Pathological Disgust: in the Thoughts, Not the Eye, of the Beholder

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Pathological Disgust: in the Thoughts, Not the Eye, of the Beholder Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, December 2006; 19(4): 335Á351 Pathological disgust: In the thoughts, not the eye, of the beholder BETHANY A. TEACHMAN University of Virginia Abstract There is now mounting evidence that disgust is critically involved in anxiety disorders. Despite the emphasis on irrational fear in understanding these disorders, there has been little investigation into the cognitive component of disgust. In this theoretical paper, a process to distinguish normal versus pathological disgust responding is proposed and its clinical implications considered. Similar to intrusive thoughts in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), it is the interpretation of a disgust reaction as personally meaningful or threatening that will lead to pathological disgust. Building upon the belief domains thought to contribute to OCD (Obsessive Compulsive Cognition Working Group [OCCWG], 1997) and the laws of sympathetic magic in normal disgust (Rozin & Nemeroff, 1990), it is suggested that beliefs about one’s ability to cope with being contaminated, dirty, or disgusted may help predict avoidance and pathological disgust. Keywords: Anxiety, appraisals, disgust, interpretations, obsessive-compulsive disorder Disgust has long been recognized as a basic emotion. Darwin (1965) defined disgust as: ‘‘something revolting, primarily in relation to the sense of taste, as actually perceived or vividly imagined; and secondarily to anything that causes a similar feeling, through the sense of smell, touch, and even eyesight’’ (p. 253). While there is certainly variability in people’s general disgust sensitivity levels (Haidt, McCauley, & Rozin, 1994), the emotion has a fairly recognizable set of elicitors within a given culture. For instance, the popular Disgust Scale evaluates seven common disgust-eliciting domains (food, animals, body products, sex, body envelope violations, death, and hygiene), and the authors of the scale have more recently recognized the domains of moral and interpersonal disgust (Haidt, Rozin, McCauley, & Imada, 1997). A number of theories have been offered to account for this varied group of elicitors (e.g., Angyal, 1941; Rozin, Haidt, & McCauley, 1993; Tomkins, 1963), but there are fewer coherent theories to explain why one person becomes significantly more disgusted than another. In particular, it is unclear why disgust responding becomes so extreme for some that it might be characterized as pathological. There is now mounting evidence for a role of disgust in anxiety disorders, including phobias of small animals like spiders and snakes (e.g., Merckelbach, de Jong, Arntz, & Schouten, 1993), bloodÁinjury phobias (e.g., Tolin, Lohr, Sawchuk, & Lee, 1997), and perhaps even obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD; e.g., Sprengelmeyer et al., 1997). Correspondence: Bethany A. Teachman, Department of Psychology, University of Virginia, P.O. Box 400400, Charlottesville, VA 22904-4400, USA. E-mail: [email protected] ISSN 1061-5806 print/ISSN 1477-2205 online # 2006 Taylor & Francis DOI: 10.1080/10615800601055923 336 B. A. Teachman Notwithstanding, there continues to be debate about the specific nature of the relationship between disgust and fear when it becomes disordered. For example, some researchers have found significantly greater disgust sensitivity among individuals with particularly high fears (e.g., Klieger & Siejak, 1997), while others have not (e.g., Thorpe and Salkovskis, 1998). How can we make sense of these incongruent findings, and under what circumstances will disgust contribute to psychopathology? Pathological Disgust To date, there is no agreed upon definition of pathological disgust, nor agreement on whether disgust will contribute to psychopathology. Defining pathological fear has rested on assumptions that excessive fear and avoidance in situations where significant harm is unlikely is irrational and disordered. It is not clear that the same criteria can be used to define pathological disgust (Woody & Teachman, 2000). As discussed below, there are numerous instances where healthy populations experience disgust and threat of contam- ination despite no objective danger (Rozin & Nemeroff, 1990). This suggests that simply defining a response as irrational will be insufficient to identify pathological disgust reactions. Although the field