relations, has come been to study racial prejudice as Defleur and into use among sociologists, in an attempt to re- Westie (1959) and many others have done. duce to something like measurable terms the They have found that social distance is a pro- grades and degrees of understanding and intimacy which characterize personal and social relations ductive means for understanding intergroup generally. relations. Bogardus took Park's concept of social dis- Application of Social Distance to Age Groups tance and developed a scale for its assess- Studies suggest that it is reasonable to ment. Racial, religious, and occupational dis- assume that the relationship between age tance were the primary subjects Bogardus groups is characterized by social distance. In- dications of social distance revealed by prior research include: perceptual barriers to inter- 'We are indebted to Susan Welch and Nicholas Babchuk, Univ. of action between age groups; age categories Nebraska, who provided useful comments on an earlier version of this paper, which was presented at The Life Cycle: Aging session of the 1977 American relegated to the status of a minority group, Sociological Assn. meetings in Chicago. Reprint requests to A. Booth. 'Department of Sociology, Univ. of Nebraska-Lincoln, Lincoln, NE 68588. and extensive age group stereotyping. 412 The Gerontologist Barriers to Interaction Between Age Groups groups. The authors concluded that such per- Golde and Kogan (1959) administered to ceptual barriers will interfere with effective college undergraduates a sentence comple- inter-generational communication. tion questionnaire that was designed to assess attitudes toward old people. They found that these young people were hesitant about hav- Age Croups as Minorities ing interpersonal relationships with old peo- Friedenberg (1966) and Holt (1974) infer ple. that young people qualify for the status of minority; others say this is an accurate de- scription of old people. The latter is the impli- Little mention is made of reciprocity, sponta- cation of research by Kogan and Wallach neity, warmth, desire to initiate conversation, or (1961), where they found that unfavorable at- other qualities that comprise the essential ingre- titudes toward old people were associated dients of friendly contacts between peers. with negative dispositions toward ethnic minorities as well as a variety of physically The young subjects attributed polar oppo- disabled groups. site qualities such as serenity and irritability Kogan and Wallach claim their results offer to old people, indicating ambiguous feelings support to a minority group theory of aging. on the part of the former toward the latter. They found that their sample of older persons They also indicated that they felt old people evaluated "Negro" and "foreigner" more Downloaded from resented young people. positively than the college-aged sample.. Kogan and Shelton (1962) administered the Since members of racial and religious minori- same test, this time to a group of older re- ties have been shown to exhibit less prejudice spondents as well as the college under- against other minorities (Allport, 1954;Trian- gerontologist.oxfordjournals.org graduates. The greatest difference between dis & Triandis, 1960), Kogan and Wallach the young and old samples seems to occur for claim that it is reasonable to infer that old those item pairs where a personal relationship people also constitute a minority group be- with a specific other is implied (for example, cause old people share this sentiment. "When I am with an older person, I . .", Kogan and Shelton (1962) feel that "while "Most of the old people I have known . .", underlying sentiments may be less ex- at New York University on September 30, 2011 and "When an old person I do not know sits treme . the dynamics of beliefs about old down next to me on a train or bus, I . ."). people are sufficiently similar to the ethnic They conclude: prejudice case to warrant discussion in a minority group context." They suggest . the age differences observed in images and Drake's (1958) label of "quasi-minority" is beliefs regarding "old people" are often suggestive most appropriate. Barron's (1953) results sup- of ambivalence, conflict, and inaccurate percep- port a minority group theory of aging. Older tion between generations. Younger individuals, subjects seem to anticipate the feelings of the perceiving that older persons resent them, attempt dominant majority of younger subjects, at- to avoid interpersonal contact and partially justify tempting to cope with them and trying to such avoidance by suggesting that older individ- avoid rejection by emphasizing certain uals are really more interested