is in its infancy in terms of empirically demonstrating the existence of pathological disgust, a preliminary set of principles are suggested to guide future investigations. The following criteria are expected to be part of most pathological disgust reactions (though it is not yet clear which of these criteria will constitute a necessary or sufficient criterion): 1. Evaluation of the threat posed by the disgust elicitor or response should be excessive relative to others’’ evaluations (and not simply part of an accepted sub-cultural response). This can refer to exaggerated perceptions of the likelihood of contamination and subsequent harm, or beliefs about the consequences of emotional dysregulation (i.e., that becoming disgusted is harmful or dangerous). 2. The disgust response should cause clinically significant distress or impairment, most typically through excessive avoidance behaviors. 3. It is unlikely that simple factual information about the low probability of harm resulting from threat of contamination (physical, psychological, or moral) will be sufficient to reduce emotional distress or avoidance. Thus, informing a disgusted person that touching a snake’s skin will not leave a harmful residue or pose any danger is not likely to reduce distress or avoidance, suggesting a rigidly held belief despite objective evidence to the contrary. This paper focuses on how interpretations following normal disgust reactions will contribute to this disordered emotional response system. The range of interpretations discussed below are hypothesized to be present on occasion in both normal and disordered populations, but they will occur with greater frequency, intensity, and be held more rigidly when disgust becomes pathological. Role of Cognition in Disgust The subjective feeling, behavioral, and physiological components of disgust have been examined, but there has been little investigation into the cognitive component of disgust (though see intriguing investigations of information processing biases in disgust, such as Charash & McKay, 2002; Sawchuk, Meunier, Lohr, & Westendorf, 2002). This is Pathological disgust thoughts 337 surprising given that disgust is often described as a complex emotion because of its ideational component, whereby disgusted individuals experience revulsion in thinking about the origins of a substance. In addition, the belief that understanding of germ transmission may be required for a full experience of disgust has been offered as one explanation for the limited disgust reactions typically observed in children (see Fallon, Rozin, & Pliner, 1984; Rozin & Fallon, 1987). The suggestion is that children do not yet have the cognitive capacity to become fully disgusted because they lack the ideational piece. Thus, while cognitive processing has been considered from a developmental perspective in disgust, and unusual beliefs about germ transmission have been noted among normal, healthy samples (e.g., Nemeroff & Rozin, 1994; Rozin, Markwith, & Nemeroff, 1992), there has been little research to date on the content of disgust cognitions. Perhaps we can distinguish between normal and disordered disgust from the standpoint of its associated cognitions. In earlier work, we suggested that appraisals of threat of contamination are often common to disgust and fear (Woody & Teachman, 2000), and this link has also been suggested in the empirical literature (e.g., Davey, 1993). This common threat appraisal likely has adaptive value in protecting the individual from touching or ingesting a potentially noxious substance. Given its adaptive value and its high prevalence among healthy samples, appraisals of contamination threat may not be sufficient to explain why disgust becomes pathological for some but not others. Because disgust is a basic emotion with a common set of elicitors within a given culture, it seems likely that most individuals, regardless of whether they are vulnerable to emotional dysregulation, will experience thoughts relevant to disgust and contamination when they confront a relevant elicitor. Accordingly, it may be necessary to look beyond the common content of disgust thoughts or initial appraisals, and look instead to the secondary interpretations that follow disgust reactions. Secondary interpretations are used here to refer to the metacognitive process of reflecting on the implications of an initial appraisal of a situation (e.g., if my primary appraisal is true, what are the repercussions?). In Salkovskis’ (1996) discussion of appraisals in OCD, primary appraisals reflect beliefs about the subjective likelihood and consequences of a perceived danger, and the secondary appraisals reflect one’s perceived ability to cope with that danger. Thus, the primary appraisal is based on the initial reaction to the stimulus, and the secondary appraisal is based on the significance of that reaction to the individual. Applying this model to understand pathological disgust, it is hypothesized that primary appraisals of disgust will likely reflect beliefs about the contaminating properties of a disgusting object (e.g., This is going to get all over me) or likelihood of a disgust reaction (e.g., I think I might vomit), and the secondary interpretations will center on beliefs about
Recommended publications
  • How Fun Are Your Meetings? Investigating the Relationship Between Humor Patterns in Team Interactions and Team Performance
    University of Nebraska at Omaha DigitalCommons@UNO Psychology Faculty Publications Department of Psychology 11-2014 How fun are your meetings? Investigating the relationship between humor patterns in team interactions and team performance Nale Lehmann-Willenbrock Vrije University Amsterdam Joseph A. Allen University of Nebraska at Omaha, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unomaha.edu/psychfacpub Part of the Industrial and Organizational Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Lehmann-Willenbrock, Nale and Allen, Joseph A., "How fun are your meetings? Investigating the relationship between humor patterns in team interactions and team performance" (2014). Psychology Faculty Publications. 118. https://digitalcommons.unomaha.edu/psychfacpub/118 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Department of Psychology at DigitalCommons@UNO. It has been accepted for inclusion in Psychology Faculty Publications by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@UNO. For more information, please contact [email protected]. In press at Journal of Applied Psychology How fun are your meetings? Investigating the relationship between humor patterns in team interactions and team performance Nale Lehmann-Willenbrock1 & Joseph A. Allen2 1 VU University Amsterdam 2 University of Nebraska at Omaha Acknowledgements The initial data collection for this study was partially supported by a grant from the German Research Foundation, which is gratefully acknowledged. We appreciate the support by Simone Kauffeld and the helpful feedback by Steve Kozlowski. Abstract Research on humor in organizations has rarely considered the social context in which humor occurs. One such social setting that most of us experience on a daily basis concerns the team context. Building on recent theorizing about the humor—performance association in teams, this study seeks to increase our understanding of the function and effects of humor in team interaction settings.
    [Show full text]
  • Spring-And-Summer-Fun-Pack.Pdf
    Spring & Summer FUN PACK BUILD OUR KIDS' SUCCESS Find many activities for kids in Kindergarten through Grade 9 to get moving and stay busy during the warmer months. Spring & Summer FUN PACK WHO IS THIS BOOKLET FOR? 1EVERYONE – kids, parents, camps, childcare providers, and anyone that is involved with kids this summer. BOKS has compiled a Spring & Summer Fun Pack that is meant to engage kids and allow them to “Create Their Own Adventure of Fun” for the warmer weather months. This package is full of easy to follow activities for kids to do independently, as a family, or for camp counselors/childcare providers to engage kids on a daily basis. We have included a selection of: BOKS Bursts (5–10 minute activity breaks) BOKS lesson plans - 30 minutes of fun interactive lessons including warm ups, skill work, games and nutrition bits with video links Crafts Games Recipes HOW DOES THIS WORK? Choose two or three activities daily from the selection outlined on page 4: 1. Get physically active with Bursts and/or BOKS fitness classes. 2. Be creative with cooking and crafts. 3. Have fun outdoors (or indoors), try our games! How do your kids benefit? • Give kids time to play and have fun. • Get kids moving toward their 60 minutes of recommended daily activity. • Build strong bones and muscles with simple fitness skills. • Reduce symptoms of anxiety. • Encourage a love of physical activity through engaging games. • We encourage your kids to have fun creating their own BOKS adventure. WHO WE ARE… BOKS (Build Our Kids' Success) is a FREE physical activity program designed to get kids active and establish a lifelong commitment to health and fitness.