in their families and characteristics and values while suppressing are preoccupied with death. The belief that "old others. people" are in need of assistance has a patronizing flavor in the present context, but such a belief may serve to assauge guilt feelings deriving from Stereotyping of Age Croups neglect of old people's emotional requirements. If a widespread stereotype is indicative of a minority status for a group, age groups again Knapp and Jackson (1963) compared the qualify. There is ample evidence that the responses to a semantic differential test given majority of people subscribe to stereotypes to a group of young respondents for an old about old people. Not only has it been that stimulus group ("old-attributed") and the re- lay people subscribe to stereotypes about the sponses given by an old group with them- old, but even professionals who work with the selves as the stimulus group ("old-direct"). aged score high on the stereotype scale (Arn- The old-attributed responses were more nega- hoff & Lorge, 1960; Tuckman & Lorge, 1956). tive than the old-direct, illustrating a lack of However, since there has been no complimen- understanding between the young and old tary research focus on stereotypes of other Vol.17, No. 5,1977 413 age groups, no valid assumptions can be other hand, we would expect individuals made as to whether or not stereotypes exist growing up in a home that included grand- for other age groups. parents, other adult relatives or boarders, or On the basis of past research it is very diffi- who have such persons in their adult home, to cult, if not impossible, to know exactly what be less constrained about developing inti- is the nature of the relationship between all mate interage relations and as a consequence age groups. As late as 1972, Ahammer and feel less age-related social distance. Simi- Baltes were able to claim themselves to be larly, people who are successful in develop- the first to systematically vary the age of both ing close ties with people in other age cate- the perceiving and the perceived groups in gories at work, in the neighborhood, or with their research. When doing this, they found relatives not in the home are less likely to de- misconceptions existed between all three age velop feelings of social distance with respect groups they tested —adolescents, middle- to individuals different from themselves in aged, and old people. Aaronson (1966) shows age. Thus, our second hypothesis is that in- three distinctly different stereotypes across dividuals who have more extensive histories the life-span. The first is childhood (ages 5- of close interage relations feel less social dis- 25), then adulthood (ages 26-54), and finally tance with people in age categories different senescence (55-85).
Recommended publications
  • Making Decisions from a Distance: the Impact of Technological
    Making Decisions From a Distance: The Impact of Technological Mediation on Riskiness and Dehumanization Min Kyung Lee1, Nathaniel Fruchter1, Laura Dabbish1,2 1Human-Computer Interaction Institute, 2Heinz College Carnegie Mellon University {mklee, fruchter, dabbish}@cmu.edu ABSTRACT Are people risk-seeking and more dehumanizing at a Telepresence means business people can make deals in distance? Are certain people more risk seeking and other countries, doctors can give remote medical advice, dehumanizing at a distance than others? and soldiers can rescue someone from thousands of miles away. When interaction is mediated, people are removed While telepresence technologies support sharing and from and lack context about the person they are making collaboration across geographical and temporal boundaries, decisions about. In this paper, we explore the impact of decision makers are now decontextualized from their technological mediation on risk and dehumanization in decision targets. Research in social psychology and decision-making. We conducted a laboratory experiment decision science suggests that this mediation and de- involving medical treatment decisions. The results suggest contextualization could create distance in how decision that technological mediation influences decision making, makers construe, feel toward, and engage with remote but its influence depends on an individual’s self-construal: people and events [45]. This perceived distance may be participants who saw themselves as defined through their exacerbated by intrinsic differences among cultural relationships (interdependent self-construal) recommended dimensions, such as styles of self-construal [10] as previous riskier and more painful treatments in video conferencing work has shown that culture changes the nature of mediated than when face-to-face.