    [Show full text]
  • Why Feelings Stray: Sources of Affective Misforecasting in Consumer Behavior Vanessa M
    Why Feelings Stray: Sources of Affective Misforecasting in Consumer Behavior Vanessa M. Patrick, University of Georgia Deborah J. MacInnis, University of Southern California ABSTRACT drivers of AMF has considerable import for consumer behavior, Affective misforecasting (AMF) is defined as the gap between particularly in the area of consumer satisfaction, brand loyalty and predicted and experienced affect. Based on prior research that positive word-of-mouth. examines AMF, the current study uses qualitative and quantitative Figure 1 depicts the process by which affective misforecasting data to examine the sources of AMF (i.e., why it occurs) in the occurs (for greater detail see MacInnis, Patrick and Park 2005). As consumption domain. The authors find evidence supporting some Figure 1 suggests, affective forecasts are based on a representation sources of AMF identified in the psychology literature, develop a of a future event and an assessment of the possible affective fuller understanding of others, and, find evidence for novel sources reactions to this event. AMF occurs when experienced affect of AMF not previously explored. Importantly, they find consider- deviates from the forecasted affect on one or more of the following able differences in the sources of AMF depending on whether dimensions: valence, intensity and duration. feelings are worse than or better than forecast. Since forecasts can be made regarding the valence of the feelings, the specific emotions expected to be experienced, the INTRODUCTION intensity of feelings or the duration of a projected affective re- Before purchase: “I can’t wait to use this all the time, it is sponse, consequently affective misforecasting can occur along any going to be so much fun, I’m going to go out with my buddies of these dimensions.
    [Show full text]
  • Classification of Human Emotions from Electroencephalogram (EEG) Signal Using Deep Neural Network
    (IJACSA) International Journal of Advanced Computer Science and Applications, Vol. 8, No. 9, 2017 Classification of Human Emotions from Electroencephalogram (EEG) Signal using Deep Neural Network Abeer Al-Nafjan Areej Al-Wabil College of Computer and Information Sciences Center for Complex Engineering Systems Imam Muhammad bin Saud University King Abdulaziz City for Science and Technology Riyadh, Saudi Arabia Riyadh, Saudi Arabia Manar Hosny Yousef Al-Ohali College of Computer and Information Sciences College of Computer and Information Sciences King Saud University King Saud University Riyadh, Saudi Arabia Riyadh, Saudi Arabia Abstract—Estimation of human emotions from [1]. Recognizing a user‘s affective state can be used to Electroencephalogram (EEG) signals plays a vital role in optimize training and enhancement of the BCI operations [2]. developing robust Brain-Computer Interface (BCI) systems. In our research, we used Deep Neural Network (DNN) to address EEG is often used in BCI research experimentation because EEG-based emotion recognition. This was motivated by the the process is non-invasive to the research subject and minimal recent advances in accuracy and efficiency from applying deep risk is involved. The devices‘ usability, reliability, cost- learning techniques in pattern recognition and classification effectiveness, and the relative convenience of conducting applications. We adapted DNN to identify human emotions of a studies and recruiting participants due to their portability have given EEG signal (DEAP dataset) from power spectral density been cited as factors influencing the increased adoption of this (PSD) and frontal asymmetry features. The proposed approach is method in applied research contexts [3], [4]. These advantages compared to state-of-the-art emotion detection systems on the are often accompanied by challenges such as low spatial same dataset.
    [Show full text]
  • What We Mean When We Talk About Suffering—And Why Eric Cassell Should Not Have the Last Word
    What We Mean When We Talk About Suffering—and Why Eric Cassell Should Not Have the Last Word Tyler Tate, Robert Pearlman Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, Volume 62, Number 1, Winter 2019, pp. 95-110 (Article) Published by Johns Hopkins University Press For additional information about this article https://muse.jhu.edu/article/722412 Access provided at 26 Apr 2019 00:52 GMT from University of Washington @ Seattle What We Mean When We Talk About Suffering—and Why Eric Cassell Should Not Have the Last Word Tyler Tate* and Robert Pearlman† ABSTRACT This paper analyzes the phenomenon of suffering and its relation- ship to medical practice by focusing on the paradigmatic work of Eric Cassell. First, it explains Cassell’s influential model of suffering. Second, it surveys various critiques of Cassell. Next it outlines the authors’ concerns with Cassell’s model: it is aggressive, obscure, and fails to capture important features of the suffering experience. Finally, the authors propose a conceptual framework to help clarify the distinctive nature of sub- jective patient suffering. This framework contains two necessary conditions: (1) a loss of a person’s sense of self, and (2) a negative affective experience. The authors suggest how this framework can be used in the medical encounter to promote clinician-patient communication and the relief of suffering. *Center for Ethics in Health Care and School of Medicine, Oregon Health and Science University, Portland. †National Center for Ethics in Health Care, Washington, DC, and School of Medicine, University of Washington, Seattle. Correspondence: Tyler Tate, Oregon Health and Science University, School of Medicine, Depart- ment of Pediatrics, 3181 SW Sam Jackson Park Road, Portland, OR 97239-3098.