    [Show full text]
  • SOCIAL DISTANCES of WHITES to RACIAL OR ETHNIC Minoritiesl Nina Michalikova Texas Woman
    SOCIAL DISTANCES OF WHITES TO RACIAL OR ETHNIC MINORITIESl Nina Michalikova Texas Woman 's University Philip Q. Yang Texas Woman 's University Social distance has been studied by researchers since Emory Bogar­ dus (1925) first developed the social distance scale almost nine decades ago. Social distance refers to "the grades and degrees of understanding and intimacy which characterize pre-social and social relations gener­ ally" (Park 1924). As social distance increases, people tend to distance themselves from members of another racial or ethnic group or exclude them from their lives (Yancey 2003). An examination of social distances between different racial or ethnic groups in the United States can help better understand racial and ethnic relations, conflict, cooperation, and alliance. Previous research on social distance almost exclusively focuses on the social distance between blacks and whites. Results are mixed. Often based on indirect measures of social distance, many studies suggest that the social distance of whites to African Americans has decreased over time, even though ethnic prejudice has not vanished (e.g., Firebaugh and Davis 1988; Schuman et al. 1997; Steeh and Schuman 1992). Today whites are more willing to live in the same neighborhood with African Americans, to accept interracial marriage, to report having a black person as a close friend, and to vote for a black president (Alba, Rumbaut, and Marotz 2005; Healey 2004; Jaynes and Williams 2000; Ladd 2002; Yan­ cey 1999). Findings from existing studies indicate improvements in school and residential integration of blacks. For example, the percentage of whites who object to sending their children to schools where the ma­ jority of students were black decreased from 67 percent in 1958 to 34 percent in 1990; the percentage of whites who would definitely move out if blacks moved into their neighborhood in great numbers also declined from 50 percent in 1958 to 18 percent in 1990 (Schuman et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Applying Construal Level Theory to Communication Strategies for Participatory Sustainable Development
    Applying Construal Level Theory to Communication Strategies for Participatory Sustainable Development D.H. Strongheart Florence Obison Fabio Bordoni School of Engineering Blekinge Institute of Technology Karlskrona, Sweden Thesis submitted for completion of Master of Strategic Leadership towards Sustainability, Blekinge Institute of Technology, Karlskrona, Sweden. Abstract: To the vast majority of people, the terms “sustainability” and “sustainable development” are unfamiliar, and, when they are recognized, there is still a great deal of interpretability as to their significance. Since no consensus exists regarding these terms, communication efforts to promote action and awareness among citizens must invariably “frame” the issue of sustainable development in one way or another. By and large, most communication strategies promote small private-sphere actions relevant to patterns of consumption. While these small actions are helpful, participatory, collective, public-sphere activism towards sustainability is much more potent and desirable. In attempting to engage this type of participatory action, communicators must understand the psychological barriers that are likely to confront their efforts. Communication professionals recognize that one such barrier, that of perceived, or, psychological distance, from issues of non-sustainability is especially pernicious. This paper attempts to apply Construal Level Theory (CLT), which provides “an account of how psychological distance influences individuals’ thoughts and behavior” (Trope et al. 2007) to the design of communication strategies for participatory sustainable development. After providing a thorough review of CLT, the authors examine the many ways that the theory can contribute to the design of communication strategies for participatory sustainable development. Keywords: Construal Level Theory, psychological distance, high construal mindset, framing, participatory sustainable development.
    [Show full text]
  • Guidance for Physical and Social Distancing
    Risk Communication and Community Engagement Guidance for Physical and Social Distancing Background Physical and social distancing measures are associated with limiting contact between people during disease outbreaks. These measures when applied are often enhanced by lockdowns or shutdowns as part of broader risk communication and community engagement strategies to halt the transmission of disease outbreaks. As the COVID-19 situation evolves, WHO has recommended the practice of physical distancing through various tools, in particular, the guidance on risk assessment for mass gatherings during COVID-19. Definitions Physical distancing refers to individual distancing and is defined as an act of keeping distance or space between one person and another especially if they are coughing, sneezing, or have a fever.1 Examples include the use of non-contact greetings, maintaining a given distance of at least one metre between individuals, staying at home, individual quarantine, etc.2 It is one of the most effective measures recommended to avoid being exposed to COVID-19 and slowing its spread locally, nationally and globally. It entails limiting close contact with others outside one’s household, indoors and in outdoor spaces. However, physical distancing may, in some settings like the African Region, have adverse effects and contribute directly and indirectly to COVID-19-related deaths. It is therefore important that while practising physical distancing, people should maintain and even increase social proximity through non-physical means, for example through social media platforms and communication technologies.2 Social distancing refers to community distancing and is a set of interventions or measures taken to prevent the spread of a contagious disease by maintaining a physical distance between people and reducing the number of times people come into close contact with one another.
    [Show full text]
  • Racial Distancing and Sensitivity to Stigmatization Among Future
    RACIAL DISTANCING AND SENSITIVITY TO STIGMATIZATION AMONG FUTURE BLACK PROFESSIONALS by CODDY L. CARTER UTZ MCKNIGHT, COMMITTEE CHAIR STEPHEN THOMA, COMMITTEE CO-CHAIR NIRMALA EREVELLES ADRIANE SHEFFIELD SARA TOMEK A DISSERTATION Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Department of Educational Studies in Psychology, Research Methodology, and Counseling in the Graduate School of The University of Alabama TUSCALOOSA, ALABAMA 2018 Copyright Coddy L. Carter 2018 ALL RIGHTS RESERVED ABSTRACT Professional occupations requiring higher education have long been paths to upward mobility for Black people in the United States. This mobility has historically been tied to both social and economic advancement. Whether advancement was subjective or objective, there was some form of distancing from the broader Black community. The three studies of the present dissertation used national and regional samples to test the problem of whether future Black professionals endorsed racial distancing behaviors. Results showed that racial distancing was composed of economic and social components. Moreover, high levels of Black social interactions and high ratings of emotional bonds to the Black community were negative determinants of the social distance defined as group distancing. High levels of emotional bonds alone were negative determinants of economic distancing. Characteristics of high racial distancing included discomfort in Black social spaces and a desire to turn one’s back on the Black community for advancement. Though racial distancing was present, approximately 73 percent of the national sample was low in economic and group distancing. In examining reasons for racial distancing, the regional sample results showed that a majority of respondents were highly sensitive to racial stigmatization whether or not they were from racially diverse communities or predominantly Black spaces.
    [Show full text]
  • Homophily and Social Distance in the General Social Survey
    A Longitudinal Analysis of Gendered Association Patterns: Homophily and Social Distance in the General Social Survey Matthew E. Brashears Department of Sociology, Cornell University, [email protected] Abstract How has the passage of time impacted the ego networks of males and females? I compare the homophily and social distances of males and females using the 1985 and 2004 GSS networks modules. The results indicate that change has been gradual and incremental rather than radical. In 2004 less social distance separates associates for women than for men, and males differentiate more among levels of education. The results suggest that macro-level structural changes have not been sufficient to produce similarly large changes in ego network composition. Keywords General social survey, discussion relations, gender, social distance, homophily, longitudinal, ego networks Page 1 of 26 Introduction How have changing structural conditions impacted the composition of male and female ego networks? Existing scholarship argues that our social networks emerge largely from the structural constraints, opportunities, and settings that we experience in the course of our lives (e.g., Blau 1977; Feld 1981; McPherson, Smith-Lovin and Cook 2001). As a result, those who have different structural opportunities and constraints (e.g., different educational or occupational trajectories) should have networks of different compositions. These effects should extend beyond the individual level, aggregating to encompass entire demographic categories and producing characteristic patterns of association. A variety of studies have confirmed this insight, finding, for example, both that male and female networks differ in composition (Ibarra 1992; Marsden 1988), and that some of this difference can be attributed to structural inequalities (Moore 1990).
    [Show full text]
  • Social, Psychological, and Demographic Characteristics of Dehumanization Toward Immigrants
    Social, psychological, and demographic characteristics of dehumanization toward immigrants David M. Markowitza and Paul Slovicb,c,1 aSchool of Journalism and Communication, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR 97403; bDecision Research, Eugene, OR 97401; and cDepartment of Psychology, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR 97403 Contributed by Paul Slovic, February 14, 2020 (sent for review December 18, 2019; reviewed by Douglas MacLean and Adam Waytz) This study extends the current body of work on dehumanization Theoretical Underpinnings of Dehumanization by evaluating the social, psychological, and demographic corre- Dehumanization is the perspective that certain people should be lates of blatant disregard for immigrants. Participants (n = 468) denied uniquely human rights and specific out-groups are were randomly assigned to read a scenario where 1) an immigrant therefore denied the privileges, activities, or agency that are or 2) an immigrant and their child were caught illegally crossing ordinarily ascribed to in-groups (8, 9, 15). Prior work (16) sug- the southern border of the United States, and then rated how long gests that dehumanization has a long history, and is often they should spend in jail if convicted. Participants reported that expressed tacitly (e.g., discounting the emotions of another they would sentence the immigrant to more jail time than the group) or explicitly through metaphors (e.g., calling immigrants immigrant and child. Those who sent immigrants to jail for more animals). Explicit dehumanization is most common; for example, time also viewed them as socially distant and less human, de- immigrants have been viewed as diseased organisms (e.g., para- scribed immigration in impersonal terms, and endorsed other so- sites, leeches), objects or materials, and invaders (8, 17, 18).