    [Show full text]
  • DISGUST: Features and SAWCHUK and Clinical Implications
    Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, Vol. 24, No. 7, 2005, pp. 932-962 OLATUNJIDISGUST: Features AND SAWCHUK and Clinical Implications DISGUST: CHARACTERISTIC FEATURES, SOCIAL MANIFESTATIONS, AND CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS BUNMI O. OLATUNJI University of Massachusetts CRAIG N. SAWCHUK University of Washington School of Medicine Emotions have been a long–standing cornerstone of research in social and clinical psychology. Although the systematic examination of emotional processes has yielded a rather comprehensive theoretical and scientific literature, dramatically less empirical attention has been devoted to disgust. In the present article, the na- ture, experience, and other associated features of disgust are outlined. We also re- view the domains of disgust and highlight how these domains have expanded over time. The function of disgust in various social constructions, such as cigarette smoking, vegetarianism, and homophobia, is highlighted. Disgust is also becoming increasingly recognized as an influential emotion in the onset, maintenance, and treatment of various phobic states, Obsessive–Compulsive Disorder, and eating disorders. In comparison to the other emotions, disgust offers great promise for fu- ture social and clinical research efforts, and prospective studies designed to improve our understanding of disgust are outlined. The nature, structure, and function of emotions have a rich tradition in the social and clinical psychology literature (Cacioppo & Gardner, 1999). Although emotion theorists have contested over the number of discrete emotional states and their operational definitions (Plutchik, 2001), most agree that emotions are highly influential in organizing thought processes and behavioral tendencies (Izard, 1993; John- Preparation of this manuscript was supported in part by NIMH NRSA grant 1F31MH067519–1A1 awarded to Bunmi O.
    [Show full text]
  • About Emotions There Are 8 Primary Emotions. You Are Born with These
    About Emotions There are 8 primary emotions. You are born with these emotions wired into your brain. That wiring causes your body to react in certain ways and for you to have certain urges when the emotion arises. Here is a list of primary emotions: Eight Primary Emotions Anger: fury, outrage, wrath, irritability, hostility, resentment and violence. Sadness: grief, sorrow, gloom, melancholy, despair, loneliness, and depression. Fear: anxiety, apprehension, nervousness, dread, fright, and panic. Joy: enjoyment, happiness, relief, bliss, delight, pride, thrill, and ecstasy. Interest: acceptance, friendliness, trust, kindness, affection, love, and devotion. Surprise: shock, astonishment, amazement, astound, and wonder. Disgust: contempt, disdain, scorn, aversion, distaste, and revulsion. Shame: guilt, embarrassment, chagrin, remorse, regret, and contrition. All other emotions are made up by combining these basic 8 emotions. Sometimes we have secondary emotions, an emotional reaction to an emotion. We learn these. Some examples of these are: o Feeling shame when you get angry. o Feeling angry when you have a shame response (e.g., hurt feelings). o Feeling fear when you get angry (maybe you’ve been punished for anger). There are many more. These are NOT wired into our bodies and brains, but are learned from our families, our culture, and others. When you have a secondary emotion, the key is to figure out what the primary emotion, the feeling at the root of your reaction is, so that you can take an action that is most helpful. .
    [Show full text]
  • Mathematics K Through 6
    Building fun and creativity into standards-based learning Mathematics K through 6 Ron De Long, M.Ed. Janet B. McCracken, M.Ed. Elizabeth Willett, M.Ed. © 2007 Crayola, LLC Easton, PA 18044-0431 Acknowledgements Table of Contents This guide and the entire Crayola® Dream-Makers® series would not be possible without the expertise and tireless efforts Crayola Dream-Makers: Catalyst for Creativity! ....... 4 of Ron De Long, Jan McCracken, and Elizabeth Willett. Your passion for children, the arts, and creativity are inspiring. Thank you. Special thanks also to Alison Panik for her content-area expertise, writing, research, and curriculum develop- Lessons ment of this guide. Garden of Colorful Counting ....................................... 6 Set representation Crayola also gratefully acknowledges the teachers and students who tested the lessons in this guide: In the Face of Symmetry .............................................. 10 Analysis of symmetry Barbi Bailey-Smith, Little River Elementary School, Durham, NC Gee’s-o-metric Wisdom ................................................ 14 Geometric modeling Rob Bartoch, Sandy Plains Elementary School, Baltimore, MD Patterns of Love Beads ................................................. 18 Algebraic patterns Susan Bivona, Mount Prospect Elementary School, Basking Ridge, NJ A Bountiful Table—Fair-Share Fractions ...................... 22 Fractions Jennifer Braun, Oak Street Elementary School, Basking Ridge, NJ Barbara Calvo, Ocean Township Elementary School, Oakhurst, NJ Whimsical Charting and
    [Show full text]
  • Emotion Classification Using Physiological Signals
    Emotion Classification Using Physiological Signals Byoung-Jun Park1, Eun-Hye Jang1, Myoung Ae Chung1, Sang-Hyeob Kim1*, Jin Hun Sohn2* 1IT Convergence Technology Research Laboratory, Electronics and Telecommunications Research Institute, Daejeon, 305-700 2Department of Psychology/Brain Research Institute, Chungnam National University, Daejeon, 305-765 ABSTRACT Objective: The aim of this study is to discriminate negative emotions, such as sadness, fear, surprise, and stress using physiological signals. Background: Recently, the main topic of emotion classification research is to recognize human’s feeling or emotion using various physiological signals. It is one of the core processes to implement emotional intelligence in human computer interaction (HCI) research. Method: Electrodermal activity (EDA), electrocardiogram (ECG), skin temperature (SKT), and photoplethysmography (PPG) are recorded and analyzed as physiological signals. And emotional stimuli are audio-visual film clips which have examined for their appropriateness and effectiveness through preliminary experiment. For classification of negative emotions, five machine learning algorithms, i.e., LDF, CART, SOM, and Naïve Bayes are used. Results: Result of emotion classification shows that an accuracy of emotion classification by CART (84.0%) was the highest and by LDA (50.7%) was the lowest. SOM showed emotion classification accuracy of 51.2% and Naïve Bayes was 76.2%. Conclusion: We could identify that CART was the optimal emotion classification algorithm for classifying 4 negative emotions (sadness, fear, surprise, and stress). Application: This result can be helpful to provide the basis for the emotion recognition technique in HCI. Keywords: Emotion classification, Negative emotion, Machine learning algorithm, Physiological signal 1. Introduction Nasoz, Alvarez, Lisetti, and Finkelstein, 2003).