    [Show full text]
  • The Impact of Social Distance on Community in University Apartments Donald F
    Iowa State University Capstones, Theses and Retrospective Theses and Dissertations Dissertations 1987 The impact of social distance on community in university apartments Donald F. Whalen Iowa State University Follow this and additional works at: https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/rtd Part of the Higher Education and Teaching Commons, Social Psychology Commons, and the Social Psychology and Interaction Commons Recommended Citation Whalen, Donald F., "The impact of social distance on community in university apartments " (1987). Retrospective Theses and Dissertations. 9317. https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/rtd/9317 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Iowa State University Capstones, Theses and Dissertations at Iowa State University Digital Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Retrospective Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Iowa State University Digital Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. INFORMATION TO USERS The most advanced technology has been used to photo­ graph and reproduce this manuscript from the microfilm master. UMI films the original text directly from the copy submitted. Thus, some dissertation copies are in typewriter face, while others may be from a computer printer. In the unlikely event that the author did not send UMI a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyrighted material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are re­ produced by sectioning the original, beginning at the upper left-hand comer and continuing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. Each oversize page is available as one exposure on a standard 35 mm slide or as a 17" x 23" black and white photographic print for an additional charge.
    [Show full text]
  • Maintaining Social Distancing During the COVID-19 Outbreak
    social sciences $€ £ ¥ Brief Report Maintaining Social Distancing during the COVID-19 Outbreak Krzysztof Goniewicz 1,* and Amir Khorram-Manesh 2,3 1 Department of Aviation Security, Military University of Aviation, 08-521 D˛eblin,Poland 2 Department of Surgery, Institute of Clinical Sciences, Sahlgrenska Academy, Gothenburg University, 413 45 Gothenburg, Sweden; [email protected] 3 Department of Development and Research, Armed Forces Center for Defense Medicine, 426 76 Gothenburg, Västra Frölunda, Sweden * Correspondence: [email protected] Abstract: The analyses of the European Centre for Disease Prevention and Control and an increasing amount of scientific research show that the spread of SARS-CoV-2 may be limited by maintain- ing social distancing, appropriate hand hygiene, and following the basic principles of prophylaxis. While simulating models applied to social distancing have all been tested and evaluated, the imple- mentation of its guidelines in different traditions and cultures has not been discussed sufficiently. Consequently, applying social distancing guidelines alone may not be enough to contain the spread of the coronavirus. This brief report aims to clarify the role of cultural and behavioral differences in the diverse outcomes of COVID-19 management. Keywords: social distancing; COVID-19; pandemic 1. Introduction The current scientific state of knowledge regarding Coronavirus Disease 2019 (COVID- 19) indicates that the death rate for COVID-19 is several times higher than that of seasonal Citation: Goniewicz, Krzysztof, and flu, the infection can lead to long-term illnesses in all age categories, including healthy Amir Khorram-Manesh. 2021. young people, and the passage of SARS-CoV-2 infection does not protect against reinfection Maintaining Social Distancing during (Petersen et al.