    [Show full text]
  • The Distancing-Embracing Model of the Enjoyment of Negative Emotions in Art Reception
    BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2017), Page 1 of 63 doi:10.1017/S0140525X17000309, e347 The Distancing-Embracing model of the enjoyment of negative emotions in art reception Winfried Menninghaus1 Department of Language and Literature, Max Planck Institute for Empirical Aesthetics, 60322 Frankfurt am Main, Germany [email protected] Valentin Wagner Department of Language and Literature, Max Planck Institute for Empirical Aesthetics, 60322 Frankfurt am Main, Germany [email protected] Julian Hanich Department of Arts, Culture and Media, University of Groningen, 9700 AB Groningen, The Netherlands [email protected] Eugen Wassiliwizky Department of Language and Literature, Max Planck Institute for Empirical Aesthetics, 60322 Frankfurt am Main, Germany [email protected] Thomas Jacobsen Experimental Psychology Unit, Helmut Schmidt University/University of the Federal Armed Forces Hamburg, 22043 Hamburg, Germany [email protected] Stefan Koelsch University of Bergen, 5020 Bergen, Norway [email protected] Abstract: Why are negative emotions so central in art reception far beyond tragedy? Revisiting classical aesthetics in the light of recent psychological research, we present a novel model to explain this much discussed (apparent) paradox. We argue that negative emotions are an important resource for the arts in general, rather than a special license for exceptional art forms only. The underlying rationale is that negative emotions have been shown to be particularly powerful in securing attention, intense emotional involvement, and high memorability, and hence is precisely what artworks strive for. Two groups of processing mechanisms are identified that conjointly adopt the particular powers of negative emotions for art’s purposes.
    [Show full text]
  • The Gay Science (`La Gaya Scienza')
    Cambridge University Press 0521631599 - The Gay Science: With a Prelude in German Rhymes and an Appendix of Songs Friedrich Nietzsche Excerpt More information The Gay Science R`La gaya scienza') This house is my own and here I dwell, I've never aped nothing from no one and ± laugh at each master, mark me well, who at himself has not poked fun. Over my front door. The title is a translation into German Rin our edition, into English) of the ProvencËal subtitle. Gaya scienza R`joyful, cheerful, or gay science') was a term used by the troubadours in the twelfth to fourteenth centuries to refer to the art of poetry. In Ecce Homo Nietzsche writes that he has used the term gaya scienza here to designate the speci®c unity of `singer, knight, and free spirit' which was characteristic of early ProvencËal culture. © Cambridge University Press www.cambridge.org Cambridge University Press 0521631599 - The Gay Science: With a Prelude in German Rhymes and an Appendix of Songs Friedrich Nietzsche Excerpt More information Preface to the second edition 1 This book might need more than one preface; and in the end there would still be room for doubting whether someone who has not experienced something similar could, by means of prefaces, be brought closer to the experiences of this book. It seems to be written in the language of the wind that brings a thaw: it contains high spirits, unrest, contradiction, and April weather, so that one is constantly reminded of winter's nearness as well as of the triumph over winter that is coming, must come, perhaps has already come.
    [Show full text]
  • OWNING YOUR FEELINGS Tips for Success
    OWNING YOUR FEELINGS It can be easy to get caught up in your emotions as you’re feeling them. Most people don’t think about what emotions they are dealing with, but taking the time to really identify what you’re feeling can help you to better cope with challenging situations. The English language has over 3,000 words for Tips for success emotions. Allow yourself to feel. Sometimes there are societal pressures that encourage people to shut down their emotions, often expressed through People who are good at statements like, “Big girls don’t cry,” or “Man up.” These outdated ideas are being specific about harmful, not helpful. Everyone has emotionsthey are part of the human identifying and labeling experienceand you have every right to feel them, regardless of gender, their emotions are less sexual orientation, ethnicity, socio-economic status, race, political likely to binge drink, be affiliation or religion. physically aggressive, or selfinjure when Don’t ignore how you’re feeling. Most of us have heard the term “bottling up distressed. your feelings” before. When we try to push feelings aside without addressing them, they build strength and make us more likely to “explode” at some point in When schoolaged kids are the future. It may not always be appropriate to process your emotions at the taught about emotions for very moment you are feeling them, but try to do so as soon as you can. 20-30 minutes per week their social behavior and Talk it out. Find someone you trust that you can talk to about how you’re school performance feeling.
    [Show full text]