    [Show full text]
  • SO 001 787 Social Distance and Race Attitudes: Astudy of the White Population of Princeton, Maine and the Indian Population of I
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 054 994 SO 001 787 AUTHOR Little, Craig B. TITLE Social Distance and Race Attitudes: AStudy of the White Population of Princeton, Maineand the Indian Population of Indian Township, Maine. INSTITUTION New Hampshire Univ., Durham. PUB DATE Feb 70 NOTE 183p. EDRS PRICE MF-$0.65 HC-$6.58 *American Indians; Behavioral ScienceResearch; DESCRIPTORS . *Caucasians; Conceptual Schemes; Cultural Factors; Intergroup Relations; Masters Theses;*Race Relations; *Racial Attitudes; SocialAttitudes; Social Isolation; *Social Relations;Socioeconomic Influences IDENTIFIERS Maine, French Canadians; Parks (Robert E.) ABSTRACT The essentially descriptive study ofthese two racially and culturally distinctpopulations started with Robert E. Park's conception of social distance asthe degree of understanding and intimacy which characterizespersonal and social relations. The social distance phenomenon was brokendown into three levels of analysis: perceived socio-economic status,attitudes of perference for association, and :frequency and intimacyof interaction between members of groups. Based on historicalbackground and contemporary intergroup relations theory, six hypotheses werepresented to be tested. Two probability samples oftwenty-five respondents each were selected. Similar interviews employing bothattitude measurement techniques based on the conceptualizationof social distance and questions requiring more qualitative responses wereadministered. Participant observations were also compiled.Although statistical associations were somewhat low,
    [Show full text]
  • How Does Social Distance Influence Behavior at the Individual Level and the Group Level? a Story Through Experimental Economics
    HOW DOES SOCIAL DISTANCE INFLUENCE BEHAVIOR AT THE INDIVIDUAL LEVEL AND THE GROUP LEVEL? A STORY THROUGH EXPERIMENTAL ECONOMICS Von der Mercator School of Management, Fakultät für Betriebswirtschaftslehre, der Universität Duisburg-Essen zur Erlangung des akademischen Grades eines Doktors der Wirtschaftswissenschaft (Dr. rer. oec.) genehmigte Dissertation von Dung V. Vu aus Hanoi, Vietnam 1 Referent: Prof. Dr. Markus Taube Korreferentin: Prof. Dr. Jeannette Brosig-Koch Tag der mündlichen Prüfung: 13.07.2016 Acknowledgement : A special thank you to the Deutsche Forchungsgemeinschaft (DFG) who funded my project, Professor Markus Taube for his academic support, Professor Brosig-Koch, Professor Dr. Heike Henning-Schmidt, Dr. Gari Walkowitz, Professor Xianghong Wang my colleagues Martin Heinberg, Kai Duttle, Chih-Chieh Wang, Taske Ueda, Huixia Wang, Xiu Chen who gave me comments, suggestions and logistics for the experiment, William Knapp for his valuable online lectures, Ian Coppers, Robert Reed and Joanna Rance for proofreading. 2 Contents: Preface..................................................................................................................................7 Chapter 1: Introduction ........................................................................................................9 Chapter 2: How does social distance influence public good behavior at the individual level? ...........................................................................................................................................20 2.1
    [Show full text]
  • Dehumanization and the Metaphysics of Genocide
    DEHUMANIZATION AND THE METAPHYSICS OF GENOCIDE: A NEW THEORY FOR GENOCIDE PREVENTION by LAUREN EICHLER A DISSERTATION Presented to the Department of Philosophy and the Graduate School of the University of Oregon in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy June 2019 DISSERTATION APPROVAL PAGE Student: Lauren Eichler Title: Dehumanization and the Metaphysics of Genocide: A New Theory for Genocide Prevention This dissertation has been accepted and approved in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Doctor of Philosophy degree in the Philosophy Department by: Scott Pratt Chairperson Scott Pratt Advisor Erin McKenna Core Member Alejandro Vallega Core Member Michelle Jacob Institutional Representative and Janet Woodruff-Borden Vice Provost and Dean of the Graduate School Original approval signatures are on file with the University of Oregon Graduate School. Degree awarded June 2019 ii © 2019 Lauren Eichler iii DISSERTATION ABSTRACT Lauren Eichler Doctor of Philosophy Department of Philosophy June 2019 Title: Dehumanization and the Metaphysics of Genocide: A New Theory for Genocide Prevention I argue that dehumanization is a necessary condition of modern genocide, and that preventing dehumanization should be part of efforts to prevent genocide. Unlike other scholarship that addresses this issue, I hold that attending to the moral status and role of nonhuman animals in the process of dehumanization is integral to this effort. Throughout the history of Western philosophy, nonhuman animals have been used to define the human and, in dehumanization, provide the excuse for one group of humans to do violence to another. The absence of a concern for nonhuman animals from both dehumanization and genocide literature generally speaking needs to be rectified if new solutions for these problems are to be developed.
    [Show full text]